Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 08 March 2019
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00042

Ecological Stoichiometry: A Link Between Developmental Speed and Physiological Stress in an Omnivorous Insect

  • 1Institute of Biosciences, Vilnius University, Vilnius, Lithuania
  • 2Department of Biotechnology, Daugavpils University, Daugavpils, Latvia
  • 3Department of Plant Protection, Estonian University of Life Sciences, Tartu, Estonia
  • 4Department of Environmental and Biological Sciences, University of Eastern Finland, Joensuu, Finland
  • 5Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
  • 6English, Drama and Writing Studies, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • 7School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • 8The Bredesen Center, Energy Science and Engineering, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States
  • 9Department of Plant Sciences, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States
  • 10Department of General and Microbial Biochemistry, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
  • 11Department of Ecology, Evolution and Behavior, the Alexander Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
  • 12Department of Biology and Turku Brain and Mind Centre, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
  • 13Department of Botany and Ecology, Faculty of Biology, University of Latvia, Riga, Latvia
  • 14Ecuela Nacional de Estudios Superiores Unidad Morelia, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Morelia, Mexico
  • 15Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States
  • 16Department of Zoology and Animal Ecology, Faculty of Biology, University of Latvia, Riga, Latvia

The elemental composition of organisms belongs to a suite of functional traits that may adaptively respond to fluctuating selection pressures. Life history theory predicts that predation risk and resource limitations impose selection pressures on organisms’ developmental time and are further associated with variability in energetic and behavioral traits. Individual differences in developmental speed, behaviors and physiology have been explained using the pace-of-life syndrome (POLS) hypothesis. However, how an organism’s developmental speed is linked with elemental body composition, metabolism and behavior is not well understood. We compared elemental body composition, latency to resume activity and resting metabolic rate (RMR) of western stutter-trilling crickets (Gryllus integer) in three selection lines that differ in developmental speed. We found that slowly developing crickets had significantly higher body carbon, lower body nitrogen and higher carbon-to-nitrogen ratio than rapidly developing crickets. Slowly developing crickets had significantly higher RMR than rapidly developing crickets. Male crickets had higher RMR than females. Slowly developing crickets resumed activity faster in an unfamiliar relative to a familiar environment. The rapidly developing crickets did the opposite. The results highlight the tight association between life history, physiology and behavior. This study indicates that traditional methods used in POLS research should be complemented by those used in ecological stoichiometry, resulting in a synthetic approach that potentially advances the whole field of behavioral and physiological ecology.

Introduction

Ecological communities consist of a variety of species and are shaped by a complex array of intra- and interspecific interactions that maintain nutrient and energy flows through ecosystems (Meunier et al., 2017; Sperfeld et al., 2017). Importantly, for any given individual, the availability of resources in any particular environment is limited; time, effort and energy used for one purpose diminish those available for another (Stearns, 1992; Sperfeld et al., 2017). This often causes trade-offs in allocations of an individual’s resources to such competing life functions as immunity, reproduction, self-maintenance, development and growth (Roff, 1992; Krams et al., 2013a; Luoto, 2019).

Biotic and abiotic environmental stressors (e.g., predation, food limitation, extreme temperatures, drought, competition, growth in stressful conditions) may challenge organismal homeostasis (Boonstra, 2013; Wingfield, 2013; Ferguson et al., 2018). The stressed individual may then alter its behavior and functional traits to accommodate to the challenge, resulting in implications to their dietary choices and the elemental composition of their bodies and waste materials (Christianson and Creel, 2010; Hawlena and Schmitz, 2010a,b). This suggests that investments in stress tolerance and biochemical and behavioral adaptations to environmental stress may further affect the amount of energy available to each individual (Hochachka and Somero, 2002; Ellis and Del Giudice, 2014; Luoto, 2019). Ecological stoichiometry, a framework based on energetics, links the study of these trade-offs with the relative supply of elements in the environment and the metabolic demands and physiological traits of organisms (Meunier et al., 2017; Sperfeld et al., 2017).

Differences in behavioral and physiological responses to stress can be explained using the pace-of-life syndrome (POLS) hypothesis (Réale et al., 2010; Debecker and Stoks, 2018; Mathot and Frankenhuis, 2018; Royauté et al., 2018). This hypothesis originated from the classic concept of r- and K-selection (MacArthur and Wilson, 1967; Pianka, 1970) and the more recent idea of fast-slow life history continuum (e.g., Promislow and Harvey, 1990; Bielby et al., 2007). It suggests that differences in life history strategies among species or populations are associated with physiological (e.g., metabolic rate; Ricklefs and Wikelski, 2002; Wikelski et al., 2003; Wiersma et al., 2007) and behavioral differences (Wolf et al., 2007; Biro and Stamps, 2008; Réale et al., 2009; Luoto, 2019). The POLS hypothesis predicts that rapidly developing individuals with high activity and boldness have faster life histories (e.g., faster development and reproduction) and higher metabolic rate, which reduces life span through increased oxidative damage (Janssens and Stoks, 2018). This prediction is based on the assumption that high activity and boldness increase resource acquisition. Passive and shy individuals, in contrast, are expected to show the opposite features (Réale et al., 2010).

