Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 11 April 2019
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00066

Long-Term Behavioral Effects of Post-weaning Social Isolation in Males and Females

Deena M. Walker1 Ashley M. Cunningham1 Jill K. Gregory2 Eric J. Nestler1*
  • 1Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, United States
  • 2Academic IT: Instructional Technology Group, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, United States

Adolescence is a developmental period associated with vast neural and behavioral changes which are accompanied by altered sensitivity to stimuli, both stressful and rewarding. Perturbations, especially stressful stimuli, during this period have been shown to alter behavior in adulthood. Social isolation rearing is one such perturbation. This review highlights the long-term behavioral consequences of adolescent social isolation rearing in rodents with a specific focus on anxiety- and addiction-related behaviors. Sex-specific effects are discussed where data are available. We then consider changes in monoaminergic neurotransmission as one possible mechanism for the behavioral effects described. This research on both normative and perturbed adolescent development is crucial to understanding and treating the increased vulnerability to psychiatric disorders seen in humans during this life stage.

Introduction

As individuals transition from childhood to adulthood, they gain sexual maturity as well as the cognitive, emotional, and social skills needed to establish independence from their parents (Spear, 2000). This transition, termed adolescence, is a time of increased exploration, including increased sensation seeking, risk taking, and drug use (Steinberg, 2004; Lipari and Jean-Francois, 2013). This dynamic transition is a developmental period associated with the emergence of psychiatric disorders such as anxiety, depression, eating disorders, and schizophrenia (Kessler et al., 2005). Though it is unclear why such disorders emerge during adolescence, one hypothesis suggests a developmental mismatch between striatal driven sensation seeking (risk taking) and prefrontal inhibition of impulsivity (behavioral control). This mismatch seems to lead to a greater sensitivity to both rewarding and stressful stimuli (Casey and Jones, 2010). It has been posited that the increased sensitivity to stress during adolescence may contribute to the increased incidence of psychiatric illness at this stage in life (Grant, 1997; Grant and Dawson, 1998; Turner and Lloyd, 2004). In adults, it is clear that individual differences in stress sensitivity influence susceptibility (vs. resilience) to numerous psychiatric disorders including depression, anxiety, and substance use disorders (SUDs). Additionally, the effects of stress are exacerbated when they occur during critical developmental periods including during gestation and neonatal development (Bagot et al., 2014; Chen and Baram, 2016). Recent evidence suggests that adolescence is another key developmental window during which stressful experiences may result in long-term, even permanent alterations to brain structure and function (Burke et al., 2017). Hence, research into the neurobiological underpinnings of adolescence may elucidate a basic understanding of normative social, emotional, reproductive, and cognitive development as well as contribute to the understanding of health risks and disorders that impact this life stage.

Adolescence and the Development of Social Reward

Adolescence is the developmental process that surrounds pubertal onset and the attainment of reproductive capacity. While puberty and adolescence are often used synonymously, they are very different processes. Puberty is defined as the attainment of adult reproductive competency and involves the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis. Adolescence, on the other hand, is the period surrounding pubertal onset and includes a longer window in which an organism prepares for independence from their parents. Specific behaviors emerge during this time which facilitate this transition including play behavior, altered social interactions, and increased exploration (Figure 1A; Steinberg, 2004; Lipari and Jean-Francois, 2013). During this period, there is a qualitative shift in the salience of social reward in particular (Spear, 2000). In humans, a hallmark of adolescence is a change in the quantity and quality of social interactions with peers and family which includes an increase in time spent with peers (Larson et al., 1996). Rather than turning to their family, adolescents rely on their contemporaries for social support and become increasingly sensitive to treatment by their peers (Ladd et al., 2014). Evidence suggests that social interactions during adolescence can influence the development and maintenance of maladaptive behaviors in adults (Patterson et al., 1992; Hankin et al., 1998). In fact, negative peer influences are a strong predictor of depression in adolescence and adolescent depression is associated with a life-time risk for major depressive disorder (MDD; Thapar et al., 2012). It is easy to think of the re-organization of a social structure during adolescence only in the context of the emergence of various psychiatric disorders (Kessler et al., 2005). However, it is important to note that the restructuring of social organization during adolescence is necessary for social species to develop behavioral strategies that are essential for survival in adulthood (Gopnik et al., 2017). Additionally, the association of such reorganization with pubertal onset is thought to decrease the likelihood of inbreeding within a social group by increasing exposure to genetically distinct individuals at a time when reproductive behaviors emerge (Lawson Handley and Perrin, 2007). If perturbations disrupt this crucial developmental window, long-term consequences in behavior can persist into adulthood and influence an organism’s social and sexual competency (Schulz et al., 2009; McCormick and Green, 2013; McCormick et al., 2013a,b).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A summary of how adolescent isolation rearing alters behavioral outcomes in male and female rodents. (A) A schematic of developmental markers from birth to adulthood in male (solid lines) and female (dashed lines) rodents. Social interactions/behaviors are indicated at each development milestone and correspond to changes in gonadal hormones and changes within the dopamine (DA) system. (B) Effects of isolation rearing on anxiety-related behaviors. In males, stress early in adolescence has opposite effects on anxiety-related behaviors than late adolescent isolation. (C) Effects of isolation rearing on addiction-related behaviors. While little information is available regarding how adolescent isolation rearing affects females, it increases addiction-related behaviors in males.

Similar to humans, adolescent changes in social interactions are prevalent in rodents. Many of these changes are sex-specific. Adolescent male rats exhibit a greater preference for social stimuli in a conditioned place preference (CPP) test when compared to adults (Douglas et al., 2004; Yates et al., 2013) or to females (Douglas et al., 2004; Weiss et al., 2015). Male rodents also place a greater value on peer-directed activities including play behaviors (Pellis and Pellis, 2017). However, it is important to note that this effect is most pronounced in males deprived of social interactions. Additionally, a peer-paired chamber negates cocaine (Zernig et al., 2013) and amphetamine (Yates et al., 2013) induced CPP in adolescent males but not females (amphetamine only; Weiss et al., 2015). These data suggest that there are striking sex differences in responses to social reward in adolescent rodents with males displaying a greater sensitivity to social reward than females. It is thought that adolescent-specific social experiences result in permanent neural and hormonal changes essential for the development of cognitive processes, which enable effective coping in adulthood (Spear, 2000). Therefore, the observed sex differences in sensitivity to social reward may profoundly influence neural circuitry involved in reward as well as sex differences in reward-associated behavior in adulthood.

Evidence of Increased Stress Sensitivity During Adolescence

Mounting evidence suggests that adolescent animals are more sensitive to both acute and chronic/repeated stressors, and this enhanced sensitivity may contribute to an increased vulnerability to psychiatric disorders that arise during the adolescent period. The convergence of multiple factors results in a unique state with regard to stress sensitivity. First, adolescent rodents exhibit prolonged stress-induced corticosterone responses when compared to adults (Romeo, 2010) or pre-weanlings (Sapolsky and Meaney, 1986). Numerous studies have identified a protracted adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and corticosterone response in adolescent (~postnatal day P30) male and female rodents, which is induced with both physical and psychological stressors (Romeo, 2013). Similar effects have been observed in humans. Adolescent boys and girls (15–17 years) display a greater stress-induced cortisol increase when compared to younger individuals (9–13 years). However, it should be noted that adults were not included in these studies, making it difficult to determine if this effect is specific to adolescence (Gunnar et al., 2009; Stroud et al., 2009). Not only are adolescents more sensitive to stressors, they also sensitize to repeated stressful stimuli. Unlike adults, which habituate to a repeated stressor, adolescent rodents display an enhanced corticosterone response to repeated stressors (Romeo et al., 2006; Lui et al., 2012). Therefore, under chronic stress conditions, the adolescent brain is exposed to a vastly different hormonal environment than adults. Furthermore, evidence suggests that the adolescent brain may be more sensitive to the effects of corticosterone. Specifically, corticosterone-induced gene expression of glutamate receptor subunits in the hippocampus is enhanced in late adolescence (P42) when compared to adult animals (P67; Lee et al., 2003). Finally, key stress-sensitive brain areas, including the amygdala (AMY), hippocampus (HIP), and prefrontal cortex (PFC; Giedd and Rapoport, 2010), and associated neurotransmitter systems continue to develop throughout adolescence (Figure 1A; Walker et al., 2017). These brain regions express glucocorticoid receptors at relatively high levels throughout adolescence (Vazquez et al., 1998; Romeo, 2013; Dziedzic et al., 2014). Taken together, these data suggest that the adolescent brain is exceptionally sensitive to stressful experiences and, given the crucial developmental changes that occur during adolescence, stress exposure could result in long-term alterations in the brain and behavior.

Adolescence and Social Stress

As mentioned above, social reward develops during adolescence. Additionally, the adolescent organism may be more sensitive to stress. Therefore, it follows that adolescence is a period of enhanced sensitivity to social stress, specifically, disruptions to normal development of social structure could influence behaviors in the long-term. Indeed, there is ample evidence that disruption of social structures during adolescence influence reward-, anxiety-, and depression-associated behaviors in rodent models. For example, social instability stress (e.g., a stressor in which animals are housed with different cage mates over a period of time, thus preventing the establishment of stable social hierarchies during adolescent development) has been shown to influence depressive- and anxiety-like behaviors as well as reward-associated behaviors including drug preference in adulthood (McCormick and Green, 2013). In addition to social instability stress, a number of stressors that have been utilized to study the long-term effects of social disruptions on adult behavior (Burke et al., 2017). One of the most prominent social stressors utilized is post-weaning social isolation stress in which an animal is housed individually throughout adolescent development. Although protocols differ, in this paradigm an animal is placed in a cage by itself throughout the adolescent period so that it is deprived of social interactions during this developmental window. In most paradigms, the timing of isolation coincides with the initiation and enhancement of play behavior (Figure 1A). Play behavior, or play fighting, occurs in many species. It is a form of non-serious fighting in which animals engage in pinning, rolling, and attacking the nape of the necks of their cage-mates. Partners take turns attacking and defending against the behaviors and rarely do the bouts turn into serious aggressive attacks. In fact, many of the behaviors involved in play resemble pre-copulatory behaviors and some hypothesize that play behavior is important for preparing animals for adult social behaviors, including defense and copulation, by providing an opportunity to practice such behaviors as juveniles (Pellis and Pellis, 2017). Play behavior is specific to the juvenile/adolescent period. In rats, play behavior emerges around P16 and increases until it peaks on ~P30, after which it declines into adulthood (Baenninger, 1967; Panksepp, 1981). The fact that play behavior is specific to the adolescent period and resembles those behaviors necessary for reproductive success suggests it may be crucial for the proper development of social reward. Thus, stressors that prevent exposure to play, such as adolescent social isolation, may result in long-term behavioral deficits in social reward and behaviors associated with reward in general.

