Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 22 May 2019
Sec. Learning and Memory
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00082

Why Study the History of Neuroscience?

  • Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada

The history of neuroscience is the memory of the discipline and this memory depends on the study of the present traces of the past; the things left behind: artifacts, equipment, written documents, data books, photographs, memoirs, etc. History, in all of its definitions, is an integral part of neuroscience and I have used examples from the literature and my personal experience to illustrate the importance of the different aspects of history in neuroscience. Each time we talk about the brain, do an experiment, or write a research article, we are involved in history. Each published experiment becomes a historical document; it relies on past research (the “Introduction” section), procedures developed in the past (“Methods” section) and as soon as new data are published, they become history and become embedded into the history of the discipline (“Discussion” section). In order to be transparent and able to be replicated, each experiment requires its own historical archive. Studying history means researching books, documents and objects in libraries, archives, and museums. It means looking at data books, letters and memos, talking to scientists, and reading biographies and autobiographies. History can be made relevant by integrating historical documents into classes and by using historical websites. Finally, conducting historical research can be interesting, entertaining, and can lead to travel to out-of-the-way and exotic places and meeting interesting people.

Introduction

I know of very few neuroscience programs that focus on the history of neuroscience. However, all research involves the study of history, even if it is only the history of a single research topic. For example, a research project on “The genetic basis of Long-Term Potentiation (LTP)” would involve some history of the discovery and relevant research on LTP. Likewise, a study on “The role of oxytocin in maternal behavior of Peromyscus” would involve a history of studies of the effects of oxytocin on behavior. In other cases, researchers testing a particular theory such as “The amyloid beta theory of Alzheimer’s disease” must discuss the history of this theory before they can make a new contribution to it. Researchers who use a particular research method, such as “single cell recording from the hippocampus” must know the history of the technique if they are going to improve upon it. Thus, all researchers are involved in studying the history of their particular research project and will summarize this history in the “Introduction” and “Discussion” sections of their research articles. Making space in a neuroscience program for a course on the history of neuroscience can be a hard sell, but neuroscientists with a strong historical understanding are more equipped to understand the context of their own work and its impact than those with no historical background. As noted by Shepherd (2010, p. 4):

“A historical perspective provides an education in how scientists are able to push past the limits of current concepts in order to fashion a new and more comprehensive understanding of the laws of nature.”

In 2008, Zoltán Molnár and I rediscovered a box of Charles Sherrington’s histological slides at Oxford University that had been neglected for over 70 years (Molnár and Brown, 2010). Sherrington is one of the most influential neurophysiologists in the history of neuroscience, winning the 1932 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine (Eccles and Gibson, 1979). Considering this, it is incredible that these important historical scientific artifacts were unknown and inaccessible for such a long period of time. For every Sherrington’s box that is rediscovered and made accessible to scientific researchers and the public, many more historical objects are lost.

What Is History?

“Everything but tomorrow is history”

Grainger (1956; p. 81).

The definition of history and the methods used to conduct historical research are fraught with criticism and debate. Elton (1967) argued that historical research is the search for the objective truth about the past. Carr (1961), on the other hand, argued that history is a product of its own time; it is the interpretation of events and facts from the past through the specific lens of the historian’s own ideas and ideologies. Later, Samuel (1992) argued that history is not a record of the past, more or less faithful to the facts, but an invention or fiction of historians themselves. Edelman (2006) discusses the concept of scientific history from the point of view of scientists and historians. Indeed, there are so many aspects of the history of science that in some areas, such as physics, one can write a history of the history of science (Južnič, 2016). Thus, the term “history” has multiple meanings. History is the study of the past and includes the study of past events, the events connected with a particular person or discovery, a chronological record of an experiment, a historical document, or events that are no longer relevant to the present. History is a discipline, a method, and a personal memoir. Within neuroscience, all of these definitions apply and this article will discuss the relevance of different aspects of history to neuroscientists today.

History as Memory

An approach to history that might appeal to neuroscientists is to treat history as memory, with all of its faults and flaws; its errors and omissions (Wachtel, 1986; Cubitt, 2007). The conception of history as a collective memory is a useful perspective for the study of neuroscience. Collective memory is a “form of memory associated with social groups, e.g., nations, families, etc”; it is a socially constructed history of the past (Poole, 2008). The recollections from one’s memory are fallible; a memory is constructed from a series of incidents connected in time and reconstructed from many components. It is not imprinted like a photograph. Memories are fragmented and very seldom do two people remember an event in the same way. Forgetting and false memories occur; emotions and opinions can distort memories and a recalled memory may become reconsolidated with erroneous information (Lee et al., 2017). Therefore, a “perfect” memory may include errors, omissions, distortions and false recollections (Loftus and Palmer, 1974; Schacter, 1996; Loftus, 2003). For example, in Dawkins’s (2013) memoir, An Appetite for Wonder, there is a group photograph that is captioned “The Animal Behavior Research Group after the move from Bevington Road.” That move occurred in 1971, but I am in that photograph, and I was only in the ABRG in 1976 and 1977. So, when was that photo taken? What was the event? My own memory of the event is vague. A third person featured in the photograph, Marian Dawkins, recalled that it was taken at a group picnic in November 1977. Clearly, even our own memories of events that we were involved in are subject to personal and unique distortions.

In considering the perspective of history as memory, the definition of history may shift towards the subjective perspective of Carr (1961) or Samuel (1992), and away from the objective perspective of Elton (1967). Overall, a memory can be seen as the raw material of history; whether it is a written or an oral recollection, it is the source from which historians draw information. History is thus a collection of individual and collective memories (Jimerson, 2003), which are the facts as we remember them. When the holders of these memories are gone, history becomes a cultural memory, which is the interpretation of these facts by someone who was not present at the event. Like any memory, history consists of a number of fragments linked together in time, with inevitable errors, confabulations, omissions, and errors of reconsolidation. Despite this, it is all we have. The definition of history as a collective memory can help to clarify the purpose of its study, as summarized by McNeill (1985):

“Historical knowledge is no more and no less than carefully and critically constructed collective memory. As such it can both make us wiser in our public choices and more richly human in our private lives.”

History Is the Study of What Remains

History is the study of the present traces of the past. If objects no longer exist, it is as though they had never existed. The essential element for historical study is the remaining evidence of materials from the past; those that no longer exist, cannot be studied (Elton, 1967). History involves analyzing and interpreting the present traces of the past and integrating information from different sources to develop a “historical memory” of events as they occurred. An example of this is the study of trepanation in ancient Greco-Roman medicine. To understand trepanation, Tullo (2010) examined the osteoarcheological evidence, the material culture and the written texts from the Roman Empire. The osteoarcheological evidence consisted of skulls, such as the skull of Chios, which showed evidence of trepanation (Figure 1). The material culture consisted of the tools used for trepanation, which were sophisticated surgical instruments for boring holes in the skull. The written texts described the procedures for trepanation. For this, Tullo (2010) consulted Hippocrates, On Head Wounds, written in 400 BC (Panourias et al., 2005). Each of these present traces of the past, however, had to be interpreted in the present as no physician now exists from ancient Rome to describe the process of trepanation, why and how it was done, how patients were treated and how many survived the treatment. Many other historical artifacts no longer survive. Bandages, ligatures, steel tools and other objects deteriorate with time and provide no present traces. Thus, the study of history involves the quest for the present traces of the past.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The three elements that are necessary to understand the significance of historical objects. In this case, the trephined skull (A) is the material object; the tools used for trephining (B) show how the holes might have been made, and the text from Hippocrates (C) gives a textual description of the instructions for using the tools to trephine the skull. (A) The trepanned skull of Chios. The arrow points to the healed linear fracture associated with the bur hole. Used with permission of the copyright holder: Copyright Hellenic Ministry of Culture and Sports, Ephorate Antiquities of Chios, Archeological Museum of Chios. Published with permission. (B) The raspatory (left), the serrated trepan or trephine (center), and the trepan (right) were used for scraping, sawing, and drilling, respectively. Used with permission from Tsermoulas et al. (2014). Panel (C) is adapted from Hanson (1999).

Archeological evidence and material culture are kept in museums and ancient manuscripts are kept in libraries and archives. These are the sites that select, classify and maintain the present traces of the past; the basic materials that comprise “archival memory” (Steedman, 1980; Jimerson, 2003; Rivera-Orraca, 2009). Like individual and collective memory, archives, museums and libraries are selective in what they collect and maintain; many objects are never saved, while others are lost, deteriorate or are discarded. Thus, “archival memory” is selective and subject to errors, omissions, and interpretations. Museum displays and archival documents reflect the perspectives of those who decide what to save (remember) and what to discard (forget), and how to classify, maintain and display their holdings (memories; Jimerson, 2003). Like individual and collective memory, archival and historical memories are reconstructions of the past from the present traces, as organized by historians at a particular time (defined as “the present”) and in a particular place. However, many historical artifacts are lost or destroyed. For the history of neuroscience, these losses are serious. If history is a collective memory of the past, the loss of the historical objects and documents of neuroscience is equivalent to “scientific Alzheimer’s disease.” Treating this disciplinary ailment is more than a matter of preserving and cataloging laboratory notes, specimens, equipment, drawings and documents. This material needs to be incorporated into the teaching and practice of neuroscience today.