However, evidence suggests that slower development of prey individuals may also incur stress as it is associated with upregulation of stress-related genes (Gutiérrez-Adán et al., 2004). Wings of late-hatched female damselflies Lestes viridis were found to be more asymmetrical than those of early-hatched ones (De Block et al., 2008). Importantly, selection for slower development confers higher levels of anxiety/neuroticism along the stress reactivity axis in crickets (Krams et al., 2017). The observed anxiety in behavior and resting metabolic rate (RMR) in slowly developing crickets under stressful conditions decreased after a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) treatment (Krams et al., 2018). On the other hand, slower development may be associated with longer lifespan (Brooks and Garratt, 2017; Kecko et al., 2017) which may require an improved immune system (Niemelä et al., 2013; Krams et al., 2015, 2016a). In some species where females have longer lifespan than males, the strength of immune responses and inflammatory immune responses are generally higher in females than in males (Klein, 2012; Klein and Flanagan, 2016; Kecko et al., 2017). However, females often suffer a higher propensity to many autoimmune diseases such as rheumatoid arthritis, fibromyalgia, anxiety and depression (e.g., Dumont-Lagacé et al., 2015), suggesting associations between slower development, longer lifespan and stress resistance (Brooks and Garratt, 2017).

It is considered that rapidly developing individuals are bolder and more stress resistant than slowly developing shy individuals (e.g., Steimer et al., 1997). Nevertheless, how an organism’s developmental speed is linked with elemental body composition, metabolism and behavior is not well understood, and a comprehensive approach that combines stoichiometry with behaviors has been lacking in prior research. Selective lines is an effective method to produce comparable individuals of varying developmental times (Krams et al., 2017). Here, we tested behavioral responses to handling (as a proxy of stress resistance; Adamo et al., 2013), RMR and elemental body composition of three selected lines of western stutter-trilling crickets (Gryllus integer) that differ in developmental speed.

Based on existing findings on the effects of stress on organismal stoichiometry (Hawlena and Schmitz, 2010a,b; Krams et al., 2016b), we predicted higher concentrations of carbon (C), lower nitrogen (N), a greater C/N ratio and greater RMR as indicators of physiological stress in the slow developmental line compared with the rapid line and possibly also with the control line because of the higher sensitivity to antidepressants (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, SSRI) found in the slow and control developmental lines (Krams et al., 2018). We predicted shorter latency of resuming activity among startled slowly developing individuals (as opposed to rapidly developing crickets) in an unfamiliar environment as an indicator of physiological stress. It has been shown that SSRI treatment increased the time to resume movements (i.e., decreased anxiety) of slowly developing crickets in an unfamiliar environment (Krams et al., 2018). Since males and females may differ in their stress responses, we tested for possible sexual dimorphism in concentrations of C, N and the C/N ratio (Bayer and Hobert, 2018).

Materials and Methods

Insects and Selection Lines

The laboratory stock originated from a wild population (Davis, CA, USA). This stock was first maintained at the University of Oulu and the University of Eastern Finland, and then moved to the University of Tartu in Estonia, where the present data were collected. In this study, we tested crickets that had been selected for five generations for their developmental speed. G. integer nymphs were reared individually in plastic containers (28 × 98 × 73 mm: length, width, height, respectively) with a hole of 30 mm in diameter covered with plastic netting for ventilation. Each container was equipped with a shelter made of cardboard. The individual crickets were kept under a constant 12:12 h light–dark cycle, at 26 ± 1°C with ad libitum food consisting of fish flakes (Eheim) and reindeer pellets (Rehuraisio Oy, Poron herkku) and ad libitum water. Although nymphal density does not affect adult behavior, it does increase life history investments in immune function and maturation (Niemelä et al., 2012b).

The selection design consisted of three main selection lines (rapid development, slow development and control). In each generation, offspring were obtained from ~20 families within each main line. For rapid and slow developmental lines, mated males and females were selected according to their maturation time, and only the most rapid or slowest maturing individuals were used for matings in each main line (for more details on selection, see Krams et al., 2017). In the control line, matings were randomized over the whole natural maturation time range. Two months after hatching, random samples of offspring from the rapid developmental line were placed into individual containers in a random order. The same procedure was performed 3 months after hatching in the control line and 4 months after hatching in the slow line in each generation.

After five generations of selection for developmental speed, developmental time (the average maturation time ± SD) for rapidly developing individuals was 91.03 ± 6.06 days (n = 29 crickets), 117.33 ± 7.53 days (n = 30) for the control individuals and 136.17 ± 8.28 days (n = 24) for slowly developing crickets. All groups differed significantly in their developmental time [one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA): F(2,77) = 257.89, P < 0.0001].

Body C and N Content

Following food deprivation of 15 h and water ad libitum, all crickets were immediately frozen at −80°C (Angelantoni Lifescience, Italy). Before elemental analysis, we dried bodies of 29 rapidly developing crickets, 30 control crickets and 24 slowly developing crickets at 60°C for 48 h. Each individual was ground to a homogenous powder and measured for C and N content using a C-N combustion auto-analyzer (Hawlena and Schmitz, 2010a,b; Krams et al., 2016b).

Behavioral Trials: Resuming Activity in a Familiar Environment

We started behavioral trials on day 10 after crickets reached maturity. Before behavioral trials and measuring RMR, we weighed each individual using a Kern analytical balance (ABT 120-4M; Kern and Sohn, Balingen, Germany). Behavioral trials were conducted under constant temperature (25 ± 1°C) and sound-proof conditions. We used dim red light (25 W red incandescent bulb) since Gryllus spp. cannot see long (red) wavelengths properly (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001), which allowed us to observe these nocturnal insects without disturbing them. The crickets were provided with drinking water before the onset of the trials, while food was removed 5 h before the beginning of experimental trials.

