Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 31 July 2018
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00157

Early Life Stress in Rodents: Animal Models of Illness or Resilience?

Sahana Murthy Elizabeth Gould*
  • Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, United States

Early life adversity is a known risk factor for mood and anxiety disorders in adult humans (Heim et al., 2010; Huh et al., 2014; Rehan et al., 2017). Given the prevalence of both maltreatment in childhood and mental illness in adulthood, understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of this connection is important as it may suggest targets for new therapeutic interventions. Ethical constraints on conducting studies with humans have highlighted the need for reliable and robust animal models that researchers can utilize to identify relevant neurobiological processes (Guzman et al., 2016). Since the work of Harlow and colleagues beginning in the 1940s, which involved raising infant macaques with cloth and wire mothers (reviewed in van der Horst and van der Veer, 2008), researchers have sought to develop useful animal models of early life adversity. These and other more recent studies have shown obvious behavioral abnormalities in monkeys subjected to early life stress (ELS) (Schino et al., 2001; Corcoran et al., 2012; Howell et al., 2014). Despite the relevance of these models to humans, nonhuman primates have practical and ethical limitations that are obstacles for their use in high-throughput studies. By contrast, animal models of early life stress in rodents, which were first used in the laboratory more than 50 years ago (Levine, 1957), have gained in usage.

One of the most commonly used manipulations to produce a rodent model of ELS has been maternal separation. Studies have shown that maternal separation in rats, as long as it is of sufficient duration (typically 3 h/day during the first 2 postnatal weeks of life) increases anxiety- and depressive-like behaviors in adulthood, suggesting that it has translational validity (Janus, 1987; Huot et al., 2001; Kalinichev et al., 2002; Romeo et al., 2003; Daniels et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2007; Wei et al., 2010; Masrour et al., 2018). However, other studies in both rats and mice have shown considerable variability in behavioral results from maternal separation, with several reports showing no behavioral effect (Lehmann et al., 1999; Eklund and Arborelius, 2006; Slotten et al., 2006; Millstein and Holmes, 2007; Savignac et al., 2011). In addition to inconsistent behavioral findings with this model, concerns have been raised about whether maternal separation mimics neglect, abuse or a combination of both. It has been reported that after prolonged separation, maternal behavior toward pups differs and these differences may be as important, if not more, than the lack of contact with the mother (Boccia and Pedersen, 2001; Huot et al., 2004). Some reports have also observed that dams increase maternal care post-separation possibly attenuating the effects of the separation itself (Millstein and Holmes, 2007). The type of human maltreatment that rodent maternal separation reflects might be important for establishing its translational validity, since human studies have separated early adverse experiences into several categories, including emotional abuse, emotional neglect, physical abuse, physical neglect and sexual abuse (Kendler et al., 2004; van Harmelen et al., 2010; Young and Widom, 2014; Rehan et al., 2017; Gallo et al., 2018) and some studies suggest that the type of maltreatment may be important for the adult outcome in terms of behavioral dysfunction (Huh et al., 2014; Young and Widom, 2014).

To address concerns about the unspecified nature of the maternal separation manipulation, researchers have developed another way to impair maternal care with the limited bedding/nesting model (Brunson et al., 2005; Cui et al., 2006; Ivy et al., 2008; Rice et al., 2008). The most extreme version of this model involves housing dams in a wire mesh floored cage with no bedding and a scarcity of material with which to make a nest, while variations involve just limiting nesting material (Walker et al., 2017). The result is an increase in maternal anxiety and fractured caregiving where behavior toward the pups might be interpreted as abusive (Rice et al., 2008). As with the earlier investigations of the maternal separation model, some studies using this manipulation reported evidence for increased anxiety- and depressive-like behavior in adulthood (Cui et al., 2006; Dalle Molle et al., 2012; Raineki et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2012), supporting its translational validity. However, other studies using this model failed to find an increase in anxiety- or depressive-like behavior (Brunson et al., 2005; Rice et al., 2008; van der Kooij et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2018) raising questions about reliability similar to those observed with the maternal separation model.