History is not static but is updated by new discoveries. As with neuroscience itself, new discoveries enable us to update our knowledge of the history of neuroscience. New discoveries of lost manuscripts, letters or memoirs; new biographies and autobiographies of scientists and the discovery of new experimental phenomena allow us to reconsider the course of history. For example, new discoveries resulted in the re-evaluation of the function of Golgi cells and Golgi’s theories of cellular connections (Kruger and Otis, 2007; Galliano et al., 2010). Likewise, modern discoveries in epigenetics have led to the up-dating of Darwin’s theory of evolution (Skinner, 2015) and our understanding of environmental influences on brain and behavior development and evolution (Keverne, 2014; Keverne et al., 2015). Likewise, the discovery of letters from Sigmund Freud to his school friend, Eduard Silberstein, not published until 1990 (Boehlich, 1990) give new insight into Freud as a student.

Saving the Vanishing History of Neuroscience

The present traces of the past are being lost. These include books, diaries, published articles, unpublished manuscripts, photographs, original data, laboratory books, equipment and instruction manuals on how to use this equipment. As the people, both scientists and technicians, who did the work retire and then die, the experimental protocols, technical skills, expertise and oral history is lost with them. This matters because the crucial element for studying history is the present evidence of the past; with no evidence, there is no history. To understand the history of neuroscience, we must save the traces of neuroscience of the past and present. But how should this be done? Books should be donated to and saved by libraries. Articles, manuscripts, diaries, lab notebooks and data should be sent to archives. Equipment and the instruction manuals for its use should be sent to museums. Why not develop a “History of Neuroscience Museum?” Lorusso et al. (2018) have proposed a “neuroscience without borders” program to preserve the history of neuroscience in Europe. IBRO, FENS and the Society for Neuroscience all have sections on their websites on the history of neuroscience and history poster sessions at their annual meetings. Autobiographical accounts of neuroscientists provide important information on how the history of neuroscience was done by those who did it (Squire, 1996–2018). More neuroscientists should be encouraged to write their memoirs and send their lab notes and other research materials to libraries, archives and museums.

The Virtual History of Neuroscience?

The internet is an important resource for saving the history of neuroscience. Many historical neuroscience books and journals are now available online as are some archives. FENS, IBRO and the SfN have history websites, but what else can be done? Why not develop a virtual museum of neuroscience? This was our thinking when we developed the Oxford History of Medicine website, which focuses on the history of neuroscience1. On this website, we have digitized the slides from the collections of Sir Charles Sherrington and Sir Wilfrid Le Gros Clark. A variety of historical instruments, teaching models and other objects have been captured using 3-D photography and can be viewed on this website. There is also a collection of clinical neurological cases with case histories, stories about the objects, art works, and videos of Oxford lectures on the history of neuroscience.

We have also developed the European Brain Museum Tour project (Lorusso et al., 2018), which is a virtual tour of museums with specialized collections on the brain and the history of neuroscience in each country of Europe2. Using this virtual tour, neuroscience enthusiasts can find exhibits of interest and use the links on this website to view each museum’s own website. This project, which is now sponsored by the FENS History of Neuroscience Committee, started as part of the “History of Neuroscience” course that I taught at Dalhousie University and we hope that it will be useful to researchers as well as students in neuroscience. There are numerous other websites about the history of neuroscience, such as the milestones in neuroscience research3. These can be found using an internet search.

Why Study the History of Neuroscience?

Neuroscience is a recent area of science, emerging as an explicit discipline in the late 20th century (Shepherd, 2010), but the history of neuroscience goes back to the ancient Egyptians (Breasted, 1930; Finger, 1994). Most students enter neuroscience to perform cutting-edge research with innovative technologies, to understand the complex workings of the brain and discover how to repair them when mechanisms fail. Seldom do students enter neuroscience in order to study its history. As with other scientific disciplines, neuroscientists focus on new discoveries and contemporary theories. However, even the disproved and rejected theories of the past, such as phrenology, which have been relegated to cautionary anecdotes and the introductory sections of textbooks, may be re-evaluated in light of more recent discoveries (Jerison, 1977; Jones et al., 2018).

Many neuroscientists are interested in the history of their discipline, but not everyone agrees that the knowledge of the history of neuroscience is helpful or important for doing modern neuroscience research (Falk and Falk, 2007). Neuroscientists tend to work pragmatically; using models that work and are consistent with the data without worrying about the philosophical or historical issues involved. However, theoretical models break down, and at some point, they cease to be able to explain observed phenomena or to produce new testable theories (Maienschein et al., 2008). At this point, a history of science perspective can be useful to analyze a theory historically, investigate the circumstances under which it was created, learn about its creators, examine the phenomena and the original data incorporated into it, critique the underlying assumptions, and examine rival theories that were previously rejected (Maienschein et al., 2008). This approach offers a wealth of material to research scientists, who can look to these historical findings to examine how theories are constructed. With a history of science approach, the end of a model’s productive life does not mean that it cannot still furnish important lessons to researchers. For example, there are numerous theories of the causes of Alzheimer’s disease (Armstrong, 2013; Cubinkova et al., 2018; Table 1), and new research attempts to integrate behavioral, genetic and environmental risk factors into an over-arching theory of AD (Nehls, 2016). Once the causes of AD are known, one should be able to follow the historical threads of each of these theories.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. What causes Alzheimer’s disease?

A history of science approach can also be an aid to understanding the interdisciplinary nature of neuroscience. Neuroscience, or more accurately, the neurosciences, involve anatomy, physiology, pharmacology, neurology, psychiatry, molecular biology, biochemistry, physics, cell biology, developmental biology, evolution, physics, chemistry, engineering, computer science, ethology, psychology and economics (Shepherd, 2010). Each of these fields has its own theories, methodologies, and data, but through collaborative research projects, these influence each other. For example, during the time that Star (1983) observed researchers in a psychophysiology laboratory, she saw that they collaborated with neurologists and imported neurological procedures into their research. In doing so, the psychophysiologists adopted the assumptions about the localization of brain function made by the neurologists. Although this interdisciplinarity is both necessary and fruitful in neuroscience (Shepherd, 2010), it comes with the risks of mixing disciplinary assumptions about neural processes without a strong grounding in the bases for these assumptions. A critical historical approach can help neuroscientists engaged in interdisciplinary research to navigate these issues (Falk and Falk, 2007).

The Importance of the History of Science in General

To understand the importance of studying the history of neuroscience, it is useful to review the importance of historical research in science in general. To appreciate the role of science and its influence on society involves understanding its history (Nutton, 2004; Lindberg, 2007), philosophy (Godfrey-Smith, 2003) and sociology (Merton, 1962; Bloor et al., 1996). The history of science describes the discoveries in science, their evolution and how they were transmitted and received (Grainger, 1956). A textbook of neuroscience, for example, documents the history and progress of each area of neuroscience (Finger, 1994). Discoveries do not appear out of the blue; they have long periods of incubation and their importance may not be recognized for many years. New scientific knowledge is transmitted through books and journal articles, which have their own history (Gross et al., 2002). Only when they are published can new discoveries be distributed and critically evaluated.

Many new discoveries have been rejected as “nonsense” when first published only to be confirmed later (Gross, 2009), resulting in paradigm shifts in science (Kuhn, 1962). The discovery of a new fact is exciting, but if it is too revolutionary it will be rejected by established scientists; it must be proved to be correct and doubters must be convinced by hard data. The discovery of a truth is one thing, but equally necessary is its transmission to other scientists and their acceptance of it (Grainger, 1956, p. 80). New discoveries and new theories result in controversies. Of such controversies are scientific histories made. For example, Vesalius’s (1543) book on human anatomy was rejected when first published and stimulated heated controversy that lasted decades (Montagu, 1955). The debate on whether neural communication was electrical or chemical (the soups vs. the sparks debate) also continued for decades (Valenstein, 2002). The theory of animal electricity incited debates between Galvani and Volta (Piccolino, 1998) and the neuron doctrine resulted in debates between Golgi and Cajal (Guillery, 2005). The history of neuroscience is important for investigating how advances in neuroscience occur. For example, while some would argue that new tools and techniques are fundamental for new discoveries in neuroscience (Bickle, 2016), others would argue that conceptual change is what drives advances in neuroscience (Parker, 2018).