We captured the focal cricket in its housing-box and handled it by holding the insect in the hand for 1 min. After the handling procedure, the cricket was placed back in its burrow-like triangular cardboard shelter (5 cm long, with a 1 × 1 × 1 cm entrance). We recorded the latency to resume activity when the insect started to move inside the cardboard shelter. We waited for all crickets to resume activity (max. 908 s). The same procedure was repeated for each individual 4 days later. Latency to resume activity indicates the duration of freezing or immobile state, a widespread anti-predator response occurring in many taxa (Chelini et al., 2009; Krams et al., 2013a,b).

Behavioral Trials: Resuming Activity in an Unfamiliar Environment

Two days after the first test conducted in an environment familiar to the crickets, we handled them for 1 min and then gave them the opportunity to escape into a burrow-like conical plastic Eppendorf test tube (volume 5 mL, Sigma-Aldrich), which was used as an insect chamber. The insect chamber was connected to the respirometer with rubber tubing (Lighton, 2008). When reaching the insect chamber, each of the crickets immediately became completely immobile, as if it was hiding in a burrow. We waited for all crickets to resume active struggling movements (max. 1850 s).

Metabolic Rate Measurements

We measured cricket RMR as the rate of carbon dioxide emission in an incurrent flow-through system. The LI-7000 differential CO2/H2O analyzer (LiCor, Lincoln, NE, USA) was calibrated at different flow rates by means of calibration gases (Trägergase, VEB, Saxon Junkalor GmbH, Dessau; Quinlan and Lighton, 1999; Lighton, 2008) with gas injection (see also Kuusik et al., 2002; Mänd et al., 2006). While measuring CO2 emissions, the insect chamber was perfused with dry (57%RH) CO2-free air, produced by passing air over Drierite (W. A. Hammond Drierite Co. Ltd., Xenia, OH, USA) and soda-lime granules at an airflow rate of 60 ml min−1. Average ambient temperature within the respirometry chamber was 23.60 ± 0.30°C. Baseline drift of the analyzer was corrected during analysis from the measurements at the beginning and end of each trial with the respirometer chamber empty (Duncan, 2003; Duncan and Byrne, 2005; Gray and Bradley, 2006). The respirometric device was combined with an infrared optical system using IR emitting diodes (TSA6203) and IR-sensor diodes (BP104) that were placed on the sides of the insect chamber. IR-diodes made it possible to record CO2 production and to follow movements of each cricket simultaneously. The insects remained in their chambers for 4 h, and we recorded their minimum rates of metabolism at moments when crickets were immobile. As soon as the measurements were over, we returned the crickets back to their plastic housing-boxes. We repeated the trials 5 days later. Since insects differed in their body mass, we used body mass-specific RMR values in this study.

Statistical Analyses

We used two-way ANOVAs with developmental line (slow, rapid, control) and sex as fixed factors to assess differences in elemental composition (C, N and C/N ratio). We considered elemental composition analyses to belong to the same families of tests (Rubin, 2017). We thus controlled for multiple testing using Holm-Bonferroni procedure to adjust P-values. An adjusted P-value < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. Assumptions of homogeneity of variances were met (Levene’s test, P > 0.05). We report only the main effects when no significant interactions between fixed factors were found; otherwise, Tukey’s HSDs are also reported. To test for the effects of developmental line and sex on RMR, linear mixed effects model (LMM) was used. Another LMM was fitted to test for the effects of developmental line, sex and environment (familiar, unfamiliar) on differences in latency to resume activity (log-transformed). In both LMMs, individual cricket ID was included as a random factor to account for the possible correlation of repeated measurements of the same individual. Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS 22 for Windows and the program R, version 3.3.2 (R Development Core Team, 2016).

Results

Carbon

Rapidly developing crickets had less body C (%; 50.5 ± 3.5, mean ± SD) than slowly developing (52.7 ± 3.0, mean ± SD) and control (53.0 ± 3.5, mean ± SD) crickets (Tukey’s tests, P = 0.047 and P = 0.02, respectively), while slowly developing and control crickets did not differ in body C (Tukey’s test, P = 0.96; Figure 1A). The main effect of developmental line to body C was significant (two-way ANOVA: F(2,77) = 4.562, P = 0.039). Body C of females and males did not show significant differences (two-way ANOVA: F(1,77) = 0.962, P = 0.33). There was no significant interaction between developmental line and sex to body C (two-way ANOVA: F(2,77) = 0.484, P = 0.618).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Average carbon percentage (A), nitrogen percentage (B) and carbon-to-nitrogen ratio (C) in Gryllus integer crickets selected for slow development (S, squares, n = 24), rapid development (R, circles, n = 29) and control developmental (C, diamonds, n = 30) lines. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks indicate significant differences between the lines (*P < 0.05).