Post-weaning social isolation stress has been used widely to understand how social stress influences behaviors in the adolescent model with conflicting results. The differences are more than likely due to differences in experimental design, length of separation, and the timing of behavioral testing (Lukkes et al., 2009b). This review highlights the long-term behavioral consequences of social stress during the adolescent period with a specific focus on social isolation stress. For the long-term effects of other forms of social stress please see Burke et al. (2017).

The social isolation model has the potential to become all the more relevant with the recent changes in social structure that have emerged due to the use of smartphones and social media. Recent evidence in humans suggests that adolescent males and females who spend more time on social media are lonelier than their peer counterparts (Primack et al., 2017). This changing dynamic within the human population makes understanding the consequences of disrupted adolescent social structures critical, now more than ever. While it has been used extensively in the field to understand how early-life experience might influence adult behavior, there currently is no standard for the implementation of adolescent social isolation stress. Generally, this protocol is considered to be relevant to loneliness, which has been shown to be a risk factor for anxiety, depression, and addiction in numerous studies. Most isolation paradigms include a period of isolation beginning around P21–P30 and ending after 3–8 weeks. Animals are either shipped to the facility on ~P21 or born onsite. At weaning, the animal is housed alone in a standard cage with access to food and water. Generally, sensory cues are not limited, in that they can see, hear, and smell other animals in the colony and experience normal husbandry procedures. However, some studies have utilized opaque cages to limit stimulation still further. Given the vast neuronal, endocrine, and behavioral changes that are associated with adolescence, the timing of isolation is critical for the long-term behavioral effects. For example, given the progression of play behavior mentioned above (see “Evidence of Increased Stress Sensitivity During Adolescence” section), starting isolation on P21 vs. P28 can lead to critical differences in social reward development. Additionally, there are known strain and sex differences in the timing of puberty (Nelson et al., 1990; Krewson et al., 2004; Parent et al., 2005; Zhou et al., 2012; Corre et al., 2016), suggesting that these small but significant differences in isolation paradigms may also coincide with vastly different hormonal and neuroendocrine profiles across studies. Therefore, it is critical to develop an understanding of normative adolescent development in order to fully understand the consequences of adolescent experience on adult behaviors.

Additionally, in most cases, animals are not resocialized before behavioral testing (Lukkes et al., 2009b; Burke et al., 2017). This adds a level of uncertainty to the analysis, as it is difficult to determine if the effects are due to social isolation throughout their lives or if the adolescent period is indeed a sensitive period to the effects of adolescent stress. Throughout the review, the term “isolation rearing” will be used when discussing studies that do not resocialize and those studies that resocialize are noted to help clarify effects that may be sensitive to adolescent-specific perturbations. In an attempt to address this issue, two well-known studies sought to determine a “critical window” for social isolation stress on anxiety- and reward-associated behaviors. Einon and Morgan (Einon and Morgan, 1977) first addressed this issue by comparing different isolation windows (P16–P25; P25–P45; and P90–P180) to group-housed (GH) controls on anxiety-related behaviors and neophobia. Isolated individuals took longer to enter the center in an open field test regardless of the timing of isolation, an effect that was reversed by resocialization, suggesting that isolation stress influences open field exploration independent of the developmental window. However, it should be noted that the effect was most pronounced when the isolation took place beginning on P25, suggesting an interaction between stress and adolescent period. Also, animals isolated from P25 to P45 fail to habituate to the presence of a novel object even after resocialization. Together, these data suggest that isolation during adolescence has pronounced effects on novelty seeking which cannot be reversed by resocialization (Einon and Morgan, 1977). Regarding reward-associated behavior, Whitaker et al. (2013) showed that male rats isolated from P21 to P43, but not P21–P28 or P42–P63, showed a greater preference for EtOH and amphetamine (AMPH), altered neuronal activity in the ventral tegmental area (VTA), greater sensitivity of VTA neurons to corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), and altered glutamatergic signaling in the VTA. They went on to demonstrate that resocialization from P43 to P63 did not reverse the effects on VTA neuronal activity. However, behavior was not examined after resocialization (Whitaker et al., 2013). These data provide valuable information and could help standardize the adolescent social isolation protocol in the field and determine sensitive windows of susceptibility for social stress in rodents.

Social Isolation Stress Disrupts Adult Behavior

Anxiety-Related Behaviors

The adolescent period is a crucial time for corticolimbic development. Alterations in dendritic pruning have been observed in almost all regions of the mesocorticolimbic circuitry, and connectivity between these critical brain regions is maturing during the adolescent process (Figure 1A; Walker et al., 2017). While further research is necessary, there is evidence that stress during adolescence disrupts connectivity between key mesocorticolimbic regions and alters dendritic pruning (Eiland and Romeo, 2013). Experimental disruption of such structure/connectivity, either through lesion studies (Winter et al., 2007; Koenigs and Grafman, 2009) or other types of circuity manipulations (Felix-Ortiz et al., 2013, 2016; Felix-Ortiz and Tye, 2014; Bagot et al., 2015) in adults alters anxiety- and depression-related behaviors in rodents. Therefore, it is not surprising that studies characterizing the behavioral effects of adolescent stress have focused on anxiety-related behaviors. Generally, exposure to physical and psychological stress in adolescence results in increased anxiety-related behavior (Romeo, 2017) and social stress, in particular, has a profound effect on adult behavior (Burke et al., 2017). However, studies investigating the effects of social isolation stress have produced inconsistent results (Table 1). These differences are likely due to differences in experimental design. For example, Holson et al. (1991) showed that the anxiogenic effects of isolation rearing could be eliminated by handling the isolated animals a few times per week. Others have suggested that differences in light cycle or time of testing may contribute to the inconsistent effects (Arakawa, 2018). However, even given these inconsistencies some themes are beginning to emerge from the data (Table 1) and many of the results highlight striking sex differences (Figure 1B) in the effects of social isolation rearing in particular.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Behavioral effects of isolation rearing/adolescent social isolation on anxiety-related behaviors.

Elevated Plus Maze

In general, studies investigating how social isolation rearing influences behavior on an elevated plus maze have produced consistent results (Table 1). During testing, an animal is placed on an “X” shaped maze, that stands ~3 feet off the floor, and is composed of two enclosed arms and two open arms, all of which the animal can explore freely. It is assumed that an animal who spends more time exploring the open arms is less “anxious” because this region of the maze is exposed and is, therefore, more dangerous (Figure 2A; Lezak et al., 2017). However, this interpretation should be viewed with caution, particularly when investigating sex differences in behavior. For example, females spend more time in the open arms than males, however, careful analysis of this behavior indicated that this is due to increased activity and not decreased anxiety- or fear-related behavior (Fernandes et al., 1999).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A schematic of the behavioral tests utilized to assess the effects of social isolation rearing on anxiety- (top) and addiction- (bottom) related behaviors. (A,B) Anxiety-related behavioral tests include elevated plus maze (A) and open field (B). An animal is considered less anxious if they spend more time in the open arm of an elevated plus maze and the center of an open field. (C,D) Addiction-related behavioral tests include conditioned place preference (CPP; C) and self-administration (SA; D). An animal who forms a preference for the side of a chamber where they received a drug in CPP is considered to find that drug more rewarding (C). In SA paradigms (D), animals learn that pressing the active lever but not the inactive lever results in an injection of a drug of abuse. Animals are thought to find drugs of abuse more rewarding if they learn the operant task more quickly.

Isolation rearing beginning in early adolescence (P21–P28) consistently decreases time spent in the open arm of an elevated plus maze in male rats (Parker, 1986; Wright et al., 1991; Weiss et al., 2004; McCool and Chappell, 2009; Chappell et al., 2013; Pritchard et al., 2013; Yorgason et al., 2013; Karkhanis et al., 2014; Skelly et al., 2015), an effect that cannot be reversed with resocialization (Wright et al., 1991). However, the timing of the stress may be critical, as males isolated on or after P30 display less anxiety-like behavior, as indicated by more time spent in the open arms of the maze (Thorsell et al., 2006; Weintraub et al., 2010). Interestingly, this coincides with the decline of play behavior in group-housed rats (Baenninger, 1967; Panksepp, 1981), and suggest that the anxiogenic effects of early adolescent isolation rearing may be due to the lack of play interactions in male rats. In females, however, isolation rearing has no effect on behavior in an elevated plus maze (Weiss et al., 2004; Weintraub et al., 2010; Jahng et al., 2012; Butler et al., 2014) regardless of whether the animals were resocialized following stress (Weintraub et al., 2010), suggesting that the effects of adolescent rearing and adolescent social isolation are more potent in male rodents.