The formal academic study of the history of science dates from the founding of the journal ISIS by George Sarton in 1913 (Sarton, 1918; Barnes, 1920; Hellman, 1958) and showed a significant increase in the 1950s (Albrecht-Carrie, 1951; Grainger, 1956). By the 1970s, Brush (1974) made the suggestion that the history of science might be subversive and should be X-rated. He suggested, with tongue-in-cheek, that the study of the history of science might pervert young minds by telling them the truth about how science is done as opposed to the idealized methods of science presented in textbooks. By this time, the traditional objective view of the history of science as presented by Elton (1967) was giving way to the more subjective interpretational view of history given by Carr (1961) and represented by the view of history as a flawed memory process (Cubitt, 2007). It is interesting to compare Grainger’s (1956) reasons for studying the history of science (Table 2), which focus on discovering objective truths, to the more subjective reasons of Maienschein (2000). On the other hand, although Maienschein (2000) states that even if the history of science is subjective and riddled with errors of memory, there are still many good reasons for studying it (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Why study the history of science?

Grainger (1956) painted an idealized view of the historian of science as someone who must have a sound knowledge of science, philosophical wisdom and a historical sense, and be able to embrace the whole picture of the role of science in society. According to Grainger (1956), the historian of science must also be aware that the education of science students focuses on teaching “facts” and technological expertise. Many scientists are so specialized and narrow in their outlook that they reject the history of science as “irrelevant” and a waste of time. The enlightened scientist, however, judges modern findings in the light of history in order to understand the relationships between seemingly disparate studies and to use knowledge from one area of science to facilitate discoveries in another. Finally, the history of science, like any history, is an exercise in detective work. In some ways understanding the history of a scientific discovery is almost as exciting as making the discovery itself (Goodfield, 1981).

Falk and Falk (2007, p. 44) suggest that there are a number of benefits of scientists becoming historians. These include the analysis of the social and cultural background of scientific discoveries; the context of scientific research; and the implicit assumptions that underlie the “dominant scientific Zeitgeist” under which scientific research is conducted. They argue that scientists “adopt unawares those doctrines (and implicit assumptions) that are established by the dominant scientific Zeitgeist,” and suggest that studying the history of science provides a way that scientists can recognize and overcome their culturally-determined biases about their research. In the past, historians of science emphasized that scientists had an almost moral duty to learn the history of science so that they could situate their pursuit of knowledge in relation to the social, cultural and political events of their time (Grainger, 1956). This is still an important reason to study the history of science. In many cases, scientists investigating the brain are unaware of the cultural biases that influence their work (Cooter, 2014). Historians of science should make, and neuroscientists should read, engaging, critical, rigorous histories of the study of the brain and the nervous system to overcome these biases. Another benefit of studying the history of science is to credit researchers whose discoveries were premature and thus unheeded, or published in older or foreign-language (non-English) journals (Gross, 2009).

Scientists who become historians also bridge the “two cultures” of the arts and social sciences and scientific research and are more able to see the influence of the arts on the sciences, and vice-versa (Edwards, 2010; Frazzetto, 2011; Garcia-Lopez, 2012). Scientists who take a historical perspective are able to have a more comprehensive perception of their scientific work and may see things that others miss (Root-Bernstein, 1988). Finally, because there is a great amount of “implicit knowledge” in science that cannot be learned from textbooks, but only from hands-on research experience, the scientist-cum-historian can explain the implicit rules of scientific work which are never included in published articles. Indeed, one of the most significant contributions of scientists to the history of science may be their personal reminiscences, stories and anecdotes about how their research was really done. These only come to light in autobiographical works such as those of Watson (1968) on the double helix and Cajal (1937/1989) on his life in neurobiology. The personal accounts of many neuroscientists (e.g., Hebb, 1980) have been given in the many volumes of The history of psychology in autobiography that began as far back as the 1930s and in the series of The history of neuroscience in autobiography edited by Larry Squire since 1996.

Finally, scientific theories influence broader societal ideas about the way the world works. The rise of the neurosciences has produced a “neuroculture,” a distinct sociocultural phenomenon that looks to neuroscience to explain all aspects of human behavior (Rolls, 2012; Mora, 2015). Neuroculture has taken hold in literature, film, television and the visual arts, as well as education and even economics (Frazzetto and Anker, 2009). Indeed, neuroculture seems to have taken hold of all aspects of modern life, but it is not without its critics (Casper, 2014; Cooter, 2014).

In summary, the answer to the question, “why study the history of neuroscience?” falls into five categories: (1) self-improvement, illuminating the theories and methods of neuroscience and improving upon them; (2) efficiency, avoiding past mistakes and learning from them; (3) perspective, providing judgment and clarity and thus enlarging the scope of neuroscience; (4) imagination, offering a wide repertoire of ideas; and (5) education, improving scientific literacy and the public understanding of neuroscience (Maienschein et al., 2008). Shepherd (2010) points out that studying the history of neuroscience helps us to understand the interdisciplinary nature of neuroscience, and shows how neuroscientific research extends across all species, systems and levels of neural organization. A historical approach examines the factors that produce discoveries: the methods and techniques used, the personalities of the scientist themselves and the social, cultural, political and ethical issues underlying neuroscience research.

How Does History Affect You?

The average neuroscientist or student of neuroscience may not feel that they are affected by the history of neuroscience. However, the history of neuroscience is not just an academic discipline; history is personal and your own research history is important in three ways. The terminology you use, the scientific articles you write, and the integrity of your research publications all depend on history.

Naming the Brain

Simply talking about the brain is an exercise in history. The term “brain” was first used by the ancient Egyptians in about 1700 BC (Breasted, 1930); the distinction between the “cerebrum” (enkephalon) and “cerebellum” (parenkephalis) was first made by Aristotle around 300 BC, and the “entorhinal area” was defined by Brodmann in 1909 (see Swanson, 2015 for the history of neuroanatomical terminology). Likewise, the terms “neuron,” “synapse,” and “neurotransmitter” all have a history (López-Muñoz and Alamo, 2009). Histological techniques, such as the Golgi stain have a history (Shepherd et al., 2011), the “neurone doctrine” (Shepherd, 1991/2015), the “amyloid cascade hypothesis” of Alzheimer’s disease (Hardy, 2017) and the theory of the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD; Hoffman et al., 2017; Suzuki, 2018) all have a history. So to talk about any aspect of neuroscience is to talk about history.

Writing a Scientific Paper

The completion of your research articles relies on the history of the problem of interest, and, when your research article is published, it becomes a part of history. There are many guides on how to write research articles (for example: Neill, 2007; Plaxco, 2010; Adams, 2011; Kallestinova, 2011; Saper, 2015). Each section of a research article involves some form of history, thus all scientists study history when completing a research project and writing a scientific article. The “Introduction” summarizes the history of the problem that you are investigating and introduces the hypotheses you plan to test. It summarizes the main discoveries in the area and who made them. When you are testing a theory, the “Introduction” describes who developed the theory, what previous data supports or refutes it, and describes any controversies, which you hope that your research will resolve. It provides the scientific context for your research problem with reference to theoretical and empirical developments in the field. Your “Methods” section describes the apparatus and procedures used, often discussing who developed the equipment and techniques, their reliability and validity. Your experimental methods, both those explicitly referenced in the article and those tacitly passed on, all have a history. In some cases, older methods may be used to solve research problems in the present (Schwerdtfeger, 2018; Schwerdtfeger and Tobet, 2018). The core of your research article is the presentation of new results. Once the experiment is completed, your “Results” section is written and adds new data to the field of study. These results are new today, but as soon as they are published, they are history. The “Discussion” section describes how your findings fit into the history of the problem and whether or not they support the theory being tested. The “Discussion” section summarizes how your research is relevant to previous research and summarizes the advances that you have made. The “Reference” section describes the history of the problem by listing the publications that you relied on to complete your study. It is a bibliographic resumé of the history of your research topic. When writing a scientific article, you become a selective historian. Your introduction does not cite every article on your topic; it is selective, focusing on the articles most relevant to your study. Likewise, you select the apparatus, methods and procedure from the many options available. You may even select which results to publish and which to omit. Your “Discussion” is also selective, focusing on one or more of the possible theories to explain your results. Finally, your reference list is also selective; you do not cite every single article on your topic.

Once published, your article can become part of the history of someone else’s research project. If you write a review article, it is a history project. Your grant proposals are also history projects: they must demonstrate how the proposed research fits into the history of the field. Grant reviewers and journal referees must evaluate whether your research adds enough to the great chain of knowledge to be funded or published. This means that the reviewers of grants and manuscripts must know the past and current history of the research being evaluated. Thus, everything that you do in neuroscience involves history. Often without realizing it, neuroscientists study history simply by conducting their research, writing review articles, grant applications and reviewing manuscripts.

Keeping Your Own Historical Archives

A scientific publication involves two types of history: the history of the problem being researched (the literature cited) and the history of the experimental project itself. If someone wants to replicate your experiment, they will need to know all of the details of the methods and procedure: the subjects, the protocols followed, equipment used, laboratory conditions and methodological issues not included in the article and they may want to see your laboratory notebooks, apparatus and methods, raw data, statistical analyses, etc. (Gorgolewski and Poldrack, 2016; Gilmore et al., 2017; Zwaan et al., 2018 and the ensuing commentaries). But what happens if other researchers cannot replicate your findings? What if you are accused of fraud or research misconduct? Your raw data, statistical analyses, letters, notebooks, diaries and e-mails will be open to scrutiny. Thus, for each published article, a personal archive is critical in order to provide a comprehensive documentation of your experimental procedures and results and to establish that there was no scientific misconduct, fudging of results, manipulation of data or photo-shopping of figures. Open and transparent research relies on a careful historical account of each research project (Iqbal et al., 2016). To improve reproducibility of published findings, many journals, such as Nature, require authors to submit research report forms that provide information on the research subjects, materials used, experimental design and statistical analyses (see Nature Publishing Group, 2017).