Nitrogen

In contrast, rapidly developing crickets had higher body N (%; 11.5 ± 1.4, mean ± SD) than slowly developing (10.6 ± 1.2, mean ± SD) and control (10.5 ± 1.5, mean ± SD) crickets (Tukey’s tests, P = 0.031 and P = 0.022, respectively). Slowly developing and control crickets did not differ in body N (Tukey’s test, P = 1.0; Figure 1B). The main effect of developmental line to body N was significant (two-way ANOVA: F(2,77) = 4.452, P = 0.03), while sex had no effect on it (two-way ANOVA: F(1,77) = 1.062, P = 0.306). There was no significant interaction between developmental line and sex to body N (two-way ANOVA: F(2,77) = 0.368, P = 0.693).

C/N Ratio

Rapidly developing crickets had a lower C/N ratio (4.48 ± 0.91, mean ± SD) than control crickets (5.18 ± 1.10, mean ± SD; Tukey’s test, P = 0.028). A marginally non-significant difference was found when comparing rapidly developing crickets with slowly developing crickets (5.1 ± 0.94, mean ± SD; Tukey’s test, P = 0.068). Slowly developing and control crickets did not differ in their C/N ratio (Tukey’s test, P = 0.98; Figure 1C). The main effect of developmental line was significant (two-way ANOVA: developmental line: F(2,77) = 3.904, P = 0.024), while C/N ratios of females and males were not significantly different (two-way ANOVA: F(1,77) = 1.144, P = 0.288). There was no significant interaction between developmental line and sex to C/N (two-way ANOVA: F(2,77) = 0.229, P = 0.796).

Resting Metabolic Rate

Body mass-specific RMR differed significantly between females and males (LMM, F(1,77) = 8.704, P = 0.004), with male crickets having higher RMR than females (Figure 2A), and between developmental lines (LMM, F(2,77) = 16.329, P < 0.0001). The highest mean RMR was recorded in crickets from the slow developmental line and the lowest RMR was recorded in crickets from the rapid developmental line (Figure 2B). We found no significant interaction between developmental line and sex (LMM, F(2,77) = 0.430, P = 0.652).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mass-specific resting metabolic rate (RMR) of females (F, open circle, n = 45) and males (M, closed circle, n = 38; A), selected for slow development (squares, n = 24), rapid development (circles, n = 29) and control developmental (diamonds, n = 30; B) lines. Symbols and error bars show averages and 95% CIs, respectively. Asterisks indicate significant differences (**P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001).

Resuming Activity in Familiar and Unfamiliar Environments

Selection line and environment had significant effects on crickets’ behaviors (LMM, F(2,332) = 44.663, P < 0.0001, and F(1,332) = 5.319, P = 0.022, respectively), while sex had no effect (LMM, F(1,332) = 0.778, P = 0.378; Figure 3). There was also a significant interaction between selection line and environment (LMM, F(2,332) = 452.9, P < 0.0001). Thus, slowly developing crickets resumed activity faster in an unfamiliar but more slowly in a familiar environment on average, while rapidly developing crickets did the opposite. Other interactions were respectively non-significant and marginally non-significant: sex*environment (LMM, F(1,332) = 0.486, P < 0.486) line*sex (LMM, F(2,332) = 3.01, P = 0.051).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Latency to resume activity (back-transformed means ± 95% CIs) of selected slow (squares), rapid (circles), and control (diamonds) developing crickets in familiar (open symbols) and unfamiliar environments (closed symbols). Different letters denote significant differences at P < 0.05.

Discussion

Our results show that differences in POLS (developmental speed) alter insect body stoichiometry and that those changes were associated with different behavioral and physiological stress responses. Thus, developmental speed should be considered as an important factor in research on ecological stoichiometry as it may influence the need for specific nutrients (see Snell-Rood et al., 2015; Camus et al., 2017) and POLS trait interactions more generally.

Surprisingly, slowly developing crickets did not differ significantly from the control line on any of the three stoichiometric parameters while rapidly developing crickets did on all three of them. Rapidly developing crickets had the lowest C, the highest N concentrations and the lowest C/N ratio. This suggests that rapidly developing crickets probably experienced the lowest levels of stress during their nymphal development or were more resilient to stress because of genetic or epigenetic factors. Rapidly developing individuals should benefit from faster growth because this provides a higher chance of surviving to reproduction at low cost (Roff, 1992; Stearns, 1992). This finding is contradictory with predictions arising from life history theory because it has been traditionally assumed that juvenile growth rates operate near their physiological maximum (Stearns and Koella, 1986; Roff, 1992; Stearns, 1992) where tight energy budgets may bring oxidative stress and other costs (Fischer et al., 2004; Slos and Stoks, 2008; Krams et al., 2017). A number of studies confirm that rapid growth is costly, which suggests that growth rates are generally optimized rather than maximized in many species (Gotthard et al., 1994; Nylin et al., 1996; Lankford et al., 2001; Arendt, 2003; Fischer et al., 2004). However, we show that slowly developing and control crickets may be under higher levels of stress as indicated by their higher body C concentration, lower body N and higher C/N ratio. This may be especially true in an unfamiliar environment where slowly developing and control crickets were found to have higher RMR than rapidly developing crickets.

These results can be framed in a larger context of ecological stoichiometry by considering an interesting set of results reported in another species. Grasshoppers (Melanoplus sanguinipes) from a subarctic region have about 80 days shorter growing (rapid growth) season compared to those from temperate areas (Fielding and Defoliart, 2007). This shorter growth period is associated with decreased body mass of the subarctic grasshoppers but improved post-ingestive efficiencies and N assimilations on low- and high-quality foods. The temperate grasshoppers, in contrast, are less efficient in digesting low-quality food. This shows that faster development may not generally affect digestive abilities of orthopterans and are unlikely to be associated with elevated levels of physiological stress.