Open Field Behavior

As with elevated plus maze, investigation into how isolation rearing affects open field behavior are relatively consistent (Table 1). During testing, an animal is allowed to explore a novel environment, usually a large plastic box. Animals in a novel environment often display an increase in locomotor behavior, interpreted as an effect of the stress of being in a new context, but typically habituate during the test. Animals thought to be more anxious tend not to habituate as quickly and will, therefore, display an increased exploration measured via increased distance within the open field and an increase in locomotor behavior. Determining the latency, or how long it takes an animal, to enter into the center of the arena is another measurement of anxiety-like behavior in an open field test. Generally, open spaces are unsafe for rodents, therefore, an animal is considered less anxious if they enter the center more readily (Figure 2B; Lezak et al., 2017). Again, caution should be used when interpreting results from this test, especially in the context of sex differences in behavior. Studies have found that females are more hyperactive and hence enter the center of an open field more readily than males. Therefore, the traditional interpretation of open field testing may not be an accurate measure of anxiety, especially in females.

The effects of isolation rearing on locomotor activity are inconsistent in both males and females (Table 1). Although the majority of studies have found that male rats reared in isolation are hyperactive in an open field test (Archer, 1969; Dalrymple-Alford and Benton, 1981; Gentsch et al., 1981; Heidbreder et al., 2000; Zhao et al., 2009; Skelly et al., 2015), an appreciable number found that isolation rearing has no effect on locomotor activity (Gardner et al., 1975; Weiss et al., 2004; Thorsell et al., 2006) and some have found that isolation rearing leads to hypoactivity (Holson et al., 1988, 1991). Similarly, resocialization has produced conflicting results with Wright et al. (1991)) showing an increase and Lukkes et al. (2009c) showing a decrease in locomotor behavior in males. In females, the inconsistencies are even more apparent. However, this is likely due to the fact that there are far fewer studies in females. The limited number of studies in female rats has shown that isolation rearing increases (Dalrymple-Alford and Benton, 1981; Jahng et al., 2012), decreases (Archer, 1969; Holson et al., 1991) or does not affect (Archer, 1969; Butler et al., 2014) locomotor behavior. It is unclear why social isolation has produced such inconsistent results, but differences in experimental design including length of isolation, lighting conditions during testing, and the open field apparatus used likely to contribute. As with other behavioral indicators of anxiety-like behavior, there appears to be a window in which isolation must occur for any effect to be observed. Archer (1969) found that isolation for 2 weeks (P28–P42) was not enough to affect anxiety-related behaviors in females (males not studied). However, after 24 weeks of isolation, a significant decrease in locomotor behavior was observed in females, whereas an increase in locomotor behavior was observed in males (Archer, 1969). This is consistent with multiple studies indicating that isolation must occur for at least 3 weeks for any behavioral effects to be observed (Whitaker et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2017).

As mentioned above, another measure of anxiety-like responses in an open field is the time it takes for an animal to enter the center of the apparatus and total time spent in the center during testing (Figure 2B). Once again, the effects of isolation rearing on this behavioral measure are inconsistent in males and females. In males, isolation in early adolescence (beginning on P21) does not affect the latency to enter the center of the apparatus (Dalrymple-Alford and Benton, 1981; Holson et al., 1988), but does decrease the number of times an animal will enter the center after resocialization (Lukkes et al., 2009a). However, isolation late in adolescence (P45–P130) results in male rats spending more time in the center of the field (Thorsell et al., 2006). Interestingly, these results mirror those observed in the elevated plus maze data discussed above and suggest that isolation in early adolescence is anxiogenic, while isolation later in adolescence is anxiolytic in male rats. To our knowledge, only one study has investigated this behavioral measure in females and found no effect on the latency to enter the center of the open field (Dalrymple-Alford and Benton, 1981), further highlighting the need for research into the effects of isolation rearing on females.

Other Anxiety/Fear-Related Behaviors

While it is beyond the scope of this review to cover all anxiety-related behaviors affected by adolescent isolation rearing, it should be noted that similar effects have been observed in fear conditioning, social interaction testing, pre-pulse inhibition, and startle response (For reviews see McCormick and Green, 2013; Burke et al., 2017). Of interest to this review are the studies which have found similar sex-specific effects of social isolation rearing on fear conditioning in rats. In this behavioral paradigm, animals are trained to associate a tone with a fear-inducing stimulus (usually a foot shock). Freezing in response to a stimulus is a behavioral marker of fear in rodents. Therefore, once an animal undergoes training, freezing behavior is observed after an animal is exposed to the paired tone within the conditioned context (McAllister, 1971). Social isolation from P21 to P42 resulted in males freezing longer in response to the conditioned tone on P77 (Lukkes et al., 2009a). Additionally, males isolated from P28 to P77 took longer to extinguish their response to the conditioned tone when compared to group-housed counterparts (Skelly et al., 2015). However, isolation reared females (P21–P112) froze less than their control counterparts when re-exposed to the conditioned context (Weiss et al., 2004). These results highlight once again that there are important sex differences in adolescent development that are disrupted by isolation rearing in sex-specific ways. Further research is necessary to not only characterize normative adolescent development of anxiety-related behaviors in males and females but also to understand how each sex might be affected by stress at key developmental time points.

Summary of Anxiety-Related Behaviors

Taken together, these data suggest that social isolation rearing has profound effects on anxiety-related behaviors, as best as they can be inferred, in male rodents and subtle, if any, effects in females (Figure 1B). Importantly, these effects are not alleviated by resocialization, suggesting that restricting social stimuli at a time when social reward is heightened results in a permanent change in exploratory and novelty-seeking behaviors. While there are inconsistencies in many of the behavioral endpoints, these differences are likely due to differences in experimental design and highlight the need for standardized stress protocols for investigating the long-term effects of adolescent social isolation on anxiety-related behaviors. Even with these inconsistencies, it seems clear that the timing of stress is important. Social isolation during early adolescence is anxiogenic in male rats, whereas isolation later in adolescence is anxiolytic. Further studies are necessary to determine the mechanisms underlying these effects and if these effects hold up across species. For example, do the anxiogenic effects hold up in species that engage in less adolescent play? Similarly, much more work is necessary to understand how adolescent social isolation affects female behavior. As it stands, the effects of social isolation rearing are more subtle in females than in males. However, given that many of the behavioral tests for anxiety were developed in males and do not seem to be indicative of anxiety-like behavior in females, it may be that new tests need to be developed for testing the effects of adolescent social isolation in females.

Addiction-Related Behaviors

SUDs—namely, drug addiction—are defined as the continued use of drugs of abuse despite negative consequences (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). It is a complex disorder characterized by cycles of behavior. In humans, a SUD begins with accelerated use after initial exposure to a drug of abuse. As the SUD becomes more severe, a person may enter a cycle of addiction that includes uncontrolled drug use followed by periods of attempted abstinence (withdrawal) and then all too often by a return to drug use (relapse; Koob and Simon, 2009). It is well established that early life adversity influences susceptibility to addiction in adulthood (Pena et al., 2014). While the initial mechanisms of action of drugs of abuse are numerous, they all converge on the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) circuitry to increase the release of DA in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) from a population of neurons within the VTA. As mentioned above, evidence suggests that this VTA—NAc circuit, as well as VTA innervation of other forebrain limbic regions, is developing during the adolescent period (Figure 1A; Walker et al., 2017) and that exposure to stressful stimuli can disrupt connectivity between these brain regions. Thus, adolescent stress may interfere with the normative development of the mesocorticolimbic system and impact the effects of drugs of abuse within the brain’s reward circuitry.

Conditioned Place Preference Testing

There is convincing evidence that isolation rearing influences an animal’s ability to form a preference for a drug-paired chamber in the CPP paradigm (Table 2). In CPP testing, an animal is trained to associate a context with a specific drug of abuse by exposing them to the drug or a vehicle control within different chambers of an CPP apparatus. These chambers differ in sensory modalities (e.g., color or pattern on the walls, types of floors, intensity of lighting, etc.). After training sessions, an animal is allowed access to both chambers of the apparatus and the time spent on the drug-paired side is determined. An animal who spends more time in the drug-paired chamber is said to have formed a preference for the drug. This preference is thought to be indicative of the reinforcing properties of a drug (Figure 2C; Belin, 2012).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Behavioral Effects of isolation rearing/adolescent social isolation on addiction-related behaviors conditioned place preference.

Studies investigating how isolation rearing influences CPP to drugs of abuse indicate that isolation rearing increases sensitivity to psychostimulants (Table 2), an effect that appears to be dose-dependent. This phenomenon is not observed for opiates, in fact, a reduction in sensitivity in response to isolation rearing has been reported. Specifically, isolation rearing increases the preference for amphetamine (Whitaker et al., 2013) and cocaine (Zakharova et al., 2009) when compared to the group housed controls at higher drug doses (5 and 10 mg/kg). However, this effect was not observed at lower doses of cocaine (0.31–2.5 mg/kg) or amphetamines (0.031–0.5 mg/kg; Schenk et al., 1986), or when isolation rearing commenced on P42 (Whitaker et al., 2013). On the other hand, while socially reared animals formed a strong preference for morphine at low (1 and 1.5 mg/kg) and high (5 mg/kg) doses, isolated animals did not (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1995, 1996), suggesting an interaction between social context during development and pharmacology. It should be noted that the number of studies is quite small and further research is necessary to determine if this finding is indeed specific to psychostimulant vs. opiate drugs of abuse. Additionally, there is a dearth of information regarding how isolation rearing or adolescent social stress in general influences drug reward in females (Figure 1C; Table 2).

Self-Administration

Investigations into the effects of social isolation rearing on drug reward have mainly focused on self-administration (SA; Table 2). In this behavioral test, animals are given the opportunity to administer drugs of abuse under their own volition. Often, animals are implanted with a jugular vein catheter and connected to a pump used to deliver drugs directly into the bloodstream after the animal has pressed a lever or nose-poked. Through operant conditioning, animals learn to discriminate between an “active” lever (drug infusion) and “inactive” lever (no consequence) with the drug serving to reinforce the lever-pressing behavior (Figure 2D; Belin, 2012). Generally speaking, isolation rearing increases the acquisition of SA (Marks-Kaufman and Lewis, 1984; Schenk et al., 1987; Phillips et al., 1994; Howes et al., 2000; Green et al., 2002; Baarendse et al., 2014), and increases intake of numerous drugs of abuse (Marks-Kaufman and Lewis, 1984; Schenk et al., 1987; Boyle et al., 1991; Bardo et al., 2001; Ding et al., 2005), with the difference most apparent at low doses (for specifics see Table 2). Importantly, this effect is not reversed by resocialization from P42 to P90, at least with regard to cocaine SA (Baarendse et al., 2014).