What do you do when others cannot replicate your results? This happened to me when other authors (Birke and Sadler, 1984) could not replicate the results of my study on olfaction and scent marking in rats (Brown, 1978). By carefully examining the differences in methods between the two studies, I was able to show that using the methods of my study, I replicated my results and using the methods of Birke and Sadler (1984), I replicated their results (Brown, 1991). Thus, by replicating the different methods used in both studies, I was able to show that the differences in social and sexual experience of the rats in each study produced the different behavioral results. There are many such methodological issues in behavioral neuroscience (Schellinck et al., 2010). Thus, it is important to keep a research archive for each publication.

When developing a personal research archive, many questions must be answered. What should be kept? Where should it be kept and for how long? How can other scientists access your archive? Your research archive is a historical document and may become subject to inspection and scrutiny. Many journals, universities, and governmental institutions now provide archives for the documentation of each research project and the storage of raw data. For example, Cambridge University has a website with a guide to research data management4, and there are a number of other guidelines to how to manage research data (see for example, Ingram, 2016).

The development of data archives enables the creation of large neuroscience databases for data sharing (Cheung et al., 2009; Freeman, 2015; Wiener et al., 2016). Researchers must, therefore, ensure that the data archived in these large scale databases are reliable and valid. Once such databases are created, those who wish to use them must learn new techniques for data-mining and neuroinformatics (Grisham et al., 2010, 2016; Akil et al., 2011; Gregory et al., 2018) and for statistical analysis (Bzdok and Yeo, 2017). Formal data archives have been developed in structural biology (Kleywegt et al., 2018) and neuroimaging (Gorgolewski et al., 2016; Borghi and Van Gulick, 2018). The Jackson Laboratory Mouse Phenome Database5 is a collaborative archive where data collected on laboratory mouse strains and populations have been collected from multiple sources. We used such public databases (The Allen Brain Atlas, STRING, GoMiner and Mouse Genome Informatics databases) for an in-silico analysis of grooming behavior (Roth et al., 2013). New resources have been published for making large databases available to neuroscientists (Vogelstein et al., 2018) and for searching these databases for designing and planning research projects (Matiasz et al., 2018). Thus, if your data is included in a public database, you must ensure that it is replicable.

What happens if your results cannot be replicated and you are accused of scientific fraud (Gunsalus and Robinson, 2018)? Other researchers, university administrators and granting agencies may want to inspect your personal archives for each experiment. If scientific misconduct is found (if your experimental data does not support the published results) your articles may be retracted and your reputation as a scientist tarnished. The Retraction Watch website6 keeps track of retracted articles. If you cannot replicate your own experiments, the published articles may also need to be retracted. For example, LoLordo and Ross (1990) retracted their own articles because the rescoring of videotapes on which the published data were based did not confirm the original findings. Zhang et al. (2012) had their article in the “Neurobiology of Aging” retracted at the request of the Editor-in-chief and the authors “due to inappropriate duplication of photomicrographs and errors in the description of the material” with the result that “the quantitative results reported in the article cannot be considered reliable” (Zhang et al., 2016). If you are falsely accused of scientific misconduct, you will need to use your scientific archive to demonstrate your research integrity (see Goldenring, 2010). On the other hand, some authors have argued that most published research is not replicable (Ioannidis, 2005), while others argue that failure to replicate is an inherent property of scientific research (Redish et al., 2018). Ioannidis (2014) has presented 12 steps for improving the reliability and validity of published research.

Even if you are certain that none of your own research articles will be retracted, you need to beware of referring to retracted articles in your research articles, reviews and grant proposals. For example, in one of our articles (O’Leary et al., 2018), we referred to Zhang et al. (2012), but when we were doing the revisions to our article, we found that this article had been retracted (Zhang et al., 2016), and so we had to find a new reference and make substantial revisions to our article. When writing review articles one must also beware of retracted articles. Uher and Weaver (2014a) wrote a commentary on a article by Perroud et al. (2014) that was subsequently retracted because one of the authors had fabricated data (Aubry et al., 2014). As a result, Uher and Weaver (2014b) had to retract their commentary on this article. In this case, the embarrassment of having to retract an article had nothing to do with the reviewers, who were innocent victims of a third party. Thus, the Retraction Watch website and database (Brainard, 2018) is becoming an essential resource for preventing neuroscientists from becoming victims of scientific malpractice by others.

In summary, history is a personal issue in neuroscience. Each of your research projects relies on previous studies, methodology and theories. Once your article is published, it becomes a part of history. Establishing a personal archive for each research project is critical to ensure that future researchers can gain access to your data and that your research documents are available for historical analysis. Such an archive is also essential for other researchers who wish to replicate your experiments and to defend yourself against claims of scientific misconduct and the danger of having your articles retracted. Finally, you must be wary of citing articles that have been retracted.

How to Make the History of Neuroscience Relevant Today

History is more than a matter of cataloging and preserving laboratory notes, specimens, equipment, drawings and photographs. This material needs to be incorporated into the teaching and practice of neuroscience research. To make history relevant to students of neuroscience, faculty could integrate historical topics into their neuroscience teaching and research using websites, artifacts and historical publications. Through the study of history, students can be introduced to the historical context of their research and learn how it is integrated with other disciplines and society in general. The history of neuroscience can be approached through textbooks (Finger, 1994, 2000; Glickstein, 2014), or books on special topics (Shepherd, 1991/2015). There are biographies of many neuroscienctists, including Galen (Mattern, 2013), Helmholtz (Meulders, 2010), Golgi (Mazzarello, 2010), and Pavlov (Todes, 2014). Neurobiographies (Söderqvist, 2002, 2007) and autobiographies (Squire, 1996–2018) provide personal histories, describing how individual neuroscientists did their research, and bridging the gap between neuroscience and the humanities.

The most common way that neuroscientists do historical research is to identify past studies on the topic of interest using reference citations, PUBMED, Web of Science or Google Scholar as a guide. However, this often does not encompass the many texts located on library shelves or in library special collections of rare books that must be accessed separately from the main library catalog. Archives keep letters, diaries, photographs, memos and other documents donated by scientists or their families. They may include drafts of articles, unpublished manuscripts, lab data books, and other valuable information. Newspaper clippings, awards, medals, and honorary degrees also find their way into archives. For example, the McGill University archives have many boxes of articles of Donald O. Hebb (Brown and Milner, 2003) and the Oxford University archives have the articles of Sir Charles Sherrington, but so do the archives at the University of Liverpool, the University of British Columbia and the Royal Society of London (Molnár and Brown, 2010). The National Archives of each country also contain materials on the history of individual neuroscientists.

Historical objects may be kept in museums, and there are many museums with brain collections that provide essential resources for studying comparative neurobiology and the history of neuroscience (Iwaniuk, 2010, 2011; Fobbs and Johnson, 2011; Manger, 2011 and other articles in this volume). The importance of saving anatomical specimens in museum collections is that they can be re-analyzed using modern methods to gain new insights into neural disorders. Such is the case of the brains of Broca’s famous patients, Leborgne and Lelong, held in the Museum Dupytren in Paris, which have been reanalyzed by modern researchers using PET and fMRI scans to reveal the damage that was unseen by Broca (Dronkers et al., 2007). In addition, the old equipment used in scientific laboratories, including machinery and lab notes, become critical historical objects (Ceccarelli, 2002; Arnold and Söderqvist, 2011) and it is important to preserve these in museum collections (Lorusso et al., 2018).

Beyond these physical locations, information on the history of the neurosciences is readily accessible through the internet. The internet is an incredibly powerful tool for the study of the history of neuroscience and for engaging students in historical research. Even the most ancient texts can be accessed online. There are also virtual archives and virtual museums. The European Brain Museum Tour website7 allows anyone to locate museums with brain collections at the click of a button. Developing a virtual museum of neuroscience was the reasoning behind the creation of the Oxford History of Medical Sciences Project8 where students and researchers can access slides, objects, art, stories, case histories and seminars that are relevant to conducting historical research. There is also the ability to analyze historical equipment using this website.