Contradictory results of studies using both of these rodent models are puzzling and may be attributable to differences in experimental design. To fully understand these discrepancies, many factors must be considered (Figure 1). First, the genetic background of the experimental animal is important. Human studies have clearly shown genetic predisposition to mood and anxiety disorders and it follows that this factor should be considered in studies using experimental animals to model the human condition. Studies have shown varying effects of maternal separation on anxiety- and depressive-like behaviors in different strains of mice; the C57Bl/6 strain appears to be most resistant to stress compared to other strains, such as the Balb/c strain, which is inherently more anxious (Millstein and Holmes, 2007; Wei et al., 2010; Savignac et al., 2011). However, different studies using the same strain have reported conflicting results with seemingly identical ELS manipulations, so genetic strain differences cannot account for all of the variance in the literature. It should be noted, however, that individual subtler genetic differences within a specific rodent strain may be relevant to establishing vulnerability to such manipulations. That is, ELS manipulations likely impact some animals more than others and such variability may obscure overall group differences in behavior. Second, the sex of the animal should be considered. Somewhat paradoxically given that women exhibit greater prevalence of mood/anxiety disorders than do men (Altemus et al., 2014), several rodent studies have shown that ELS produces either no effect or a reduction in anxiety- and depressive-like behaviors in females (Lehmann et al., 1999; McIntosh et al., 1999; Eklund and Arborelius, 2006; Slotten et al., 2006). These unexpected results raise questions about whether the standard laboratory tests of anxiety- and depressive-like behavior, which were developed for use in males and typically involve measures of behavioral inhibition, are accurate measures of these states in female rodents, given known estrous cycle variations in behavioral activity levels. Clearly, the field would benefit from new sensitive behavioral assays that are useful for both sexes, particularly given the need to correct the under-emphasis of research on females (Clayton and Collins, 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Factors influencing variability in ELS animal models. Cartoon depicting the two most commonly used models of ELS and the different factors influencing behavioral outcomes in adulthood. MS, Maternal separation stress; LB-LN, Limited bedding-Limited nesting.

The timing and duration of the stressful experience during the postnatal period may also be important to consider. In rodent studies, differential effects of early vs. late postnatal stress exposure on depressive-like behaviors have been demonstrated (van der Kooij et al., 2015; Peña et al., 2017). By contrast, however, a recent human study concluded that data on the link between childhood maltreatment and psychopathology do not fit a sensitive period theoretical model (Dunn et al., 2018), again raising questions about the direct translational validity of some ELS models in rodents. It is likely relevant that the HPA axis response to stress is attenuated in pups during the stress hyporesponsive period, a phenomenon that serves a protective effect on the developing brain (Sapolsky and Meaney, 1986). A similar state has been reported in humans up until about 1 year of age, but it does not extend throughout childhood when the majority of reported maltreatment occurs (Gunnar and Donzella, 2002).

The duration of stress seems to be more definitively associated with worse outcomes compared to the timing of stress, and data from human studies support a cumulative and/or recency model of stress effects on vulnerability to psychopathology (Dunn et al., 2018). To address the issue about the duration of stress as well as inconsistencies in the ELS literature, researchers have developed “two-hit” models that incorporate maternal separation followed by additional stress, either shortly thereafter or in adulthood. The models are based on the assumption that the first stressful period may create an internal vulnerability that is alone insufficient to manifest itself behaviorally, but when aggravated by subsequent stress, produces detectable behavioral changes. One set of such studies used longer periods of separation followed by early weaning of pups (George et al., 2010). Early weaning by itself has been shown to increase anxiety-like behavior in adulthood (Kikusui et al., 2004) and when combined with maternal separation, it not only increases anxiety-like behaviors but also results in hyperactivity, gene dysregulation and neuroanatomical changes to the brain; some of which have been observed in humans with a history of early life abuse. Another set of such studies used maternal separation and/or limited bedding followed by exposure to chronic stress in adulthood (Vargas et al., 2016; Peña et al., 2017). Both of these approaches mimic the “dose-response” or “cumulative” stress links to mental illness that have been described in humans. However, like the other rodent models of ELS, data from these two-hit models need to be interpreted with caution as null effects have also been reported (Santarelli et al., 2017; Tan et al., 2017).