Cocaine SA, in particular, has been extensively used to study the effects of social isolation rearing on different aspects of drug-taking behaviors. Acquisition of SA is defined as the transition to a regular pattern of intake as indicated by an animal reliably pressing the drug-paired over the inactive lever. It is interpreted as a behavioral marker of a drug’s reinforcing properties. From a translational perspective, the rate of acquisition could provide insight into the progression of SUDs in humans. Variables that increase the rate of acquisition are considered to be risk factors for SUDs, while those that delay acquisition are considered to be protective against SUDs in humans (Belin, 2012). To our knowledge, studies on the effects of isolation rearing on cocaine SA behaviors in females have not been investigated (Table 2). However, studies in males reveal that isolation rearing increases the rate of acquisition of cocaine SA at low doses in males [between 0.083 and 1 mg/kg/infusion (inf)] with relative consistency. It is important to highlight that, in each of these studies, the group-housed control animals did not acquire, suggesting that low doses of cocaine are more reinforcing after isolation rearing (Schenk et al., 1987; Howes et al., 2000; Gipson et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2014). A more complicated picture emerges when the dose of cocaine is increased to the point where control animals also acquire cocaine SA. In these cases, if the group-housed control animals acquire SA, the rate of acquisition is delayed in isolated animals (Phillips et al., 1994; Howes et al., 2000) or not affected by housing condition (Howes et al., 2000; Gipson et al., 2011). It is tempting to interpret these data as an increase in sensitivity to cocaine in isolated animals. However, this is not supported by the few dose-response experiments that have been conducted. Phillips et al. (1994) showed that isolation rearing results in a rightward shift in the dose response to cocaine (0.023–1.5 mg/kg/inf), suggesting that isolated males were less sensitive to the reinforcing effects of cocaine. In support of this, a recent study (Baarendse et al., 2014), which included a period of resocialization from P42 to ~P90, showed that isolation specifically during the adolescent period had no effect on the dose-response curve in adult males (0.03–0.5 mg/kg/inf). These inconsistencies are likely due to differences in experimental design and reflect the high variability of complex behaviors in control populations. Additionally, these results highlight the need for standardized protocols for SA.

As with acquisition, lever pressing and intake are often interpreted as a behavioral marker of the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse. Generally speaking, isolation rearing in males increases the intake of cocaine (Schenk et al., 1987; Boyle et al., 1991; Ding et al., 2005), amphetamine (Bardo et al., 2001), and morphine (Marks-Kaufman and Lewis, 1984), again suggesting that isolated animals may display differences in the sensitivity to drugs of abuse. Most of these effects have been observed in cocaine SA where similar to acquisition, the effects are dose dependent and must be interpreted in relation to group-housed controls in each experiment. At low doses (0.08–0.32 mg/kg/inj), isolated animals show no differences in intake when compared to group-housed control animals (Schenk et al., 1987; Boyle et al., 1991). At higher doses (0.5 and 1 mg/kg/inj) isolated animals take more cocaine when compared to control animals (Schenk et al., 1987; Boyle et al., 1991; Ding et al., 2005; Baarendse et al., 2014). Similar to acquisition, these results must be interpreted in the context of the group-housed control behaviors. When the control animals show a preference for cocaine over saline (Schenk et al., 1987; Boyle et al., 1991; Ding et al., 2005; Baarendse et al., 2014), there is no difference in intake between isolated and group-housed animals. On the other hand, when there is no difference in intake between saline and cocaine in the control group, isolated animals tend to lever press more than their control counterparts (Schenk et al., 1987; Boyle et al., 1991; Ding et al., 2005; Baarendse et al., 2014), suggesting that there are substantial differences in reward sensitivity across the groups. Importantly, to our knowledge, no study has found that group-housed controls consume more than isolated animals for any drug of abuse (Table 2).

Upon first glance, it might seem that isolation rearing increases sensitivity to the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse. However, these results should be interpreted with caution. First, isolation rearing has been reported to increase not only active but inactive lever pressing (Boyle et al., 1991; Ding et al., 2005; Baarendse et al., 2014) and isolated animals will lever press more for saline (Schenk et al., 1987). Therefore, increases in lever pressing/intake may be driven by isolation rearing effects on activity levels, especially at low drug doses (see “Anxiety-Related Behaviors” section; Table 1). This could explain the dose-response effects observed in acquisition as well. At low doses, hyperactive animals may be exposed to drug as a function of more interactions with the levers, whereas at higher doses when a drug serves as a stronger reinforcer both group housed and isolated animals acquire cocaine SA. In support of this, there is a body of evidence that suggests that isolated animals are more sensitive to the locomotor effects of psychostimulants (reviewed in Fone and Porkess, 2008). Thus, a feedforward effect would be predicted, as increased locomotor response would increase interactions with the levers and increase exposure to drug. Regardless of the mechanism driving this finding, it is noteworthy that attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity is a risk factor for SUDs (Wilens, 2004). Thus, isolation rearing may provide a translational model for understanding the behavioral, neural, and cellular/molecular mechanisms underlying susceptibility to SUDs.

While understudied, other behavioral measures may provide more information regarding how isolation rearing influences the progression of addiction-like behaviors in animal models. Escalation is considered a model of the transition from recreational to compulsive drug taking (Belin, 2012). Under short access conditions (~2 h sessions), an animal controls its intake to maintain a stable blood level throughout the session. However, when moved to long-access (~6 h) conditions, animals escalate their intake, especially in the first hour, and no longer defend their preferred plasma drug concentrations (Ahmed and Koob, 1998). Gipson et al. (2011) investigated escalation at two different doses in male rats isolated from P21 to P55 and found that isolated, but not group housed, animals escalated their intake of cocaine at 0.1 mg/kg/inf after being switched to long-access. However, at a higher dose (0.5 mg/kg/inf), group-housed males escalated their intake, an effect not observed in the isolated animals. In this experiment, isolated animals received more injections on the first day of long-access testing. This level of intake was similar to the control animals on the final day of testing, suggesting that isolated animals may have reached a “ceiling” in the number of responses and that isolation results in a loss of control in drug consumption (Gipson et al., 2011). It should be highlighted that this is only one study and further investigation is necessary to determine how social stress in adolescent males and females influences escalation of consumption of numerous drugs of abuse.

Finally, motivation to gain access to a drug can be assessed by changing the criteria for reward during an SA session. In a progressive ratio scheduling paradigm, increasing response ratios are used to determine when an animal will no longer “work” for the reinforcer, termed the “break-point.” A higher break-point is interpreted as an increased motivation for reward (Roberts et al., 1989; Richardson and Roberts, 1996). To our knowledge, only one study has investigated how adolescent isolation affects motivation for cocaine reward in this paradigm. Baarendse et al. (2014) found that males isolated for 3 weeks (P21–P42) followed by several weeks of resocialization had a higher breakpoint under a progressive ratio schedule for cocaine at both low (0.083 mg/inf) and high (0.25 mg/inf) doses when compared to their group housed control counterparts.

Summary of Addiction-Related Behaviors

Taken together, these data suggest that isolation rearing affects susceptibility to drug addiction through two mechanisms: (1) both CPP and SA data suggest that reward sensitivity is altered by isolation rearing; and (2) hyperactivity induced by isolation rearing may interact with a sensitized reward circuity to induce susceptibility to addiction later in life. Further research is necessary to determine if similar pharmacological effects are observed in SA paradigms as those observed in CPP paradigms between psychostimulants vs.opiates (and other classes of drugs of abuse) and if social isolation rearing in females results in similar behavioral effects to those observed in males.

Social Isolation and Disruption of Monoamine Systems

Monoamines are a class of neurotransmitters that are derived from aromatic amino acids. Generally speaking, these molecules modulate the excitatory and inhibitory effects of glutamate and GABA, respectively. Monoamines including serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine or 5-HT), DA, norepinephrine, and epinephrine, are important signaling molecules that influence higher cognitive function, reward integration, and arousal (Lipton, 1974). While all monoamines play a role in anxiety and addiction, 5-HT and DA have been implicated as crucial to stress and reward processing. Specifically, increased 5-HT function in the brain underlies therapeutic responses to most classes of antidepressant medications. While it is hypothesized that susceptibility to anxiety and depression in humans may relate to differences in 5-HT function (Blier, 2016), considerable evidence shows no such relationship (Nestler et al., 2002). On the other hand, DA has been extensively implicated in reward processing and it is hypothesized that dysregulation of the DA system is an important component of SUDs (Nutt et al., 2015). Accumulating evidence suggests that these two monoaminergic systems are crucial for the environmental influence on behavior in adolescence (Burke et al., 2017) and both are important for CNS development (Sodhi and Sanders-Bush, 2004). Given their roles in regulating emotion and stress responses, and their importance in CNS development, it is not surprising that many studies have focused on the effects of isolation rearing on 5-HT and DA signaling dynamics.