Historical research also involves personal interactions: contacting the colleagues, students, friends and families of neuroscientists to locate hidden information about their lives and work. Former students and colleagues of neuroscientists may have letters, manuscripts and photographs stored away in filing cabinets. There may be old materials in university store-rooms that have never been sent to the archives. Still, other historical artifacts have been stored in back rooms of university departments, or in display cases in corridors. Other documents reside in the basements and attics of the friends and families of neuroscientists. Interviews with colleagues, students, spouses, children and grandchildren of neuroscientists may turn up unseen documents, as well as valuable information that has never been published. For example, in this way I obtained some letters and other documents from the families of Sir Wilfred Le Gros Clark and Donald Hebb. Such family documents are not publically available and are on the verge of being lost if someone is not there to salvage them. Once all such information has been collected, one must sort and arrange it and decide how to preserve it. Too often there are large gaps, missing information and one-sided conversations. One may find the letters from A to B, but none of the letters from B to A, and so inferences must be made about the information they might contain. Additionally, if the topic that you are researching is controversial, you may find yourself the center of a war of words, as I found with my investigation of Donald Hebb’s research on sensory deprivation (Brown, 2007a).

History is the study of the present traces of the past or the memories of the past. Thus libraries, archives, and museums are all repositories of the memories of the history of neuroscience. However, we are now facing a number of problems in preserving the history of neuroscience (Lorusso et al., 2018). Many of these repositories have selective memories; some select only certain items to keep and others have political and personal preferences for what is collected. Some archives have online indexes and others do not. Some archives no longer have archivists. Some archives and museums have been closed and the materials are no longer accessible. In addition, libraries, archives and museums “get rid of” or de-access items they no longer deem interesting. Old books, just the kind of thing a historian looks for, which have not been taken out of the library for years, get discarded, so are no longer available to anyone. Archival materials sit in boxes without being indexed and museums reject donations of old equipment as they have no space for it. Most museums exhibit only a small fraction of their holdings; the rest is in storage and may become lost. And, worst of all, documents and equipment may have been destroyed or thrown out by over-zealous administrators who just want to “get rid of all of this old junk.” Finally, many neuroscientists do not leave their records to any library, archive or museum and these records get destroyed or languish unknown in attics and basements. My aim in this article is to encourage the preservation of these historical documents.

The Excitement of Historical Research

Historical research is interesting, illuminating and enlightening. To be able to conceptualize the perspectives of older neuroscientists gives us an ability to understand their discoveries more deeply and make sense of our own research, theories, and methodology. In addition, historical research allows one to hear exciting stories and meet interesting people. If these are not appealing as reasons to study history, some may find consolation in the fact that doing historical research often involves travel to incredible places. Some examples from my own research illustrate how the search for historical documents can be interesting and exciting: academic detective work. These include finding ancient Greco-Roman surgical tools in Greece, searching for Leonardo da Vinci in Italy, finding Sir Charles Sherrington’s box of slides in Oxford, the search for traces of Donald O. Hebb in Montreal, Boston, and Chicago and the opportunity to visit Pavlov’s laboratories and home in Russia.

Ancient Greco-Roman Surgical Tools

During a FENS meeting in Thessaloniki, Greece, in October 2015, I visited the Archeological Museum, which had exhibits on trepanning and on ancient surgical tools. Sets of exact replicas of these tools were for sale in the gift shop and I bought one to use for teaching (Figure 2). These tools include hooks, knives, probes, needles, spatulas, spoons, and knife handles. A copper-alloy knife, used to make incisions during operations, has an engraved depiction of a snake on the blade and a snake’s head at the end, which link the instrument to Asklepios, the god of medicine. A scalpel handle carved in the shape of a mouse is also linked to Asklepios (see Figure 2). When I lecture on the writings of Hippocrates or Galen, I open the box of ancient surgical instruments and the students are struck by how “modern” they look. This increases their interest in history.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Surgical tools from the 3rd century AD. These tools are replicas of those on exhibit at the National Archelogical Museum of Greece and were purchased at the museum in Thessaloniki, Greece. On the far left is a copper-alloy spoon for preparing and taking medicines and applying them to wounds. Next to it is a copper-alloy knife handle that is decorated with a small animal, possibly a mouse, which links the instrument with Asklepios. The mouse was seen as a daemonic being with prophetic powers and was associated with Apollo Smintheus, who protected people against evil and epidemics. The blade is missing. Because blades were made of iron, they often rusted away. The item at the top of the six items shown horizontally is a copper-alloy double hook which is decorated with silver bands at the head and in the middle. It was used during surgical operations on blood vessels (aneurysms), on membranes in the eye, and on tonsils, and to clasp pieces of tissue and the edges of wounds during surgery. The second horizontal item is a copper-alloy knife used to make incisions in the flesh during operations. It has an engraved depiction of a snake on the blade and a snake’s head at the end. The third item is a copper-alloy spoon-shaped probe that was used to prepare and apply medicines. The fourth is a copper-alloy needle used to sew bandages applied to wounds. The fifth is a copper-alloy spatula probe used to mix and apply medicines in deep surgical incisions, to diagnose and measure the depth of injuries, and more rarely to clean internal wounds to the nose and other, larger wounds. The bottom item is also a copper-alloy spatula probe. On the far right is a copper-alloy clasp with serrated ends. This was used to clasp or cut away flesh and tumors during surgical operations. Next to this is another copper-alloy spatula used to clean wounds and incisions, scrape away fistulae, and remove foreign bodies and broken bones from the ear and nose. In eye operations, it was used to remove cysts. It was also used to prepare and apply medicines, particularly to the eyes. These tools are described by Bliquez (1982).

The Search for Leonardo

In order to complete a presentation on Leonardo da Vinci for a history poster (Brown, 2014), I obtained security clearance to view the original pages from Leonardo’s notebooks, which are held in the print room at Windsor Castle in England. When I left I ordered my own copies of the prints to use for teaching. During the FENS Milan meeting, Lorenzo Lorusso organized tours of libraries and archives such as the Biblioteca Nazionale Braidense, which displayed Leonardo’s drawings. On this tour, we visited the town of Vinci (Figure 3), the Leonardo museum and the house where Leonardo was born. In Florence, we visited other Leonardo da Vinci museums, libraries, and exhibits where we purchased books on Leonardo and prints of his drawings. In researching Leonardo da Vinci as a neuroscientist, a tour of Italy is interesting and exciting.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The house in Vinci, Italy, where Leonardo Da Vinci was born [Photo by Richard Brown].

Sherrington’s Box of Wonders

In 2008, Zoltán Molnár discovered a box of histological slides used by Sherrington from 1888 to 1935 (Figure 4). This box contains 21 drawers of slides from Sherrington’s years at St. Thomas Hospital (1888–1895), Liverpool University (1896–1914) and Oxford University (1914–1935). It also contains slides presented to him by other leading contemporary neuroscientists. Much of the histological data behind these incredible discoveries are available for research at the University of Oxford. This material provided information for our publication on the work of Sherrington (Molnár and Brown, 2010) and the impetus to develop our History of Neuroscience Website9. This website allows the viewer to use a program called “Zoomify” to magnify each image as if it were under a microscope. The materials in this website can now be used for teaching classes anywhere in the world (Chang and Molnár, 2015).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. I am examining Sherrington’s box of slides at Oxford University [Photo by Zoltán Molnár].

The Man Behind the Hebb Synapse

Donald O. Hebb’s influential book, The Organization of Behavior (Hebb, 1949/2002), established the concepts of synaptic change and cell assemblies. This was the basis for the prominent Hebbian theory, which has been summarized as “cells that fire together wire together.” Hebb’s research was diverse and involved a wide range of ideas (Brown, 2007b) and his research on intelligence A and B was used by Cattell to develop his theories of fluid and crystallized intelligence (Brown, 2016). Hebb was Professor Emeritus at Dalhousie University from 1977 to 1985 (Figure 5) and, after he died, a Hebb Memorial Lecture was established. I began to write the introductions to these lectures and this resulted in having Hebb’s book republished (Hebb, 1949/2002) and writing articles on his life and work (Brown and Milner, 2003; Brown, 2007b). To research the life of Donald Hebb took me to the archives at Dalhousie University, McGill University, the University of Chicago, Harvard University and the archives of the History of Psychology in Akron Ohio, as well as to many small archives and museums. Hebb’s family provided me with materials and I met his brother, Andrew (aged 98) who told me about their childhood and schooling. I scoured the National Archives of Canada in Ottawa for information about Hebb and met with his former students and colleagues and I am not finished yet; indeed, I have my own “Quest for Corvo” (Symons, 1934/1966) in trying to write a biography of Donald Hebb.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. At the Hebb “cottage” in Chester Basin, Nova Scotia. Hebb’s 80th birthday 1984. Left to right: Richard Brown, Donald Hebb, Raymond Klein and his daughter [Photo taken by John Fentress].

Pavlov’s Physiology Labs

In May 2017, the FENS History committee participated in the 100th anniversary of the Russian Physiological Society in Saint Petersburg Russia. During our visit, we not only presented lectures but also visited the laboratories used by Pavlov. The highlights of the trip were our visits to the lab at Koltushi and to Pavlov’s apartment, where we had tea in Pavlov’s dining room (Brown et al., 2017).