The current results on behavioral traits support an earlier study which showed that slowly developing crickets (compared with rapidly developing ones) resume their daily activities more slowly after being handled in a familiar (less stressful) environment and faster when being handled in a novel (more stressful) environment (Krams et al., 2017). This suggests that slowly developing, often shy crickets are more stressed than rapidly developing, often bold ones. Previous studies show that slowly and rapidly developing crickets markedly differ in behavioral and physiological traits: slowly developing individuals are shy, generally larger, more stressed under unfamiliar conditions, they show stronger encapsulation responses and have higher RMR and lower maximum metabolic rate (MMR) compared with rapidly developing crickets (Niemelä et al., 2012a, 2013; Krams et al., 2017).

Although field cricket females are bigger in body size than males and can occasionally kill them (Kortet and Hedrick, 2005), we did not find any signs of higher stress in males. It is likely that the higher dominance position of female crickets is outweighed by their higher somatic and reproductive costs because a significant correlation between female body size and fecundity is often observed in G. integer and other insects (Blanckenhorn, 2000; Hedrick and Kortet, 2012; but see Tammaru et al., 2002). It is important to note that crickets lived in individual cages in this study so that males and females could not interact with each other. We hypothesize that living in groups would increase stress in males, which needs to be tested by measuring behavioral reactions, concentrations of hormones and metabolic rate coupled with research methods used in ecological stoichiometry.

While it is possible that hormone concentrations in the brains of slowly developing crickets serves as a proximate mechanism underlying their higher stress levels and differences in body elemental composition (Stevenson et al., 2005; Zhou et al., 2008; Adamo and McKee, 2017; Krams et al., 2018), it is not clear what is the ultimate reason for becoming shy when developing slowly. Intuitively, shy individuals may benefit from limited activities and greater suspiciousness under higher predator risk. However, it is not clear why this adaptation should be reached via a stressed phenotype (e.g., higher C, higher RMR, lower N, slower development). Acquiring a more holistic understanding of these questions would be facilitated by studying crickets’ physiological condition, body elemental composition, neurotransmitter concentrations, antipredator responses, sexual selection and survival under natural conditions to test whether shy personality always co-occurs with elevated anxiety and heightened physiological markers of stress. Future studies should include an assessment of phosphorus concentration which is important for RNA production and serves (quantified as the RNA:DNA ratio) as a proxy for protein synthesis (Janssens et al., 2017). These approaches are needed to further develop the general stress paradigm (Hawlena and Schmitz, 2010a).

Conclusion

The results of this study show that slow development is associated with a stressed phenotype. This phenotype is characterized by shorter behavioral latencies in a novel environment and higher stress levels associated with higher body C and lower N concentrations. This study shows that ecological stoichiometry is a tool that needs to be used alongside other traditional methods to study animal stress. Explicit focus on ecological stoichiometry has the potential to explain contradictory results, to sharpen predictions and to move the general stress research paradigm forward through a more holistic understanding of organismal responses to fluctuating selection pressures.

Author Contributions

GT, IK and RKo: conceptualization. IK, TK, RKr, SH, RKo and MR: designed the methodology. DE, GT, JC-G and IK: performed the formal analysis. SEI, DB, TK, RKr, IK, PJ and MR: performed the experiments. IK, GT, SH and SL: wrote the original draft. TK, RKo, RKr, SEI, DB, DH, PJ, MR, JC-G and SL: wrote, reviewed and edited the submitted version. GT designed the figures. IK and TK: responsible for funding acquisitions, supervision and administration of the project.

Funding

This study was supported by Fulbright Program of the US Department of State. Funding was provided by Latvian Science Council (Latvijas Zinātnes Padome; Grant Nos. 290/2012, lzp-2018/1-0393). The Estonian Ministry of Education and Science [Eesti Teadusagentuur (Estonian Research Council)] supported TK (Grant No. IUT34-8, IUT36-2 and PUT1223), IK (Grant No. PUT1223) and PJ (Grant No. PUT573).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Prof. Christine R.B. Boake, Prof. Todd M. Freeberg and Prof. Gordon M. Burghardt for their support during all phases of this study. Professors Jae H. Park, Mariano Labrador, Ranjan Ganguly and Joshua N. Bembenek kindly provided access to their lab facilities.