Social Isolation and Dysregulation of Serotonin Signaling

5-HT neurons are concentrated in the midbrain raphe nuclei and such neurons in the dorsal raphe project throughout the brain, including mesocorticolimbic regions essential for reward processing (Sodhi and Sanders-Bush, 2004). There is an interplay between 5-HT and DA signaling which appears to influence anxiety- and reward-related behaviors (Blier, 2016). In rodents, the vast majority of serotonergic development occurs from embryonic day 14–17, suggesting that the 5-HT system is in place prior to adolescent development. However, one study found that isolation rearing increased 5-HT fiber density in the dorsal striatum and amygdala in gerbils (Lehmann et al., 2003), suggesting that 5-HT development might continue into adolescence or that there are important species differences in the developmental trajectory of the 5-HT system. Additionally, 5-HT exerts growth factor-like effects and has been shown to influence the development of other neurotransmitter systems (Sodhi and Sanders-Bush, 2004). This evidence suggests that 5-HT signaling during adolescence may be crucial for the proper development of the adolescent brain and adult behavior. Indeed, recent evidence suggests that altering serotonergic signaling, specifically during adolescence, increases anxiety-like behavior in adulthood (Ansorge et al., 2007; Donaldson et al., 2014). The importance of 5-HT in brain development coupled with its influence on anxiety-related behaviors suggest it as a key regulator of the long-term effects of isolation rearing.

Studies investigating the effects of isolation rearing on 5-HT signaling have found brain region-specific effects on basal 5-HT release and turnover (Figure 3A). In the NAc, a region important for the integration of reward signaling, isolation rearing has no effect on basal extracellular 5-HT levels (Howes et al., 2000), but decreases basal turnover of 5-HT (Heidbreder et al., 2000), suggesting that isolation rearing alters 5-HT dynamics within the NAc. Additionally, exposure to an aversive stimulus (foot shock; Fulford and Marsden, 1998a), but not a rewarding stimulus (cocaine; Howes et al., 2000), increases the release of 5-HT into the NAc after isolation rearing. In the PFC, a critical region of executive function and decision making, a similar effect to NAc is observed on basal 5-HT; isolation rearing has no effect on basal extracellular 5-HT levels but decreases its metabolism, as indicated by decreased 5-HIAA levels (Holson et al., 1991). This finding suggests once again that 5-HT dynamics, including reuptake, in this brain region may be affected by social isolation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A summary of how adolescent social isolation rearing alters serotonin (top) and DA (bottom) dynamics within the reward circuitry of rodents. Social isolation rearing has been shown to alter metabolism (red) and release (orange and yellow) of serotonin in the PFC, NAc and HIP in a region-specific manner. Similarly, social isolation rearing has been shown to alter baseline DA (pink) as well as metabolism (red), release (orange and yellow) and reuptake through actions on DAT (green) in a region-specific manner. Abbreviations: PFC, prefrontal cortex; NAc, nucleus accumbens; BLA, basolateral amygdala; HIP, hippocampus; NE, no effect; DAT, dopamine transporter.

Region-specific differences emerge when investigating stimuli-induced 5-HT release (Figure 3A). In the PFC, aversive (footshock; Lapiz et al., 2003) and rewarding (amphetamine and para-chloroamphetamine; Dalley et al., 2002) stimuli attenuate 5-HT release compared to group-housed controls. In the hippocampus, little is known regarding how isolation rearing affects basal 5-HT levels. However, para-chloroamphetamine-induced 5-HT release is decreased after isolation rearing (Muchimapura et al., 2002). Further investigation within the hippocampus revealed that 5-HT reuptake was not affected by isolation rearing, suggesting that 5-HT transporter (SERT) function is unaffected (Muchimapura et al., 2002). These data suggest that isolation rearing alters both basal and stimulus-induced 5-HT signaling in a region-specific manner. Importantly, these region-specific differences may be due, in part, to differences in receptor sensitivity across the brain. Numerous studies have shown that total binding and affinity of multiple 5-HT receptor subtypes are altered by isolation rearing (reviewed in Fone and Porkess, 2008). The reported effects of isolation rearing on the serotonergic system have important implications for the long-term behavioral consequences of adolescent stress. Given the importance of 5-HT in controlling anxiety- and depression-related behaviors, these data provide a possible mechanism for the effects of isolation rearing on anxiety discussed above.

Social Isolation and Dysregulation of Dopamine Signaling

As stated earlier, the mesolimbic DA system is characterized by a collection of DA-releasing neurons within the VTA that project to the dorsal striatum, NAc, PFC, HIP, and basolateral amygdala (BLA), among other regions. There is substantial evidence that VTA DA neuronal projections undergo vast developmental changes during adolescence, which can be disrupted by exposure to drugs of abuse and stress in rodents (Walker et al., 2017). Specifically, in male mice, DA axonal projections grow through the NAc to establish connections within the PFC. Disruption of this process results in excessive axonal growth to the PFC and decreased connectivity between the VTA and NAc (Reynolds et al., 2018). Normative development of this system is crucial for proper function for reward learning. Thus, disruption of these connections by stress during adolescence can have long-term behavioral consequences regarding motivation and reward. This work highlights the sensitivity of DA circuitry to environmental stimuli during adolescence. However, to our knowledge, no study has investigated how isolation rearing affects DA neuronal connectivity in adults. Instead, most studies have focused on how isolation rearing alters DA release into important reward-associated regions in adulthood.

Similar to the 5-HT system, isolation rearing alters DA release in a region-specific manner (Figure 3B). In male rats, isolation rearing has no effect on extracellular DA levels in the NAc (Jones, 1992; Fulford and Marsden, 1998b; Hall et al., 1998; Howes et al., 2000; Karkhanis et al., 2014). However, basal DA turnover is increased (Hall et al., 1998), suggesting that DA dynamics including release and uptake may be altered after isolation rearing. Additionally, electrical stimulation (Yorgason et al., 2013), footshock (Fulford and Marsden, 1998b; Lapiz et al., 2003), amphetamine (Hall et al., 1998; Lapiz et al., 2003; Yorgason et al., 2016), EtOH (Karkhanis et al., 2014), and cocaine (Yorgason et al., 2016) all increase DA release into the NAc in isolated animals when compared to group-housed controls. Importantly, DA depletion in the NAc can reverse the observed behavioral phenotypes following isolation rearing in male rats (Powell et al., 2003). These results suggest that isolation rearing modulates DA signaling within the NAc that may have an important impact on behavior. In the PFC, basal DA is unchanged by isolation rearing (Holson et al., 1991; Dalley et al., 2002; Powell et al., 2003), however, turnover is decreased (Heidbreder et al., 2000). In the BLA, a reward-associated region implicated in reward valuation, isolation rearing reduces basal DA concentrations (Karkhanis et al., 2015), which are coupled with increased DA turnover (Heidbreder et al., 2000). Interestingly, these changes in basal DA concentrations may have an effect on electrophysiological properties within the amygdala, as social isolation rearing has been shown to decrease cellular activity within the extended amygdala (Adams and Rosenkranz, 2016). Further functional studies are necessary to determine how social isolation rearing might affect cellular physiology throughout the reward circuitry. Additionally, unlike the 5-HT system, isolation rearing does not affect DA receptor sensitivity or expression. To our knowledge, no study has investigated how isolation rearing affects expression or activity of the DA receptor-1 (Drd1). However, several studies have found that isolation rearing has no effect on Drd2 mRNA, protein, or activity levels in the NAc, PFC, HIPP, or AMY of male rats (Del Arco et al., 2004; Malone et al., 2008; Yorgason et al., 2013). These findings may be species-specific, as a recent study in male mice found that Drd2 mRNA and protein levels are increased in the NAc, but not PFC or HIPP, after social isolation rearing (Li et al., 2017).

Unlike the 5-HT system, these region-specific differences may be due in part to differences in DA transporter (DAT) function. DAT is a membrane-spanning protein that pumps DA from the synaptic cleft back into the cytosol of DA neurons. DAT is the immediate protein target for all psychostimulant drugs of abuse, and its function reportedly influences clinical depression and alcoholism (Salatino-Oliveira et al., 2018). Studies have found that isolation rearing increases baseline DAT protein levels in the dorsal striatum, NAc, and BLA when compared to group-housed controls (Karkhanis et al., 2015; Yorgason et al., 2016). Additionally, DAT protein levels are induced in the NAc in response to three daily injections of cocaine in isolated males. This effect was not observed in animals reared in an enriched environment (Zakharova et al., 2009). Altered DAT protein levels may impact the rate at which DA is removed from the synapse. Studies investigating DAT activity have focused on changes within the NAc. Yorgason et al. (2013) demonstrated that DA uptake was enhanced in isolation reared male rats at baseline. In a follow-up study, this effect was further enhanced by administration of psychostimulants including, methylphenidate, a stimulant used to treat ADHD, in the NAc in isolated male rats (Yorgason et al., 2016). Overall, these data suggest that the region-specific differences seen in DA signaling may be due, in part, to DAT function. However, further research is required to understand how DAT function is altered in regions other than the NAc.

Summary of Effects of Isolation on Monoamines

To date, studies have found that isolation rearing changes both 5HT and DA signaling. Further research is necessary to fully integrate these neurotransmitter effects with the behavioral consequences of isolation rearing and future studies must include more research on the monoaminergic effects of isolation rearing on females to develop a complete understanding the dysregulation of these systems. Finally, regulation of the brain’s monoaminergic systems must be integrated with the regulation of numerous other classes of neurotransmitters, neurotrophic factors, and cytokines that together control brain development, structure, and function.

Summary and Conclusions

We are only beginning to understand the complex interplay of neural networks, endocrine signaling, and environmental inputs that drive normative adolescent development. These complicated interactions are a highly coordinated process that results in specific adult-typical behaviors which are often directed at increasing the likelihood of reproduction and offspring survival. Therefore, stressful events during this sensitive window of development would be predicted to have long-term behavioral consequences. This review has highlighted how disruptions in social structure during adolescence alters anxiety- and reward-associated behaviors and points to a few emerging themes throughout. First, the timing of social isolation stress results in different behavioral outcomes and suggests that disruption of play behavior during early adolescence may have profound effects on male anxiety in adulthood (Figure 1B). Second, isolation rearing in males increases susceptibility to addiction-related behaviors (Figure 1C) and this may be the result, in part, of interactions of the effects of isolation on anxiety and locomotor behaviors. Third, far more research is needed in females to assess how adolescent stress may influence adult behaviors. Fourth, changes in serotonergic and dopaminergic systems may contribute to the behavioral consequences of isolation rearing, although research is needed into the contributions of many other signaling mechanisms. Fifth, many studies have compared isolation rearing to different paradigms of environmental enrichment, and it would be important to understand how the latter influences adolescent development as well. Finally, while it is clear that adolescence is critical for expression of adult behaviors, the field would benefit greatly from further research into the development of the adolescent brain and circuitry as well as the mechanisms by which neural, endocrine, and environmental information is coordinated to produce a functional system. These investigations would be instrumental for better understanding of the psychiatric disorders that emerge during adolescence, thus leading to novel therapeutics and interventions in vulnerable populations.