Notwithstanding the potential importance of strain, sex, timing, duration, type of stress experience and other factors (Figure 1) across studies as reasons for variable results, it is clear that variable results can emerge even in the face of virtually identical experimental designs. What is the explanation for these differences? While we do not know for certain, there are some important points to consider. First, baseline housing and testing conditions may vary across laboratories in seemingly unspecified ways (Cavigelli et al., 2006; Sorge et al., 2014), adding additional stress to both control and experimental groups and potentially reducing the behavioral differences between them. Second, evidence suggests that rodent maternal behavior varies considerably even within control groups (Francis and Meaney, 1999). In other words, some rat and mouse dams may be more capable of compensating for the effects of maternal separation or limited bedding than others. This could be influenced by the early life experiences of the dams themselves and the amount of stress they were exposed to before entering breeding. This natural variation in maternal behavior may introduce additional variability into ELS-induced long-term behavioral outcomes. Third, perhaps related to the second point, rodent populations likely display considerable individual variability in response to ELS, such that depending on the cohort examined, statistically significant differences in anxiety- and depressive-like behavior may or may not be detectable. Thus, reproducible significant differences may require larger numbers of animals than are often used in such studies, consistent with what has been the norm for human studies (Collins and Tabak, 2014). In addition, these studies might be more informative if the data from rodents subjected to ELS manipulations were analyzed in ways that do not group them together with the assumption that they comprise a homogenous group. In searching for neurobiological mechanisms underlying behavioral signs of mental illness, it may be more fruitful to separate out the experimental animals that show robust ELS-induced increases in anxiety- and depressive-like behavior. This approach might reveal informative correlations between brain changes and relevant behaviors. While this suggestion makes experimental designs and statistical analyses more complicated than commonly used methods of comparing means between groups, it may produce more reliable results across laboratories.

Considering rodent populations as heterogeneous with regard to their susceptibility to ELS-induced behavioral changes would address an interesting parallel with humans. While the connection between early life adversity and mood/anxiety disorders in humans has been widely accepted, it is perhaps less well-known that the majority of people subjected to childhood maltreatment (>70%) do not show anxiety and depression symptoms that are clinically significant (Rehan et al., 2017). Thus, as with rodents, humans display a considerable amount of resilience and resistance to early life adversity, a phenomenon that deserves scientific attention as it may provide clues about how to encourage these characteristics in the entire population. Finally, it deserves mention that many people develop anxiety and mood disorders that are not retrospectively traceable to childhood maltreatment, so examining control rodents that score as more anxious/depressed despite a lack of prior stress manipulation may be informative as well. Here again, looking at individual differences within groups may be most informative and also help to reduce the inconsistency across studies using rodent models of stress-induced mental illness.

Author Contributions

SM wrote the article, edited the article and made the figure. EG wrote the article, edited the article and edited the figure.