References

Adamo, S. A., Kovalko, I., and Mosher, B. (2013). The behavioural effects of predator-induced stress responses in the cricket (Gryllus texensis): the upside of the stress response. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 4608–4614. doi: 10.1242/jeb.094482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adamo, S. A., and McKee, R. (2017). Differential effects of predator cues versus activation of fight-or-flight behaviour on reproduction in the cricket Gryllus texensis. Anim. Behav. 134, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.09.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arendt, J. D. (2003). Reduced burst speed is a cost of rapid growth in anuran tadpoles: problems of autocorrelation and inferences about growth rates. Funct. Ecol. 17, 328–334. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2435.2003.00737.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayer, E. A., and Hobert, O. (2018). Past experience shapes sexually dimorphic neuronal wiring through monoaminergic signalling. Nature 561, 117–121. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0452-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bielby, J., Mace, G. M., Bininda-Emonds, O. R. P., Cardillo, M., Gittleman, J. L., Jones, K. E., et al. (2007). The fast-slow continuum in mammalian life history: an empirical reevaluation. Am. Nat. 169, 748–757. doi: 10.2307/4136994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biro, P. A., and Stamps, J. A. (2008). Are animal personality traits linked to life-history productivity? Trends Ecol. Evol. Amst. 23, 361–368. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanckenhorn, W. U. (2000). The evolution of body size: what keeps organisms small? Q. Rev. Biol. 75, 385–407. doi: 10.1086/393620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, R. (2013). Reality as the leading cause of stress: rethinking the impact of chronic stress in nature. Funct. Ecol. 27, 11–23. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briscoe, A. D., and Chittka, L. (2001). The evolution of color vision in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 471–510. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, R. C., and Garratt, M. G. (2017). Life history evolution, reproduction, and the origins of sex-dependent aging and longevity. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1389, 92–107. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camus, M. F., Fowler, K., Piper, M. W. D., and Reuter, M. (2017). Sex and genotype effects on nutrient-dependent fitness landscapes in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 284:20172237. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chelini, M. C., Willemart, R. H., and Hebets, E. A. (2009). Costs and benefits of freezing behavior in the harvestman Eumesosoma roeweri (Arachnida, Opiliones). Behav. Processes 82, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christianson, D., and Creel, S. (2010). A nutritionally mediated risk effect of wolves on elk. Ecology 91, 1184–1191. doi: 10.1890/09-0221.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debecker, S., and Stoks, R. (2018). Pace of life syndrome under warming and pollution: integrating life history, behavior, and physiology across latitudes. Ecol. Monographs 89:e01332. doi: 10.1002/ecm.1332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Block, M., Campero, M., and Stoks, R. (2008). Developmental costs of rapid growth in a damselfly. Ecol. Entomol. 33, 313–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2311.2007.00957.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumont-Lagacé, M., St-Pierre, C., and Perreault, C. (2015). Sex hormones have pervasive effects on thymic epithelial cells. Sci. Rep. 5:12895. doi: 10.1038/srep12895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, F. D. (2003). The role of the subelytral cavity in respiration in a tenebrionid beetle, Onymacris multistriata (Tenebrionidae: Adesmiini). J. Insect Physiol. 49, 339–346. doi: 10.1016/s0022-1910(03)00018-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, F. D., and Byrne, M. J. (2005). The role of the mesothoracic spiracles in respiration in flighted and flightless dung beetles. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 907–914. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, B. J., and Del Giudice, M. (2014). Beyond allostatic load: rethinking the role of stress in regulating human development. Dev. Psychopathol. 26, 1–20. doi: 10.1017/s0954579413000849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, L. V., Kortet, R., and Sinclair, B. J. (2018). Eco-immunology in the cold: the role of immunity in shaping the overwintering survival of ectotherms. J. Exp. Biol. 221:jeb163873. doi: 10.1242/jeb.163873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fielding, D. J., and Defoliart, L. S. (2007). Growth, development, and nutritional physiology of grasshoppers from subarctic and temperate regions. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 80, 607–618. doi: 10.1086/521801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, K., Zeilstra, I., Hetz, S. N., and Fiedler, K. (2004). Physiological costs of growing fast: does accelerated growth reduce pay-off in adult fitness? Evol. Ecol. 18, 343–353. doi: 10.1007/s10682-004-2004-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotthard, K., Nylin, S., and Wiklund, C. (1994). Adaptive variation in growth rate: life history costs and consequences in the speckled wood butterfly, Pararge aegeria. Oecologia 99, 281–289. doi: 10.1007/bf00627740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, E. M., and Bradley, T. J. (2006). Evidence from mosquitoes suggests that cyclic gas exchange and discontinuous gas exchange are two manifestations of a single respiratory pattern. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 1603–1611. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez-Adán, A., Rizos, D., Fair, T., Moreira, P. N., Pintado, B., de la Fuente, J., et al. (2004). Effect of speed of development on mRNA expression pattern in early bovine embryos cultured in vivo or in vitro. Mol. Reprod. Dev. 68, 441–448. doi: 10.1002/mrd.20113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawlena, D., and Schmitz, O. J. (2010a). Herbivore physiological response to predation risk and implications for ecosystem nutrient dynamics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 15503–15507. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009300107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawlena, D., and Schmitz, O. J. (2010b). Physiological stress as a fundamental mechanism linking predation to ecosystem functioning. Am. Nat. 176, 537–556. doi: 10.1086/656495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedrick, A. V., and Kortet, R. (2012). Effects of body size on selectivity for mating cues in different sensory modalities. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 105, 160–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01786.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochachka, P. W., and Somero, G. N. (2002). Biochemical Adaptation: Mechanism and Process in Physiological Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Janssens, L., Op de Beeck, L., and Stoks, R. (2017). Stoichiometric responses to an agricultural pesticide are modified by predator cues. Environ. Sci. Technol. 51, 581−588. doi: 10.1021/acs.est.6b03381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssens, L., and Stoks, R. (2018). Rapid larval development under time stress reduces adult life span through increasing oxidative damage. Funct. Ecol. 32, 1036–1045. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.13068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kecko, S., Mihailova, A., Kangassalo, K., Elferts, D., Krama, T., Krams, R., et al. (2017). Sex-specific compensatory growth in the larvae of the greater wax moth Galleria mellonella. J. Evol. Biol. 30, 1910–1918. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, S. L. (2012). Sex influences immune responses to viruses and efficacy of prophylaxis and treatments for viral diseases. Bioessays 34, 1050–1059. doi: 10.1002/bies.201200099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, S. L., and Flanagan, K. L. (2016). Sex differences in immune responses. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 16, 626–638. doi: 10.1038/nri.2016.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kortet, R., and Hedrick, A. (2005). The scent of dominance: female field crickets use odour to predict the outcome of male competition. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 77–83. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0011-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I., Burghardt, G. M., Krams, R., Trakimas, G., Kaasik, A., Luoto, S., et al. (2016a). A dark cuticle allows higher investment in immunity, longevity and fecundity in a beetle upon a simulated parasite attack. Oecologia 182, 99–109. doi: 10.1007/s00442-016-3654-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I., Eichler Inwood, S., Trakimas, G., Krams, R., Burghardt, G. M., Butler, D. M., et al. (2016b). Short-term exposure to predation affects body elemental composition, climbing speed and survival ability in Drosophila melanogaster. PeerJ 4:e2314. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I., Daukšte, J., Kivleniece, I., Kaasik, A., Krama, T., Freeberg, T. M., et al. (2013a). Trade-off between cellular immunity and life span in mealworm beetles Tenebrio molitor. Curr. Zool. 59, 340–346. doi: 10.1093/czoolo/59.3.340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I., Kivleniece, I., Kuusik, A., Krama, T., Freeberg, T. M., Mänd, R., et al. (2013b). Predation selects for low resting metabolic rate and consistent individual differences in anti-predator behavior in a beetle. Acta Ethol. 16, 163–172. doi: 10.1007/s10211-013-0147-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I., Kecko, S., Kangassalo, K., Moore, F. R., Jankevics, E., Inashkina, I., et al. (2015). Effects of food quality on trade-offs among growth, immunity and survival in the greater wax moth Galleria mellonella. Insect Sci. 22, 431–439. doi: 10.1111/1744-7917.12132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I. A., Niemelä, P. T., Trakimas, G., Krams, R., Burghardt, G. M., Krama, T., et al. (2017). Metabolic rate associates with, but does not generate covariation between, behaviours in western stutter-trilling crickets, Gryllus integer. Proc. Biol. Sci. 284:20162481. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.2481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krams, I., Trakimas, G., Kecko, S., Elferts, D., Krams, R., Luoto, S., et al. (2018). Linking organismal growth, coping styles, stress reactivity, and metabolism via responses against a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor in an insect. Sci. Rep. 8:8599. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26722-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuusik, A., Martin, A. J., Mänd, M., Hiiesaar, K., Metspalu, L., and Tartes, U. (2002). Interrelations of gas exchange cycles, body movements and heartbeats in the foragers of bumblebee Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae) at low temperatures. Eur. J. Entomol. 99, 209–214. doi: 10.14411/eje.2002.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lankford, T. E. Jr., Billerbeck, J. M., and Conover, D. O. (2001). Evolution of intrinsic growth rates and energy acquisition rates. II. Trade-offs with vulnerability to predation in Menidia menidia. Evolution 55, 1873–1881. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb00836.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lighton, J. R. B. (2008). Measuring Metabolic Rate: A Manual for Scientists. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Luoto, S. (2019). An updated theoretical framework for human sexual selection: from ecology, genetics, and life history to extended phenotypes. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0103-6 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacArthur, R. H., and Wilson, E. O. (1967). The Theory of Island Biogeography. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Mänd, M., Kuusik, A., Martin, A. J., Williams, I. H., Luik, A., Karise, R., et al. (2006). Regular periods of abdominal contractions recorded from larvae of the bumblebee, Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Eur. J. Entomol. 103, 319–322. doi: 10.14411/eje.2006.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathot, K. J., and Frankenhuis, W. E. (2018). Models of pace-of-life syndromes (POLS): a systematic review. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72:41. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2459-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meunier, C. L., Boersma, M., El-Sabaawi, R., Halvorson, H. M., Herstoff, E. M., Van de Waal, D. B., et al. (2017). From elements to function: toward unifying ecological stoichiometry and trait-based ecology. Front. Environ. Sci. 5:18. doi: 10.3389/fenvs.2017.00018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemelä, P. T., Dingemanse, N., Alioravainen, N., Vainikka, A., and Kortet, R. (2013). Personality pace-of-life hypothesis: testing genetic associations among personality and life-history. Behav. Ecol. 24, 935–941. doi: 10.1093/beheco/art014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemelä, P. T., Vainikka, A., Hedrick, A. V., and Kortet, R. (2012a). Integrating behavior with life history: boldness of the field cricket, Gryllus integer during ontogeny. Funct. Ecol. 26, 450–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2011.01939.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemelä, P. T., Vainikka, A., Lahdenperä, S., and Kortet, R. (2012b). Nymphal density, behavioral development, and life history in a field cricket. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 645–652. doi: 10.1007/s00265-011-1312-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nylin, S., Gotthard, K., and Wiklund, C. (1996). Reaction norms for age and size at maturity in Lasiommata butterflies: predictions and tests. Evolution 50, 1351–1358. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1996.tb02377.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pianka, E. R. (1970). On r- and K-selection. Am. Nat. 104, 592–597. doi: 10.1086/282697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Promislow, D. E. L., and Harvey, P. H. (1990). Living fast and dying young: a comparative analysis of life-history variation among mammals. J. Zool. 220, 417–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1990.tb04316.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinlan, M. C., and Lighton, J. R. B. (1999). Respiratory physiology and water relations of three species of Pogonomyrmex harvester ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Physiol. Entomol. 24, 293–302. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3032.1999.00140.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/.