Together, the data highlight three areas of focus for the field that would greatly enhance our understanding of the interplay between adolescent social experience and psychiatric disorders in adulthood. First, the field would benefit greatly from a clear description of normative adolescent brain development across several rodent and primate species. This would provide valuable information regarding sensitive periods when regions, circuitry and molecules might be disrupted due to adolescent isolation rearing. Most studies utilizing social isolation to date have subjected rodents to isolation throughout the adolescent period and it is apparent that the timing of isolation is crucial for the behavioral outcomes. Second, the field is greatly lacking in studies investigating how females respond to adolescent isolation rearing. Given the vast sex differences in adolescent development and stress responses in general, it is likely that there are sex-specific effects of isolation rearing and these must be incorporated into future studies. This work could also be leveraged to understand how differences in the timing of developmental milestones (e.g., puberty) might influence the long-term behavioral effects of adolescence social stress. Finally, the field is lacking in mechanistic studies indicating how adolescent experience reshapes molecular, synaptic and circuit processes throughout the brain to influence future behavioral responses. Given the importance of adolescence as a key period for the reprogramming of the reward-circuitry and enhanced vulnerability for the development of psychiatric disorders, further studies investigating the mechanism driving such programming would provide valuable insight into the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying psychiatric disorders.

Author Contributions

DW, AC and EN all wrote and commented on the manuscript. JG commented on the manuscript and made all of the figures.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Mental Health [NIMH—P50MH096890 (EN), National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA)—P01DA008227 (EN)] and a NIDA K99 Pathway to Independence Award [DA042100 (DW)].

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adams, T., and Rosenkranz, J. A. (2016). Social isolation during postweaning development causes hypoactivity of neurons in the medial nucleus of the male rat amygdala. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 1929–1940. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, S. H., and Koob, G. F. (1998). Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: change in hedonic set point. Science 282, 298–300. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5387.298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansorge, M. S., Hen, R., and Gingrich, J. A. (2007). Neurodevelopmental origins of depressive disorders. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 7, 8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2006.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arakawa, H. (2018). Ethological approach to social isolation effects in behavioral studies of laboratory rodents. Behav. Brain Res. 341, 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J. (1969). Contrasting effects of group housing and isolation on subsequent open field exploration in laboratory rats. Psychonomic. Sci. 14, 234–235. doi: 10.3758/BF03332812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Edn. (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing).

Google Scholar

Baarendse, P. J., Limpens, J. H., and Vanderschuren, L. J. (2014). Disrupted social development enhances the motivation for cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology 231, 1695–1704. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3362-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baenninger, L. P. (1967). Comparison of behavioural development in socially isolated and grouped rats. Anim. Behav. 15, 312–323. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(67)90018-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagot, R. C., Labonte, B., Pena, C. J., and Nestler, E. J. (2014). Epigenetic signaling in psychiatric disorders: stress and depression. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 16, 281–295.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bagot, R. C., Parise, E. M., Pena, C. J., Zhang, H. X., Maze, I., Chaudhury, D., et al. (2015). Ventral hippocampal afferents to the nucleus accumbens regulate susceptibility to depression. Nat. Commun. 6:7062. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardo, M. T., Klebaur, J. E., Valone, J. M., and Deaton, C. (2001). Environmental enrichment decreases intravenous self-administration of amphetamine in female and male rats. Psychopharmacology 155, 278–284. doi: 10.1007/s002130100720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, AB-RaD. (2012). “Animal Models of Drug Addiction,” in Addictions from Pathophysiology to Treatment, ed. D. Belin (London, UK: IntechOpen Limited), 21–64.

Blier, P. (2016). Neurobiology of depression and mechanism of action of depression treatments. J. Clin. Psychiatry 77:e319. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13097tx3c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyle, A. E., Gill, K., Smith, B. R., and Amit, Z. (1991). Differential effects of an early housing manipulation on cocaine-induced activity and self-administration in laboratory rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 39, 269–274. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90178-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, A. R., McCormick, C. M., Pellis, S. M., and Lukkes, J. L. (2017). Impact of adolescent social experiences on behavior and neural circuits implicated in mental illnesses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 76, 280–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, T. R., Carter, E., and Weiner, J. L. (2014). Adolescent social isolation does not lead to persistent increases in anxiety- like behavior or ethanol intake in female long-evans rats. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 2199–2207. doi: 10.1111/acer.12476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., and Jones, R. M. (2010). Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 1189–1201; quiz 1285. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chappell, A. M., Carter, E., McCool, B. A., and Weiner, J. L. (2013). Adolescent rearing conditions influence the relationship between initial anxiety-like behavior and ethanol drinking in male Long Evans rats. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 37 Suppl 1, E394–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01926.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., and Baram, T. Z. (2016). Toward understanding how early-life stress reprograms cognitive and emotional brain networks. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 197–206. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corre, C., Shinoda, G., Zhu, H., Cousminer, D. L., Crossman, C., Bellissimo, C., et al. (2016). Sex-specific regulation of weight and puberty by the Lin28/let-7 axis. J. Endocrinol. 228, 179–191. doi: 10.1530/JOE-15-0360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalley, J. W., Theobald, D. E., Pereira, E. A., Li, P. M., and Robbins, T. W. (2002). Specific abnormalities in serotonin release in the prefrontal cortex of isolation-reared rats measured during behavioural performance of a task assessing visuospatial attention and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology 164, 329–340. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1215-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalrymple-Alford, J. C., and Benton, D. (1981). Activity differences of individually and group-housed male and female rats. Anim. Learn Behav. 9, 50–55. doi: 10.3758/bf03212025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Arco, A., Zhu, S., Terasmaa, A., Mohammed, A. H., and Fuxe, K. (2004). Hyperactivity to novelty induced by social isolation is not correlated with changes in D2 receptor function and binding in striatum. Psychopharmacology 171, 148–155. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1578-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, Y., Kang, L., Li, B., and Ma, L. (2005). Enhanced cocaine self-administration in adult rats with adolescent isolation experience. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 82, 673–677. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donaldson, Z. R., Piel, D. A., Santos, T. L., Richardson-Jones, J., Leonardo, E. D., Beck, S. G., et al. (2014). Developmental effects of serotonin 1A autoreceptors on anxiety and social behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 291–302. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, L. A., Varlinskaya, E. I., and Spear, L. P. (2004). Rewarding properties of social interactions in adolescent and adult male and female rats: impact of social versus isolate housing of subjects and partners. Dev. Psychobiol. 45, 153–162. doi: 10.1002/dev.20025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dziedzic, N., Ho, A., Adabi, B., Foilb, A. R., and Romeo, R. D. (2014). Shifts in hormonal stress reactivity during adolescence are not associated with changes in glucocorticoid receptor levels in the brain and pituitary of male rats. Dev. Neurosci. 36, 261–268. doi: 10.1159/000362873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eiland, L., and Romeo, R. D. (2013). Stress and the developing adolescent brain. Neuroscience 249, 162–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.10.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einon, D. F., and Morgan, M. J. (1977). A critical period for social isolation in the rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 10, 123–132. doi: 10.1002/dev.420100205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix-Ortiz, A. C., Beyeler, A., Seo, C., Leppla, C. A., Wildes, C. P., and Tye, K. M. (2013). BLA to vHPC inputs modulate anxiety-related behaviors. Neuron 79, 658–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix-Ortiz, A. C., Burgos-Robles, A., Bhagat, N. D., Leppla, C. A., and Tye, K. M. (2016). Bidirectional modulation of anxiety-related and social behaviors by amygdala projections to the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 321, 197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix-Ortiz, A. C., and Tye, K. M. (2014). Amygdala inputs to the ventral hippocampus bidirectionally modulate social behavior. J. Neurosci. 34, 586–595. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4257-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, C., Gonzalez, M. I., Wilson, C. A., and File, S. E. (1999). Factor analysis shows that female rat behaviour is characterized primarily by activity, male rats are driven by sex and anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 64, 731–738. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00139-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fone, K. C., and Porkess, M. V. (2008). Behavioural and neurochemical effects of post-weaning social isolation in rodents-relevance to developmental neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1087–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fulford, A. J., and Marsden, C. A. (1998a). Conditioned release of 5-hydroxytryptamine in vivo in the nucleus accumbens following isolation-rearing in the rat. Neuroscience 83, 481–487. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00423-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fulford, A. J., and Marsden, C. A. (1998b). Effect of isolation-rearing on conditioned dopamine release in vivo in the nucleus accumbens of the rat. J. Neurochem. 70, 384–390. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1998.70010384.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, E. B., Boitano, J. J., Mancino, N. S., and D’Amico, D. P. (1975). Environmental enrichment and deprivation: effects on learning, memory and exploration. Physiol. Behav. 14, 321–327. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(75)90040-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., and Feer, H. (1981). Locomotor activity, defecation score and corticosterone levels during an openfield exposure: a comparison among individually and group-housed rats and genetically selected rat lines. Physiol. Behav. 27, 183–186. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90320-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., and Rapoport, J. L. (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron 67, 728–734. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gipson, C. D., Beckmann, J. S., El-Maraghi, S., Marusich, J. A., and Bardo, M. T. (2011). Effect of environmental enrichment on escalation of cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology 214, 557–566. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2060-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gopnik, A., O’Grady, S., Lucas, C. G., Griffiths, T. L., Wente, A., Bridgers, S., et al. (2017). Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700811114 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, B. F. (1997). Prevalence and correlates of alcohol use and DSM-IV alcohol dependence in the United States: results of the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J. Stud. Alcohol 58, 464–473. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, B. F., and Dawson, D. A. (1998). Age of onset of drug use and its association with DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence: results from the national longitudinal alcohol epidemiologic survey. J. Subst. Abuse 10, 163–173. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(99)80131-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, T. A., Gehrke, B. J., and Bardo, M. T. (2002). Environmental enrichment decreases intravenous amphetamine self-administration in rats: dose-response functions for fixed- and progressive-ratio schedules. Psychopharmacology 162, 373–378. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1134-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnar, M. R., Talge, N. M., and Herrera, A. (2009). Stressor paradigms in developmental studies: what does and does not work to produce mean increases in salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 953–967. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, F. S., Wilkinson, L. S., Humby, T., Inglis, W., Kendall, D. A., Marsden, C. A., et al. (1998). Isolation rearing in rats: pre- and postsynaptic changes in striatal dopaminergic systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 59, 859–872. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00510-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., and Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 107, 128–140. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.107.1.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heidbreder, C. A., Weiss, I. C., Domeney, A. M., Pryce, C., Homberg, J., Hedou, G., et al. (2000). Behavioral, neurochemical and endocrinological characterization of the early social isolation syndrome. Neuroscience 100, 749–768. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00336-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holson, R. R., Scallet, A. C., Ali, S. F., Sullivan, P., and Gough, B. (1988). Adrenocortical, beta-endorphin and behavioral responses to graded stressors in differentially reared rats. Physiol. Behav. 42, 125–130. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90286-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holson, R. R., Scallet, A. C., Ali, S. F., and Turner, B. B. (1991). "Isolation stress" revisited: isolation-rearing effects depend on animal care methods. Physiol. Behav. 49, 1107–1118. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90338-o