These examples illustrate some of the excitement of the search for the history of neuroscience. It is interesting because you discover things that you would never have expected. It is illuminating as it shows the ideas and methods used to make some of the basic discoveries in neuroscience. Finally, it is enlightening to view the lives of famous neuroscientists through their writings, letters and photographs and through the eyes of their students and families. In the search for the history of neuroscience, you have the opportunities to meet interesting people, hear fascinating stories and to travel to far away places. Finally, the study of the history of science helps you to make sense of your own research.

Summary and Conclusions

The history of neuroscience can be important for students and researchers who can use insights from the history of science to illuminate their work. By methodologically investigating historical data, models, hypotheses and experiments, alternatives to contemporary theories can be contemplated. Lessons from the history of neuroscience also reveal the cultural context and social responsibility of those investigating the brain. Popular ideas about the brain influence the direction that neuroscientists take in their research. Especially in the past half-century, new discoveries in neuroscience have had a widespread popular appeal. Nerves and brain function have become a powerful analogy in spheres of thought far removed from neuroscience. In order to avoid repeating prejudices, neuroscientists can take a history of science approach to their discipline.

Because history points out the flaws and problems with past research, it has been suggested that it be “X-rated,” as the focus has moved away from the search for the “objective truths” in history to the more subjective “memory” approach to history, with all its errors, omissions and flaws. Since all research relies on history, and each research project has its own history, neuroscientists rely on their historical records to demonstrate their research integrity. Without history, whether in the form of actual physical objects, written documents, or personal reminiscences, neuroscientists have little context for their contemporary work. Historical approaches can be integrated into research and teaching in neuroscience and many neuroscientists will find interest and pleasure in the study of the history of neuroscience.

Author Contributions

RB wrote the manuscript.

Funding

Publication of this article was supported, in part, by grant A7441 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) of Canada.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank my students, Brianna Aird and Jessica Pelley for their assistance in the preparation of previous drafts of this article, and Aimee Wong for her help in editing the article.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://history.medsci.ox.ac.uk/
  2. ^ https://www.fens.org/Outreach/History/EBM/
  3. ^ https://faculty.washington.edu/chudler/hist.html
  4. ^ https://www.data.cam.ac.uk/data-management-guide/organising-your-data
  5. ^ https://phenome.jax.org
  6. ^ http://retractionwatch.com
  7. ^ https://www.fens.org/Outreach/History/EBM/
  8. ^ http://history.medsci.ox.ac.uk
  9. ^ https://history.medsci.ox.ac.uk/

References

Adams, J. (2011). “Writing in neurosceicne”: a course designed for neuroscience undergraduate students. J. Undergrad. Neurosci. Educ. 10, A50–A57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Akil, H., Martone, M. E., and Van Essen, D. C. (2011). Challenges and opportunities in mining neuroscience data. Science 331, 708–712. doi: 10.1126/science.1199305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albrecht-Carrie, R. (1951). Of science, its history and the teaching thereof. Sci. Mon. 73, 16–24.

Google Scholar

Armstrong, R. A. (2013). What causes Alzheimer’s disease? Folia Neuropathol. 51, 169–188. doi: 10.5114/fn.2013.37702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, K., and Söderqvist, T. (2011). Medical instruments in museums: immediate impressions and historical meanings. Isis 102, 718–729. doi: 10.1086/663613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aubry, J. M., Dayer, A., Perroud, N., Piguet, C., Nallet, A., and Favre, S. (2014). Retraction of “Childhood maltreatment and methylation of the glucocorticoid receptor gene NR3C1 in bipolar disorder”. Br. J. Psychiatry 205, 164. doi: 10.1192/bjp.205.2.164a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, H. E. (1920). The historian and the history of science. Sci. Mon. 11, 112–126.

Google Scholar

Bickle, J. (2016). Revolutions in neuroscience: tool development. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:24. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birke, L. I. A., and Sadler, D. (1984). Scent-marking behaviour in response to conspecific odours by the rat, Rattus norvegicus. Anim. Behav. 32, 493–500. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(84)80286-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloor, D., Barnes, B., and Henry, J. (1996). Scientific Knowledge: A Sociological Analysis. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Bliquez, L. J. (1982). The Tools of Asclepius: the surgical gear of the Greeks and Romans. Vet. Surg. 11, 150–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-950x.1982.tb00692.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehlich, W. (Eds). (1990). The Letters of Sigmund Freud to Eduard Silberstein, 1871–1881. Translated by Jeffrey M. Masson. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Borghi, J. A., and Van Gulick, A. E. (2018). Data management and sharing in neuroimaging: practices and perceptions of MRI researchers. PLoS One 13:e0200562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainard, J. (2018). Rethinking retractions. Science 362, 390–393. doi: 10.1126/science.362.6413.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breasted, J. H. (1930). The Edwin Smith Surgical Papyrus. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Brown, R. E., Molnár, Z., Filaretova, L., Ostrovskiy, M. A., Piccolino, M., and Lorusso, L. (2017). The 100th anniversary of the Russian Pavlov Physiological Society. Physiology 32, 402–407. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00023.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E. (1978). Hormonal control of odor preferences and urine-marking in male and female rats. Physiol. Behav. 20, 21–24. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90197-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E. (1991). Effects of rearing condition, gender, and sexual experience on odor preferences and urine-marking in Long-Evans rats. Anim. Learn. Behav. 19, 18–28. doi: 10.3758/bf03197856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E. (2007a). Alfred McCoy, Hebb, the CIA and torture. J. Hist. Behav. Sci. 43, 205–213. doi: 10.1002/jhbs.20225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E. (2007b). The life and work of Donald Olding Hebb, Canada’s greatest psychologist. Proc. N. S. Inst. Sci. 44, 1–25. doi: 10.15273/pnsis.v44i1.3880

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E. (2014). “Leonardo Da Vinci and the search for the anatomical basis of the soul,” in Program No. 21.04SA/UU24. 2014 Neuroscience Meeting Planner (Washington DC: Society for Neuroscience).

Brown, R. E. (2016). Hebb and Cattell: the genesis of the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:606. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E., and Milner, P. M. (2003). The legacy of Donald O. Hebb: more than the Hebb synapse. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 1013–1019. doi: 10.1038/nrn1257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brush, S. G. (1974). Should the history of neuroscience be rated X?: the way scientists behave (according to historians) might not be a good model for students. Science 183, 1164–1172. doi: 10.1126/science.183.4130.1164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bzdok, D., and Yeo, B. T. T. (2017). Inference in the age of big data: future perspectives on neuroscience. Neuroimage 155, 549–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajal, S. R. (1937/1989). Recollections of My Life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Carr, E. H. (1961). What is History? Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.

Google Scholar

Casper, S. T. (2014). History and neuroscience: an integrative legacy. Isis 105, 123–132. doi: 10.1086/675554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceccarelli, L. (2002). A scientific rhetoric [Review of Communicating Science by Alan G. Gross, Joseph E. Harmon and Michael Reidy]. Science 298:757. doi: 10.1126/science.1076841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, B., and Molnár, Z. (2015). Practical neuroanatomy teaching in the 21st century. Ann. Neurol. 77, 911–916. doi: 10.1002/ana.24405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, K. H., Lim, E., Samwald, M., Chen, H., Marenco, L., Holford, M. E., et al. (2009). Approaches to neuroscience data integration. Brief. Bioinform. 10, 345–353. doi: 10.1093/bib/bbp029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooter, R. (2014). Neural veils and the will to historical critique: why historians of science need to take the Neuro-turn seriously. Isis 105, 145–154. doi: 10.1086/675556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cubinkova, V., Valachova, B., Uhrinova, I., Brezovakova, V., Smolek, T., Jadhav, S., et al. (2018). Alternative hypotheses related to Alzheimer’s disease. Bratisl. Lek. Listy 119, 210–216. doi: 10.4149/BLL_2018_039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cubitt, G. (2007). History and Memory. Manchester: Manchester University Press.

Google Scholar

Dawkins, R. (2013). An Appetite for Wonder: The Making of A Scientist. A Memoir. London: Bantam.

Google Scholar

Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M. T., and Cabanis, E. A. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain 130, 1432–1441. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. C., and Gibson, W. C. (1979). Sherrington: His Life and Thought. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Edelman, G. M. (2006). Second Nature: Brain Science and Human Knowledge. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Edwards, D. (2010). The Lab: Creativity and Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Elton, G. R. (1967). The Practice of History. New York, NY: Thomas Y. Crowell.

Falk, R., and Falk, R. (2007). “Why should scientists become historians?,” in Positioning the History of Science, eds K. Gavroglu and J. Rean (New York, NY: Springer), 43–48.