Funding

This work was supported by grant NIH MH117459-01 (EG) and a CV Starr Fellowship (SM).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Altemus, M., Sarvaiya, N., and Neill Epperson, C. (2014). Sex differences in anxiety and depression clinical perspectives. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 35, 320–330. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boccia, M. L., and Pedersen, C. A. (2001). Brief vs. long maternal separations in infancy: contrasting relationships with adult maternal behavior and lactation levels of aggression and anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 657–672. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00019-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunson, K. L., Kramár, E., Lin, B., Chen, Y., Colgin, L. L., Yanagihara, T. K., et al. (2005). Mechanisms of late-onset cognitive decline after early-life stress. J. Neurosci. 25, 9328–9338. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2281-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavigelli, S. A., Guhad, F. A., Ceballos, R. M., Whetzel, C. A., Nevalainen, T., Lang, C. M., et al. (2006). Fecal corticoid metabolites in aged male and female rats after husbandry-related disturbances in the colony room. J. Am. Assoc. Lab Anim. Sci. 45, 17–21. doi: 10.1530/EC-17-0338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, J. A., and Collins, F. S. (2014). Policy: NIH to balance sex in cell and animal studies. Nature 509, 282–283. doi: 10.1038/509282a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, F. S., and Tabak, L. A. (2014). Policy: NIH plans to enhance reproducibility. Nature 505, 612–613. doi: 10.1038/505612a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corcoran, C. A., Pierre, P. J., Haddad, T., Bice, C., Suomi, S. J., Grant, K. A., et al. (2012). Long-term effects of differential early rearing in rhesus macaques: behavioral reactivity in adulthood. Dev. Psychobiol. 54, 546–555. doi: 10.1002/dev.20613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, M., Yang, Y., Yang, J., Zhang, J., Han, H., Ma, W., et al. (2006). Enriched environment experience overcomes the memory deficits and depressive-like behavior induced by early life stress. Neurosci. Lett. 404, 208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.05.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalle Molle, R., Portella, A. K., Goldani, M. Z., Kapczinski, F. P., Leistner-Segal, S., Salum, G. A., et al. (2012). Associations between parenting behavior and anxiety in a rodent model and a clinical sample: relationship to peripheral BDNF levels. Trans. Psychiatry 2:e195. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daniels, W. M., Pietersen, C. Y., Carstens, M. E., and Stein, D. J. (2004). Maternal separation in rats leads to anxiety-like behavior and a blunted ACTH response and altered neurotransmitter levels in response to a subsequent stressor. Metab. Brain Dis. 19, 3–14. doi: 10.1023/B:MEBR.0000027412.19664.b3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, E. C., Soare, T. W., Raffeld, M. R., Busso, D. S., Crawford, K. M., Davis, K. A., et al. (2018). What life course theoretical models best explain the relationship between exposure to childhood adversity and psychopathology symptoms: recency, accumulation, or sensitive periods? Psychol. Med. 26, 1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eklund, M. B., and Arborelius, L. (2006). Twice daily long maternal separations in Wistar rats decreases anxiety-like behaviour in females but does not affect males. Behav. Brain Res. 172, 278–285. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, D. D., and Meaney, M. J. (1999). Maternal care and the development of stress responses. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 128–134. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80016-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallo, E. A. G., Munhoz, T. N., Loret de Mola, C., and Murray, J. (2018). Gender differences in the effects of childhood maltreatment on adult depression and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl. 79, 107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, E. D., Bordner, K. A., Elwafi, H. M., and Simen, A. A. (2010). Maternal separation with early weaning: a novel mouse model of early life neglect. BMC Neurosci. 11:123. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnar, M. R., and Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology 27, 199–220. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00045-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzman, D. B., Howell, B., and Sanchez, M. (2016). “Early life stress and development: preclinical science,” in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: From Neurobiology to Treatment, ed J. D. Bremner (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell), 61–80.