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howes, S. R., Dalley, J. W., Morrison, C. H., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2000). Leftward shift in the acquisition of cocaine self-administration in isolation-reared rats: relationship to extracellular levels of dopamine, serotonin and glutamate in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala-striatal FOS expression. Psychopharmacology 151, 55–63. doi: 10.1007/s002130000451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jahng, J. W., Yoo, S. B., Ryu, V., and Lee, J. H. (2012). Hyperphagia and depression-like behavior by adolescence social isolation in female rats. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 30, 47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G. H. (1992). Social isolation and individual differences: behavioural and dopaminergic responses to psychomotor stimulants. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 15 Suppl 1, 253A–254A. doi: 10.1097/00002826-199201001-00132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karkhanis, A. N., Alexander, N. J., McCool, B. A., Weiner, J. L., and Jones, S. R. (2015). Chronic social isolation during adolescence augments catecholamine response to acute ethanol in the basolateral amygdala. Synapse 69, 385–395. doi: 10.1002/syn.21826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karkhanis, A. N., Locke, J. L., McCool, B. A., Weiner, J. L., and Jones, S. R. (2014). Social isolation rearing increases nucleus accumbens dopamine and norepinephrine responses to acute ethanol in adulthood. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 2770–2779. doi: 10.1111/acer.12555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenigs, M., and Grafman, J. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of depression: distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 201, 239–243. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Simon, E. J. (2009). The neurobiology of addiction: where we have been and where we are going. J. Drug Issues 39, 115–132. doi: 10.1177/002204260903900110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krewson, T. D., Supelak, P. J., Hill, A. E., Singer, J. B., Lander, E. S., Nadeau, J. H., et al. (2004). Chromosomes 6 and 13 harbor genes that regulate pubertal timing in mouse chromosome substitution strains. Endocrinology 145, 4447–4451. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-0543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladd, G. W., Ettekal, I., Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., Rudolph, K. D., and Andrews, R. K. (2014). Relations among chronic peer group rejection, maladaptive behavioral dispositions and early adolescents’ peer perceptions. Child Dev. 85, 971–988. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapiz, M. D., Fulford, A., Muchimapura, S., Mason, R., Parker, T., and Marsden, C. A. (2003). Influence of postweaning social isolation in the rat on brain development, conditioned behavior and neurotransmission. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 33, 13–29.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Larson, R. W., Richards, M. H., Moneta, G. B., and Holmbeck, G. (1996). Changes in adolescents’ daily interactions with their families from ages 10 to 18, disengagement and transformation. Dev. Psychol. 32, 744–754. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.4.744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawson Handley, L. J., and Perrin, N. (2007). Advances in our understanding of mammalian sex-biased dispersal. Mol. Ecol. 16, 1559–1578. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294x.2006.03152.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, E. H., Hsu, W. L., Ma, Y. L., Lee, P. J., and Chao, C. C. (2003). Enrichment enhances the expression of sgk, a glucocorticoid-induced gene and facilitates spatial learning through glutamate AMPA receptor mediation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 2842–2852. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03032.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, K., Lesting, J., Polascheck, D., and Teuchert-Noodt, G. (2003). Serotonin fibre densities in subcortical areas: differential effects of isolated rearing and methamphetamine. Brain Res. Dev. 147, 143–152. doi: 10.1016/s0165-3806(03)00130-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, K. R., Missig, G., and Carlezon, W. A. Jr. (2017). Behavioral methods to study anxiety in rodents. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 19, 181–191.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Li, B. J., Liu, P., Chu, Z., Shang, Y., Huan, M. X., Dang, Y. H., et al. (2017). Social isolation induces schizophrenia-like behavior potentially associated with HINT1, NMDA receptor 1 and dopamine receptor 2. Neuroreport 28, 462–469. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipari, R., and Jean-Francois, B. (2013). “Trends in Perception of Risk and Availability of Substance Use Among Full-Time College Students,” in The CBHSQ Report, Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (US); 2013–2016 Aug 16.

Google Scholar

Lipton, M. A. (1974). Neuropsychopharmacology of monoamines and their regulatory enzymes. Adv. Biochem. Psychopharmacol. 12, 443–454.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lui, P., Padow, V. A., Franco, D., Hall, B. S., Park, B., Klein, Z. A., et al. (2012). Divergent stress-induced neuroendocrine and behavioral responses prior to puberty. Physiol. Behav. 107, 104–111. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukkes, J. L., Mokin, M. V., Scholl, J. L., and Forster, G. L. (2009a). Adult rats exposed to early-life social isolation exhibit increased anxiety and conditioned fear behavior and altered hormonal stress responses. Horm. Behav. 55, 248–256. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukkes, J. L., Summers, C. H., Scholl, J. L., Renner, K. J., and Forster, G. L. (2009c). Early life social isolation alters corticotropin-releasing factor responses in adult rats. Neuroscience 158, 845–855. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.10.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukkes, J. L., Watt, M. J., Lowry, C. A., and Forster, G. L. (2009b). Consequences of post-weaning social isolation on anxiety behavior and related neural circuits in rodents. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 3:18. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.018.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, D. T., Kearn, C. S., Chongue, L., Mackie, K., and Taylor, D. A. (2008). Effect of social isolation on CB1 and D2 receptor and fatty acid amide hydrolase expression in rats. Neuroscience 152, 265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.10.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks-Kaufman, R., and Lewis, M. J. (1984). Early housing experience modifies morphine self-administration and physical dependence in adult rats. Addict. Behav. 9, 235–243. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(84)90015-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAllister, W. MaD. (1971). “Behavioral measurement of conditioned fear,” in Aversive Conditioning and Learning, ed. F. Brush (New York, NY: Academic Press), 105–179.