Google Scholar

Finger, S. (1994). The Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations Into Brain Function. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Finger, S. (2000). Minds Behind the Brain: A History of the Pioneers and Their Discoveries. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Fobbs, A. J. Jr., and Johnson, J. I. (2011). Brain collections at the national museum of health and medicine. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1225, E20–E29. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06036.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazzetto, G. (2011). How to bridge neuroscience, society, and culture. PLoS Biol. 9:e1001178. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazzetto, G., and Anker, S. (2009). Neuroculture. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 815–821. doi: 10.1038/nrn2736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, J. (2015). Open source tools for large-scale neuroscience. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 32, 156–163. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galliano, E., Mazzarello, P., and D’Angelo, E. (2010). Discovery and rediscoveries of Golgi cells. J. Physiol. 588, 3639–3655. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.189605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Lopez, P. (2012). Sculpting the brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:5. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilmore, R. O., Diaz, M. T., Wyble, B. A., and Yarkoni, T. (2017). Progress toward openness, transparency, and reproducibility in cognitive neuroscience. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1396, 5–18. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glickstein, M. (2014). Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Godfrey-Smith, P. (2003). Theory and Reality: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Goldenring, J. R. (2010). Innocence and due diligence: managing unfounded allegations of scientific misconduct. Acad. Med. 85, 527–530. doi: 10.1097/acm.0b013e3181cd4c7a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodfield, J. (1981). An Imagined World: A Story of Scientific Discovery. London: Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Gorgolewski, K. J., Auer, T., Calhoun, V. D., Craddock, R. C., Das, S., Duff, E. P., et al. (2016). The brain imaging data structure, a format for organizing and describing outputs of neuroimaging experiments. Sci. Data 3:160044. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2016.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorgolewski, K. J., and Poldrack, R. A. (2016). A practical guide for improving transparency and reproducibility in neuroimaging research. PLoS Biol. 14:e1002506. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, T. H. (1956). Why study the history of science? Improv. Coll. Univ. Teach. 4, 79–82. doi: 10.1080/00193089.1956.10533944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregory, K., Khalsa, S. J., Michener, W. K., Psomopoulos, F. E., de Waard, A., and Wu, M. (2018). Eleven quick tips for finding research data. PLoS Comput Biol. 14:e1006038. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grisham, W., Lom, B., Lanyon, L., and Ramos, R. L. (2016). Proposed training to meet challenges of large- scale data in neuroscience. Front. Neuroinform. 10:28. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2016.00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grisham, W., Schottler, N. A., Valli-Marill, J., Beck, L., and Beatty, J. (2010). Teaching bioinformatics and neuroinformatics by using free web-based tools. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 9, 98–107. doi: 10.1187/cbe.09-11-0079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, C. G. (2009). Three before their time: neuroscientists whose ideas were ignored by their contemporaries. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 321–334. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1481-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, A. G., Harmon, J. E., and Reidy, M. (2002). Communicating Science. The Scientific Article From the 17th Century to the Present. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Guillery, R. W. (2005). Observations of synaptic strutures: origins of the neuron doctrine and its current status. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1281–1307. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunsalus, C. K., and Robinson, A. D. (2018). Nine pitfalls of research misconduct. Nature 557, 297–299. doi: 10.1038/d41586-018-05145-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, J. (2017). The discovery of Alzheimer-causing mutations in the APP gene and the formulation of the “amyloid cascade hypothesis”. FEBS J. 284, 1040–1044. doi: 10.1111/febs.14004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, M. (1999). Hippocrates: On Head Wounds. Translation and Commentary by Maury Hanson CMG I 4,1. Berlin: Corpvs Medicorvm Graecorvm.

Google Scholar

Hebb, D. O. (1949/2002). The Organization of Behaviour: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY: Wiley. [Reprinted 2002 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey]

Hebb, D. O. (1980). “D. O. Hebb,” in A History of Psychology in Autobiography, Vol VII, ed. G. Lindzey (San Francisco: WH Freeman), 273–309.

Google Scholar

Hellman, C. D. (1958). George Sarton, historian of science and new humanist. Science 128, 641–644. doi: 10.1126/science.128.3325.641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, D. J., Reynolds, R. M., and Hardy, D. B. (2017). Developmental origins of health and disease: current knowledge and potential mechanisms. Nutr. Rev. 75, 951–970. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nux053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingram, C. (2016). How and why you should manage your research data: a guide for researchers: an introduction to engaging with research data management processes. Available online at: https://www.jisc.ac.uk/guides/how-and-why-you-should-manage-your-research-data# [Accessed March 15, 2019].

Ioannidis, J. P. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2:e124. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannidis, J. P. (2014). How to make more published research true. PLoS Med. 11:e1001747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iqbal, S. A., Wallach, J. D., Khoury, M. J., Schully, S. D., and Ioannidis, J. P. (2016). Reproducible research practices and transparency across the biomedical literature. PLoS Biol. 14:e1002333. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N. (2010). Comparative brain collections are an indispensable resource for evolutionary neurobiology. Brain Behav. Evol. 76, 87–88. doi: 10.1159/000320214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N. (2011). The importance of scientific collecting and natural history museums for comparative neuroanatomy. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1225, E1–E19. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05977.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jerison, H. J. (1977). Should phrenology be rediscovered? Curr. Anthropol. 18, 744–746. doi: 10.1086/201981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, O. P., Alfaro-Almagro, F., and Jbabdi, S. (2018). An empirical, 21st century evaluation of phrenology. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/243089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jimerson, R. C. (2003). Archives and memory. OCLC Syst. Serv. 19, 89–95. doi: 10.1108/10650750310490289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Južnič, S. (2016). History of history of physics. Acta Balt. Hist. Philos. Sci. 4, 5–26. doi: 10.11590/abhps.2016.2.01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kallestinova, E. D. (2011). How to write your first research paper. Yale J. Biol. Med. 84, 181–190.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keverne, E. B., Pfaff, D. W., and Tabansky, I. (2015). Epigenetic changes in the developing brain: effects on behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, 6789–6795. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1501482112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keverne, E. B. (2014). Significance of epigenetics for understanding brain development, brain evolution and behaviour. Neuroscience 264, 207–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleywegt, G. J., Velankar, S., and Patwardhan, A. (2018). Structural biology data archiving—where we are and what lies ahead. FEBS Lett. 592, 2153–2167. doi: 10.1002/1873-3468.13086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruger, L., and Otis, T. S. (2007). Whither withered Golgi? A retrospective evaluation of reticularist and synaptic constructs. Brain Res. Bull. 72, 201–207. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, T. (1962). On the Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Lee, J. L. C., Nader, K., and Schiller, D. (2017). An update on memory reconsolidation updating. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 531–545. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindberg, D. C. (2007). The Beginnings of Western Science. The European Scientific Tradition in Philosophical, Religious and Institutional Context, Prehistory to A.D. 1450. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Loftus, E. (2003). Our changeable memories: legal and practical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 231–234. doi: 10.1038/nrn1054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loftus, E. F., and Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of auto-mobile destruction: an example of the interaction between language and memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 13, 585–589. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(74)80011-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoLordo, V. M., and Ross, R. T. (1990). Retraction of Ross and LoLordo findings concerning blocking in serial feature-positive discriminations. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 16, 402–406. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.16.4.402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Muñoz, F., and Alamo, C. (2009). Historical evolution of the neurotransmission concept. J. Neural Transm. 116, 515–533. doi: 10.1007/s00702-009-0213-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorusso, L., Brown, R. E., Motta, S., Gasparello, A., Barbara, J.-G., Molnár, Z., et al. (2018). Neuroscience without borders: preserving the history of neuroscience. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2099–2109. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maienschein, J. (2000). Why study history for science? Biol. Philos. 15, 339–348. doi: 10.1023/a:1006733114136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maienschein, J., Laubichler, M., and Loettgers, A. (2008). How can history of science matter to scientists? Isis 99, 341–349. doi: 10.1086/588692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manger, P. R. (2011). Collectibles and collections for comparative and evolutionary neurobiological research in Africa. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1225, E85–E93. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05948.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makin, S. (2018). The amyloid hypothesis on trial. Nature 559, S4–S7. doi: 10.1038/d41586-018-05719-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matiasz, N. J., Wood, J., Doshi, P., Speier, W., Beckemeyer, B., Wang, W., et al. (2018). ResearchMaps.org for integrating and planning research. PLoS One 13:e0195271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattern, S. P. (2013). The Prince of Medicine. Galen and the Roman Empire. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Mazzarello, P. (2010). Golgi. A Biography of the Founder of Modern Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Translated by Aldo Badiani and Henry A. Buchtel]

McNeill, W. (1985). Why study history? American historical association. Available online at: https://www.historians.org/about-aha-and-membership/aha-history-and-archives/historical-archives/why-study-history [Accessed March 15, 2019].

Google Scholar

Merton, R. (1962). The Sociology of Science: Theoretical and Empirical Investigations. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Meulders, M. (2010). Helmholtz. From Enlightenment to Neurosceince. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Translated and edited by Laurence Garey]

Google Scholar

Molnár, Z., and Brown, R. E. (2010). Insights into the life of Sir Charles Sherrington. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 429–436. doi: 10.1038/nrn2835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montagu, M. F. A. (1955). Vesalius and the Galenists. Sci. Mon. 80, 230–239.

Google Scholar

Mora, F. (2015). “Neuroculture: a new cultural revolution?” in Art, Aesthetics and the Brain, eds J. P. Huston, M. Nadal, F. Mora, L. F. Agnati and C. J. C. Conde (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 3–18.