Réale, D., Garant, D., Humphries, M. M., Bergeron, P., Careau, V., and Montiglio, P. O. (2010). Personality and the emergence of the pace-of-life syndrome concept at the population level. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 365, 4051–4063. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Réale, D., Martin, J., Coltman, D. W., Poissant, J., and Festa-Bianchet, M. (2009). Male personality, life-history strategies and reproductive success in a promiscuous mammal. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 1599–1607. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01781.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricklefs, R. E., and Wikelski, M. (2002). The physiology/life-history nexus. Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, 462–468. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(02)02578-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roff, D. A. (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories: Theory and Analysis. New York, NY: Chapman and Hall.

Google Scholar

Royauté, R., Berdal, M. A., Garrison, C. R., and Dochtermann, N. A. (2018). Paceless life? A meta-analysis of the pace-of-life syndrome hypothesis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72:64. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2472-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, M. (2017). Do p values lose their meaning in exploratory analyses? It depends how you define the familywise error rate. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 21, 269–275. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slos, S., and Stoks, R. (2008). Predation risk induces stress proteins and reduces antioxidant defense. Funct. Ecol. 22, 637–642. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01424.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snell-Rood, E., Cothran, R., Espeset, A., Jeyasingh, P., Hobbie, S., and Morehouse, N. I. (2015). Life-history evolution in the anthropocene: effects of increasing nutrients on traits and trade-offs. Evol. Appl. 8, 635–649. doi: 10.1111/eva.12272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperfeld, E., Wagner, N. D., Halvorson, H. M., Malishev, M., and Raubenheimer, D. (2017). Bridging ecological stoichiometry and nutritional geometry with homeostasis concepts and integrative models of organism nutrition. Funct. Ecol. 31, 286–296. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stearns, S. C. (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Stearns, S. C., and Koella, J. C. (1986). The evolution of phenotypic plasticity in life-history traits: predictions of reaction norms for age and size at maturity. Evolution 40, 893–913. doi: 10.2307/2408752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steimer, T., la Fleur, S., and Schulz, P. E. (1997). Neuroendocrine correlates of emotional reactivity and coping in male rats from the Roman high (RHA/Verh)- and low (RLA/Verh)-avoidance lines. Behav. Genet. 27, 503–512. doi: 10.1023/A:1021448713665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, P. A., Dyakonova, V., Rillich, J., and Schildberger, K. (2005). Octopamine and experience-dependent modulation of aggression in crickets. J. Neurosci. 25, 1431–1441. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4258-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tammaru, T., Esperk, T., and Castellanos, I. (2002). No evidence for costs of being large in females of Orgyia spp. (Lepidoptera, Lymantriidae): larger is always better. Oecologia 133, 430–438. doi: 10.1007/s00442-002-1057-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiersma, P., Muñoz-Garcia, A., Walker, A., and Williams, J. B. (2007). Tropical birds have a slow pace of life. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 9340–9345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702212104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikelski, M., Spinney, L., Schelsky, W., Scheuerlein, A., and Gwinner, E. (2003). Slow pace of life in tropical sedentary birds: a common-garden experiment on four stonechat populations from different latitudes. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 2383–2388. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, J. C. (2013). Ecological processes and the ecology of stress: the impacts of abiotic environmental factors. Funct. Ecol. 27, 37–44. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, M., van Doorn, G. S., Leimar, O., and Weissing, F. J. (2007). Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal personalities. Nature 447, 581–584. doi: 10.1038/nature05835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, C., Rao, Y., and Rao, Y. (2008). A subset of octopaminergic neurons are important for Drosophila aggression. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1059–1067. doi: 10.1038/nn.2164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: carbon-to-nitrogen ratio, developmental speed, ecological stoichiometry, elemental body composition, trait-based ecology, Gryllus integer, pace-of-life syndrome, physiological stress

Citation: Trakimas G, Krams R, Krama T, Kortet R, Haque S, Luoto S, Eichler Inwood S, Butler DM, Jõers P, Hawlena D, Rantala MJ, Elferts D, Contreras-Garduño J and Krams I (2019) Ecological Stoichiometry: A Link Between Developmental Speed and Physiological Stress in an Omnivorous Insect. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:42. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00042

Received: 29 September 2018; Accepted: 14 February 2019;
Published: 08 March 2019.

Edited by:

Evan Preisser, University of Rhode Island, United States

Reviewed by:

Varvara Dyakonova, Koltzov Institute of Developmental Biology (RAS), Russia
Brandon Barton, Mississippi State University, United States

Copyright © 2019 Trakimas, Krams, Krama, Kortet, Haque, Luoto, Eichler Inwood, Butler, Jõers, Hawlena, Rantala, Elferts, Contreras-Garduño and Krams. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Indrikis Krams, indrikis.krams@ut.ee

Download