McCool, B. A., and Chappell, A. M. (2009). Early social isolation in male Long-Evans rats alters both appetitive and consummatory behaviors expressed during operant ethanol self-administration. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 273–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00830.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., and Green, M. R. (2013). From the stressed adolescent to the anxious and depressed adult: investigations in rodent models. Neuroscience 249, 242–257. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.08.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Green, M. R., Cameron, N. M., Nixon, F., Levy, M. J., and Clark, R. A. (2013b). Deficits in male sexual behavior in adulthood after social instability stress in adolescence in rats. Horm. Behav. 63, 5–12. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Mongillo, D. L., and Simone, J. J. (2013a). Age and adolescent social stress effects on fear extinction in female rats. Stress 16, 678–688. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2013.840283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muchimapura, S., Fulford, A. J., Mason, R., and Marsden, C. A. (2002). Isolation rearing in the rat disrupts the hippocampal response to stress. Neuroscience 112, 697–705. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00107-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J. F., Karelus, K., Felicio, L. S., and Johnson, T. E. (1990). Genetic influences on the timing of puberty in mice. Biol. Reprod. 42, 649–655.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J., Barrot, M., DiLeone, R. J., Eisch, A. J., Gold, S. J., and Monteggia, L. M. (2002). Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 34, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00653-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nutt, D. J., Lingford-Hughes, A., Erritzoe, D., and Stokes, P. R. (2015). The dopamine theory of addiction: 40 years of highs and lows. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 305–312. doi: 10.1038/nrn3939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1981). The ontogeny of play in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 14, 327–332. doi: 10.1002/dev.420140405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parent, A. S., Rasier, G., Gerard, A., Heger, S., Roth, C., Mastronardi, C., et al. (2005). Early onset of puberty: tracking genetic and environmental factors. Horm. Res. 64, 41–47. doi: 10.1159/000087753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, V. M. A. (1986). The socially isolated rat as a model of anxiety. Neuropharmacology 25, 663–664. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(86)90224-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, T. L., Smith, L. W., Smith, T. L., Yager, J., and Grant, I. (1992). Symptoms of illness in late adulthood are related to childhood social deprivation and misfortune in men but not in women. J. Behav. Med. 15, 113–125. doi: 10.1007/bf00848320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellis, S. M., and Pellis, V. C. (2017). What is play fighting and what is it good for? Learn Behav. 45, 355–366. doi: 10.3758/s13420-017-0264-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pena, C. J., Bagot, R. C., Labonte, B., and Nestler, E. J. (2014). Epigenetic signaling in psychiatric disorders. J. Mol. Biol. 426, 3389–3412. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, G. D., Howes, S. R., Whitelaw, R. B., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1994). Isolation rearing impairs the reinforcing efficacy of intravenous cocaine or intra-accumbens d-amphetamine: impaired response to intra-accumbens D1 and D2/D3 dopamine receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology 115, 419–429. doi: 10.1007/bf02245085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, S. B., Geyer, M. A., Preece, M. A., Pitcher, L. K., Reynolds, G. P., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2003). Dopamine depletion of the nucleus accumbens reverses isolation-induced deficits in prepulse inhibition in rats. Neuroscience 119, 233–240. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00122-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Primack, B. A., Shensa, A., Sidani, J. E., Whaite, E. O., Lin, L. Y., Rosen, D., et al. (2017). Social media use and perceived social isolation among young adults in the US. Am. J. Prev. Med. 53, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pritchard, L. M., Van Kempen, T. A., and Zimmerberg, B. (2013). Behavioral effects of repeated handling differ in rats reared in social isolation and environmental enrichment. Neurosci. Lett. 536, 47–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.12.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, L. M., Pokinko, M., Torres-Berrio, A., Cuesta, S., Lambert, L. C., Del Cid Pellitero, E., et al. (2018). DCC receptors drive prefrontal cortex maturation by determining dopamine axon targeting in adolescence. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 181–192. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, N. R., and Roberts, D. C. (1996). Progressive ratio schedules in drug self-administration studies in rats: a method to evaluate reinforcing efficacy. J. Neurosci. Methods 66, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(95)00153-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, D. C., Loh, E. A., and Vickers, G. (1989). Self-administration of cocaine on a progressive ratio schedule in rats: dose-response relationship and effect of haloperidol pretreatment. Psychopharmacology 97, 535–538. doi: 10.1007/bf00439560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeo, R. D. (2010). Pubertal maturation and programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal reactivity. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31, 232–240. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeo, R. D. (2013). The teenage brain: the stress response and the adolescent brain. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 140–145. doi: 10.1177/0963721413475445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeo, R. D. (2017). The impact of stress on the structure of the adolescent brain: Implications for adolescent mental health. Brain Res. 1654, 185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeo, R. D., Bellani, R., Karatsoreos, I. N., Chhua, N., Vernov, M., Conrad, C. D., et al. (2006). Stress history and pubertal development interact to shape hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis plasticity. Endocrinology 147, 1664–1674. doi: 10.1210/en.2005-1432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salatino-Oliveira, A., Rohde, L. A., and Hutz, M. H. (2018). The dopamine transporter role in psychiatric phenotypes. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 177, 211–231. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M., and Meaney, M. J. (1986). Maturation of the adrenocortical stress response: neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress hyporesponsive period. Brain Res. 396, 64–76. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(86)90010-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenk, S., Hunt, T., Malovechko, R., Robertson, A., Klukowski, G., and Amit, Z. (1986). Differential effects of isolation housing on the conditioned place preference produced by cocaine and amphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24, 1793–1796. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90523-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenk, S., Lacelle, G., Gorman, K., and Amit, Z. (1987). Cocaine self-administration in rats influenced by environmental conditions: implications for the etiology of drug abuse. Neurosci. Lett. 81, 227–231. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(87)91003-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. M., Molenda-Figueira, H. A., and Sisk, C. L. (2009). Back to the future: The organizational-activational hypothesis adapted to puberty and adolescence. Horm. Behav. 55, 597–604. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skelly, M. J., Chappell, A. E., Carter, E., and Weiner, J. L. (2015). Adolescent social isolation increases anxiety-like behavior and ethanol intake and impairs fear extinction in adulthood: Possible role of disrupted noradrenergic signaling. Neuropharmacology 97, 149–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. A., Lacy, R. T., and Strickland, J. C. (2014). The effects of social learning on the acquisition of cocaine self-administration. Drug Alcohol Depend. 141, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sodhi, M. S., and Sanders-Bush, E. (2004). Serotonin and brain development. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 59, 111–174. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(04)59006-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00014-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: what changes and why? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 51–58. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroud, L. R., Foster, E., Papandonatos, G. D., Handwerger, K., Granger, D. A., Kivlighan, K. T., et al. (2009). Stress response and the adolescent transition: performance versus peer rejection stressors. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 47–68. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Collishaw, S., Pine, D. S., and Thapar, A. K. (2012). Depression in adolescence. Lancet 379, 1056–1067. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60871-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorsell, A., Slawecki, C. J., El Khoury, A., Mathe, A. A., and Ehlers, C. L. (2006). The effects of social isolation on neuropeptide Y levels, exploratory and anxiety-related behaviors in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 83, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. J., and Lloyd, D. A. (2004). Stress burden and the lifetime incidence of psychiatric disorder in young adults: racial and ethnic contrasts. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 481–488. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.5.481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazquez, D. M., Lopez, J. F., Morano, M. I., Kwak, S. P., Watson, S. J., and Akil, H. (1998). Alpha, beta and gamma mineralocorticoid receptor messenger ribonucleic acid splice variants: differential expression and rapid regulation in the developing hippocampus. Endocrinology 139, 3165–3177. doi: 10.1210/en.139.7.3165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, D. M., Bell, M. R., Flores, C., Gulley, J. M., Willing, J., and Paul, M. J. (2017). Adolescence and reward: making sense of neural and behavioral changes amid the chaos. J. Neurosci. 37, 10855–10866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1834-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weintraub, A., Singaravelu, J., and Bhatnagar, S. (2010). Enduring and sex-specific effects of adolescent social isolation in rats on adult stress reactivity. Brain Res. 1343, 83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, I. C., Pryce, C. R., Jongen-Relo, A. L., Nanz-Bahr, N. I., and Feldon, J. (2004). Effect of social isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 152, 279–295. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, V. G., Hofford, R. S., Yates, J. R., Jennings, F. C., and Bardo, M. T. (2015). Sex differences in monoamines following amphetamine and social reward in adolescent rats. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 23, 197–205. doi: 10.1037/pha0000026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitaker, L. R., Degoulet, M., and Morikawa, H. (2013). Social deprivation enhances VTA synaptic plasticity and drug-induced contextual learning. Neuron 77, 335–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E. (2004). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the substance use disorders: the nature of the relationship, subtypes at risk and treatment issues. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 27, 283–301. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00113-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, C., von Rumohr, A., Mundt, A., Petrus, D., Klein, J., Lee, T., et al. (2007). Lesions of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta and in the ventral tegmental area enhance depressive-like behavior in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 184, 133–141. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wongwitdecha, N., and Marsden, C. A. (1995). Isolation rearing prevents the reinforcing properties of amphetamine in a conditioned place preference paradigm. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 279, 99–103. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(95)00212-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wongwitdecha, N., and Marsden, C. A. (1996). Effect of social isolation on the reinforcing properties of morphine in the conditioned place preference test. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 53, 531–534. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02046-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, I. K., Upton, N., and Marsden, C. A. (1991). Resocialisation of isolation-reared rats does not alter their anxiogenic profile on the elevated X-maze model of anxiety. Physiol. Behav. 50, 1129–1132. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90572-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yates, J. R., Beckmann, J. S., Meyer, A. C., and Bardo, M. T. (2013). Concurrent choice for social interaction and amphetamine using conditioned place preference in rats: effects of age and housing condition. Drug Alcohol Depend. 129, 240–246. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yorgason, J. T., Calipari, E. S., Ferris, M. J., Karkhanis, A. N., Fordahl, S. C., Weiner, J. L., et al. (2016). Social isolation rearing increases dopamine uptake and psychostimulant potency in the striatum. Neuropharmacology 101, 471–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yorgason, J. T., Espana, R. A., Konstantopoulos, J. K., Weiner, J. L., and Jones, S. R. (2013). Enduring increases in anxiety-like behavior and rapid nucleus accumbens dopamine signaling in socially isolated rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 1022–1031. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakharova, E., Miller, J., Unterwald, E., Wade, D., and Izenwasser, S. (2009). Social and physical environment alter cocaine conditioned place preference and dopaminergic markers in adolescent male rats. Neuroscience 163, 890–897. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zernig, G., Kummer, K. K., and Prast, J. M. (2013). Dyadic social interaction as an alternative reward to cocaine. Front. Psychiatry 4:100. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X., Sun, L., Jia, H., Meng, Q., Wu, S., Li, N., et al. (2009). Isolation rearing induces social and emotional function abnormalities and alters glutamate and neurodevelopment-related gene expression in rats. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 1173–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Guan, Q., Li, K., Tao, L., Hu, J., and Xiao, J. (2012). Dissection of the maternal effects on puberty onset by embryo transplantation in mouse. J. Endocrinol. Invest. 35, 676–680. doi: 10.3275/8125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adolescence, isolation rearing, anxiety, depression, reward, addiction, serotonin, dopamine

Citation: Walker DM, Cunningham AM, Gregory JK and Nestler EJ (2019) Long-Term Behavioral Effects of Post-weaning Social Isolation in Males and Females. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:66. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00066

Received: 03 December 2018; Accepted: 14 March 2019;
Published: 11 April 2019.

Edited by:

Kevin G. Bath, Brown University, United States

Reviewed by:

Charlis Raineki, University of British Columbia, Canada
Tania L. Roth, University of Delaware, United States
Maya Opendak, New York University, United States

Copyright © 2019 Walker, Cunningham, Gregory and Nestler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eric J. Nestler, eric.nestler@mssm.edu

Download