Google Scholar

Heim, C., Shugart, M., Craighead, W. E., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2010). Neurobiological and psychiatric consequences of child abuse and neglect. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 671–690. doi: 10.1002/dev.20494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howell, B. R., Grand, A. P., McCormack, K. M., Shi, Y., LaPrarie, J. L., Maestripieri, D., et al. (2014). Early adverse experience increases emotional reactivity in juvenile rhesus macaques: relation to amygdala volume. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 1735–1746. doi: 10.1002/dev.21237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huh, H. J., Kim, S. Y., Yu, J. J., and Chae, J. H. (2014). Childhood trauma and adult interpersonal relationship problems in patients with depression and anxiety disorders. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry 13:26. doi: 10.1186/s12991-014-0026-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huot, R. L., Gonzalez, M. E., Ladd, C. O., Thrivikraman, K. V., and Plotsky, P. M. (2004). Foster litters prevent hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis sensitization mediated by neonatal maternal separation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 279–289. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00028-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huot, R. L., Thrivikraman, K. V., Meaney, M. J., and Plotsky, P. M. (2001). Development of adult ethanol preference and anxiety as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in Long Evans rats and reversal with antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacology 158, 366–373. doi: 10.1007/s002130100701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivy, A. S., Brunson, K. L., Sandman, C., and Baram, T. Z. (2008). Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: a clinically relevant model for early-life stress. Neuroscience 154, 1132–1142. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janus, K. (1987). Effects of early separation of young rats from the mother on their open-field behavior. Physiol. Behav. 40, 711–715. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90272-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, F. K., Delpech, J. C., Thompson, G. J., Wei, L., Hao, J., Herman, P., et al. (2018). Amygdala hyper-connectivity in a mouse model of unpredictable early life stress. Transl. Psychiatry 8:49. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0092-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalinichev, M., Easterling, K. W., Plotsky, P. M., and Holtzman, S. G. (2002). Long-lasting changes in stress-induced corticosterone response and anxiety-like behaviors as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in Long-Evans rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73, 131–140. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00781-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S., Kuhn, J. W., and Prescott, C. A. (2004). Childhood sexual abuse, stressful life events and risk for major depression in women. Psychol. Med. 34, 1475–1482. doi: 10.1017/S003329170400265X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikusui, T, Takeuchi, Y, and Mori, Y (2004). Early weaning induces anxiety and aggression in adult mice. Physiol. Behav. 81, 37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J. G., Ryu, V., Kim, B. T., Kang, D. W., et al. (2007). Depressive behaviors and decreased expression of serotonin reuptake transporter in rats that experienced neonatal maternal separation. Neurosci. Res. 58, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, J., Pryce, C. R., Bettschen, D., and Feldon, J. (1999). The maternal separation paradigm and adult emotionality and cognition in male and female Wistar rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 64, 705–715. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00150-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, S. (1957). Infantile experience and resistance to physiological stress. Science 126:405. doi: 10.1126/science.126.3270.405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masrour, F. F., Peeri, M., Azarbayjani, M. A., and Hosseini, M. J. (2018). Voluntary exercise during adolescence mitigated negative the effects of maternal separation stress on the depressive-like behaviors of adult male rats: role of nmda receptors. Neurochem. Res. 43, 1067–1074. doi: 10.1007/s11064-018-2519-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, J., Anisman, H., and Merali, Z. (1999). Short- and long-periods of neonatal maternal separation differentially affect anxiety and feeding in adult rats: gender-dependent effects. Dev. Brain Res. 113, 97–106. doi: 10.1016/S0165-3806(99)00005-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millstein, R. A., and Holmes, A. (2007). Effects of repeated maternal separation on anxiety- and depression-related phenotypes in different mouse strains. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peña, C. J., Kronman, H. G., Walker, D. M., Cates, H. M., Bagot, R. C., Purushothaman, I., et al. (2017). Early life stress confers lifelong stress susceptibility in mice via ventral tegmental area OTX2. Science 356, 1185–1188. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raineki, C., Cortes, M. R., Belnoue, L., and Sullivan, R. M. (2012). Effects of early-life abuse differ across development: infant social behavior deficits are followed by adolescent depressive-like behaviors mediated by the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 32, 7758–7765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5843-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehan, W., Antfolk, J., Johansson, A., Jern, P., and Santtila, P. (2017). Experiences of severe childhood maltreatment, depression, anxiety and alcohol abuse among adults in Finland. PLoS ONE 12:e0177252. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, C. J., Sandman, C. A., Lenjavi, M. R., and Baram, T. Z. (2008). A novel mouse model for acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology 149, 4892–4900. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-0633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeo, R. D., Mueller, A., Sisti, H. M., Ogawa, S., McEwen, B. S., and Brake, W. G. (2003). Anxiety and fear behaviors in adult male and female C57BL/6 mice are modulated by maternal separation. Horm. Behav. 43, 561–567. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00063-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarelli, S., Zimmermann, C., Kalideris, G., Lesuis, S. L., Arloth, J., Uribe, A., et al. (2017). An adverse early life environment can enhance stress resilience in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology 78, 213–221. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M., and Meaney, M. J. (1986). Maturation of the adrenocortical stress response: neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress hyporesponsive period. Brain Res. 396, 64–76. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(86)90010-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savignac, H. M., Dinan, T. G., and Cryan, J. F. (2011). Resistance to early-life stress in mice: effects of genetic background and stress duration. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 5:13. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schino, G., Speranza, L., and Troisi, A. (2001). Early maternal rejection and later social anxiety in juvenile and adult Japanese macaques. Dev. Psychobiol. 38, 186–190. doi: 10.1002/dev.1012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slotten, H. A., Kalinichev, M., Hagan, J. J., Marsden, C. A., and Fone, K. C. (2006). Long-lasting changes in behavioural and neuroendocrine indices in the rat following neonatal maternal separation: gender-dependent effects. Brain Res. 1097, 123–132. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorge, R. E., Martin, L. J., Isbester, K. A., Sotocinal, S. G., Rosen, S., Tuttle, A. H., et al. (2014). Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat. Methods 11, 629–632. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, S., Ho, H. S., Song, A. Y., Low, J., and Je, H. S. (2017). Maternal separation does not produce a significant behavioral change in mice. Exp. Neurobiol. 26, 390–398. doi: 10.5607/en.2017.26.6.390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Horst, F. C., and van der Veer, R. (2008). Loneliness in infancy: Harry Harlow, John Bowlby and issues of separation. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 42, 325–335. doi: 10.1007/s12124-008-9071-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kooij, M. A., Grosse, J., Zanoletti, O., Papilloud, A., and Sandi, C. (2015). The effects of stress during early postnatal periods on behavior and hippocampal neuroplasticity markers in adult male mice. Neuroscience 311, 508–518. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.10.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Harmelen, A. L., de Jong, P. J., Glashouwer, K. A., Spinhoven, P., Penninx, B. W., and Elzinga, B. M. (2010). Child abuse and negative explicit and automatic self-associations: the cognitive scars of emotional maltreatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 486–494. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, J., Junco, M., Gomez, C., and Lajud, N. (2016). Early life stress increases metabolic risk, hpa axis reactivity, and depressive-like behavior when combined with postweaning social isolation in rats. PLoS ONE 11:e0162665. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, C. D., Bath, K. G., Joels, M., Korosi, A., Larauche, M., Lucassen, P. J., et al. (2017). Chronic early life stress induced by limited bedding and nesting (LBN) material in rodents: critical considerations of methodology, outcomes and translational potential. Stress 20, 421–448. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2017.1343296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. D., Labermaier, C., Holsboer, F., Wurst, W., Deussing, J. M., Muller, M. B., et al. (2012). Early-life stress-induced anxiety-related behavior in adult mice partially requires forebrain corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor 1. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2360–2367. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08148.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, L., David, A., Duman, R. S., Anisman, H., and Kaffman, A. (2010). Early life stress increases anxiety-like behavior in Balb c mice despite a compensatory increase in levels of postnatal maternal care. Horm. Behav. 57, 396–404. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. C., and Widom, C. S. (2014). Long-term effects of child abuse and neglect on emotion processing in adulthood. Child Abuse Negl. 38, 1369–1381. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: early life stress, anxiety, depression, rodent models, resilience

Citation: Murthy S and Gould E (2018) Early Life Stress in Rodents: Animal Models of Illness or Resilience? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:157. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00157

Received: 08 May 2018; Accepted: 06 July 2018;
Published: 31 July 2018.

Edited by:

Xiao-Dong Wang, Zhejiang University, China

Reviewed by:

Brittany Rollins Howell, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United States

Copyright © 2018 Murthy and Gould. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elizabeth Gould, goulde@princeton.edu

Download