Google Scholar

Nature Publishing Group. (2017). Announcement: toward greater reproducibility for life-sciences research in Nature. Nature 546:8. doi: 10.1038/546008a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nehls, M. (2016). Unified theory of Alzheimer’s disease (UTAD): implications for prevention and curative therapy. J. Mol. Psychiatry 4:3. doi: 10.1186/s40303-016-0018-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neill, U. S. (2007). How to write a scientific masterpiece. J. Clin. Invest. 117, 3599–3602. doi: 10.1172/jci34288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nutton, V. (2004). Ancient Medicine. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

O’Leary, T. P., Hussin, A. T., Gunn, R. K., and Brown, R. E. (2018). Locomotor activity, emotionality, sensori-motor gating, learning and memory in the APPswe/PS1dE9 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res. Bull. 140, 347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2018.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panourias, I. G., Skiadas, P. K., Sakas, D. E., and Marketos, S. G. (2005). Hippocrates: a pioneer in the treatment of head injuries. Neurosurgery 57, 181–189. doi: 10.1227/01.neu.0000163416.66374.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, D. (2018). Kuhnian revolutions in neuroscience: the role of tool development. Biol. Philos. 33:17. doi: 10.1007/s10539-018-9628-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perroud, N., Dayer, A., Piguet, C., Nallet, A., Favre, S., Malafosse, A., et al. (2014). Childhood maltreatment and methylation of the glucocorticoid receptor gene NR3C1 in bipolar disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 204, 30–35. [Retracted in Br. J. Psychiatry 2014, 205 (2): 164]. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.120055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccolino, M. (1998). Animal electricity and the birth of electrophysiology: the legacy of Luigi Galvani. Brain Res. Bull. 46, 381–407. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(98)00026-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaxco, K. W. (2010). The art of writing science. Protein Sci. 19, 2261–2266. doi: 10.1002/pro.514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poole, R. (2008). Memory, history and the claims of the past. Mem. Stud. 1, 149–166. doi: 10.1177/1750698007088383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redish, A. D., Kummerfeld, E., Morris, R. L., and Love, A. C. (2018). Reproducibility failures are essential to scientific enquiry. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 115, 5042–5046. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1806370115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera-Orraca, L. (2009). Are museums sites of memory? New School Psychol. Bull. 6, 32–37.

Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (2012). Neuroculture: On the Implications of Brain Science. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Root-Bernstein, R. S. (1988). Setting the stage for discovery. Breakthroughs depend on more than luck. Sciences, 28, 26–34.

Google Scholar

Roth, A., Kyzar, E., Cachat, J., Stewart, A. M., Green, J., Gaikwad, S., et al. (2013). Potential translational targets revealed by linking mouse grooming behavioral phenotypes to gene expression using public databases. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 312–325. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuel, R. (1992). Reading the signs: II. Fact-grubbers and mind-readers. Hist. Workshop J. 33, 220–251. doi: 10.1093/hwj/33.1.220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saper, C. B. (2015). Academic publishing, part III: how to write a research paper (so that it will be accepted) in a high-quality journal. Ann. Neurol. 77, 8–12. doi: 10.1002/ana.24317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarton, G. (1918). The teaching of the history of science. Sci. Mon. 7, 193–211.

Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L. (1996). Searching for Memory. The Brain, The Mind, and The Past. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Schellinck, H. M., Cyr, D. P. and Brown, R.E. (2010). How many ways can mouse behavioral experiments go wrong? Confounding variables in mouse models of neurodegenerative diseases and how to control them. Adv. Study Behav. 41, 255–366. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(10)41007-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwerdtfeger, L. A. (2018). Spirals of science. Science 362:1318. doi: 10.1126/science.362.6420.1318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwerdtfeger, L. A., and Tobet, S. A. (2018). From organotypic culture to body-on-a-chip: a neuroendocrine perspective. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2018:e12650. doi: 10.1111/jne.12650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepherd, G. M. (1991/2015). Foundations of the Neuron Doctrine. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Shepherd, G. M. (2010). Creating Modern Neuroscience: The Revolutionary 1950s. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Shepherd, G. M., Greer, C. A., Mazzarello, P., and Sassoè-Pognettoc, M. (2011). The first images of nerve cells: Golgi on the olfactory bulb 1875. Brain Res. Rev. 66, 92–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, M. K. (2015). Environmental epigenetics and a unified theory of the molecular aspects of evolution: a neo-Lamarckian concept that facilitates neo-Darwinian evolution. Genome Biol. Evol. 7, 1296–1302. doi: 10.1093/gbe/evv073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Söderqvist, T. (2002). Neurobiographies: writing lives in the history of neurology and the neurosciences. J. Hist. Neurosci. 11, 38–48. doi: 10.1076/jhin.11.1.38.9099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Söderqvist, T. (2007). “A new look at the genre of scientific biography,” in The History and Poetics of Scientific Biography, ed. T. Söderqvist (Cornwall: Ashgate), 1–15.

Squire, L. S. (Eds). (1996–2018). The History of Neuroscience in Autobiography, 10 Volumes. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience.

Google Scholar

Star, S. L. (1983). Simplification in Scientific Work: an example from neuroscience research. Soc. Stud. Sci. 13, 205–228. doi: 10.1177/030631283013002002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steedman, C. (1980). The space of memory: in an archive. Hist. Hum. Sci. 11, 65–83. doi: 10.1177/095269519801100405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, K. (2018). The developing world of DOHaD. J. Dev. Orig. Health Dis. 9, 266–269. doi: 10.1017/s2040174417000691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, L. (2015). Neuroanatomical Terminology: A Lexicon of Classical Origins and Historical Foundations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Symons, A. J. A. (1934/1966). The Quest for Corvo. London: Penguin books.

Todes, D. P. (2014). Ivan Pavlov. A Russian Life in Science. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Tsermoulas, G., Aidonis, A., and Flint, G. (2014). The skull of Chios: trepanation in Hippocratic medicine. J. Neurosurg. 121, 328–332. doi: 10.3171/2014.4.jns131886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tullo, E. (2010). Trepanation and Roman medicine: a comparison of osteoarchaeological remains, material culture and written texts. J. R. Coll. Physicians Edinb. 40, 165–171. doi: 10.4997/jrcpe.2010.215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uher, R., and Weaver, I. C. (2014a). Epigenetic traces of childhood maltreatment in peripheral blood: a new strategy to explore gene-environment interactions. Br. J. Psychiatry 204, 3–5. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.127209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uher, R., and Weaver, I. C. (2014b). Retraction. Br. J. Psychiatry 205:249. doi: 10.1192/bjp.205.3.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenstein, E. S. (2002). The discovery of chemical neurotransmitters. Brain Cogn. 49, 73–95. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vesalius, A. (1543). De Humani Corporis Fabrica Libri Septum. Basil: Karger. [Translated and Annotated by D. H. Garrison and M. H. Hast. 2014. The Fabric of the Human Body]

Google Scholar

Vogelstein, J. T., Perlman, E., Falk, B., Baden, A., Gray Roncal, W., Chandrashekhar, V., et al. (2018). A community-developed open-source computational system for big neuro data. Nat. Methods 15, 846–847. doi: 10.1038/s41592-018-0181-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wachtel, N. (1986). Memory and history. Hist. Anthropol. 2, 207–224. doi: 10.1080/02757206.1986.9960766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, J. (1968). The Double Helix: A Personal Account of the Discovery of the Structure of DNA. New York, NY: Atheneum Press.

Google Scholar

Wiener, M., Sommer, F. T., Ives, Z. G., Poldrack, R. A., and Litt, B. (2016). Enabling an open data ecosystem for the neurosciences. Neuron 92, 617–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Bai, M., Xi, Y., Hao, J., Zhang, Z., Su, C., et al. (2012). Multiple inflammatory pathways are involved in the development and progression of cognitive deficits in APPswe/PS1dE9 mice. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2661–2677. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Bai, M., Xi, Y., Hao, J., Zhang, Z., Su, C., et al. (2016). Retraction notice to “Multiple inflammatory pathways are involved in the development and progression of cognitive deficits in APPswe/PS1dE9 mice” [Neurobiol. Aging 33 (2012) 2661-2677]. Neurobiol. Aging 38:219. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A., Etz, A., Lucas, R. E., and Donnellan, B. (2018). Making replication mainstream. Behav. Brain Sci. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X17001972 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: history, research, archives, libraries, museums

Citation: Brown RE (2019) Why Study the History of Neuroscience? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:82. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00082

Received: 28 November 2018; Accepted: 05 April 2019;
Published: 22 May 2019.

Edited by:

Chrystalina A. Antoniades, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Lazaros C. Triarhou, University of Macedonia, Greece
Joshua C. Brumberg, Queens College (CUNY), United States

Copyright © 2019 Brown. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Richard E. Brown rebrown@dal.ca orcid.org/0000-0002-4762-8175

Download