Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 05 December 2019
Sec. Behavioral Endocrinology
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00246

Lower Digit Ratio (2D:4D) Indicative of Excess Prenatal Androgen Is Associated With Increased Sociability and Greater Social Capital

Verena N. Buchholz* Christiane Mühle Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors Johannes Kornhuber Bernd Lenz
  • Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nürnberg (FAU), Erlangen, Germany

Positive social interactions are crucial for human well-being. Elevated prenatal exposure to testosterone as indicated by a low second-to-fourth finger length ratio (2D:4D) relates to more aggressive/hostile behavior in men of low 2D:4D, especially in challenging situations. How much people enjoy interacting with others is determined by the personality trait sociability. Given its role in approach and avoidance behavior, sociability might also be influenced by prenatal sex hormones, but studies are inconclusive so far. Here, we investigated the association between 2D:4D and the personality trait sociability complemented by personal social capital and personal social network size, in a population-based cohort of 4998 men. Lower 2D:4D correlated significantly with higher trait sociability, bigger personal social capital, and larger personal social network size. These effects were consistent across both hands separately and their mean value. Furthermore, both factors of sociability (1) liking party and company of friends and (2) isolation intolerance, correlated significantly with the prenatal testosterone marker. The exploratory analysis revealed no link between 2D:4D and responses to the personality trait aggression items or items of anti-social-personality disorder. Our data suggest that prenatal androgen exposure organizes the brain with lasting effects on social behavior.

Introduction

During the early prenatal window, androgens and estrogens influence the development with long-lasting effects on the structure and composition of the body and on behavior. Prenatal stress relates to increased androgen load; accordingly, intervention programs to reduce maternal stress during pregnancy are being developed (Lenz et al., 2018b). Animal models (mice, sheep) have shown permanent organizational effects of prenatal testosterone on the brain (Brown et al., 2015; Huber et al., 2018). These early effects also contribute to sex differences in adult behavior.

Aggression and social relationships are subject to gender dimorphisms. In comparison to men, women show less direct aggressive behavior (Archer, 2004), but in conduct disorder, women show hurtful manipulation of relationships (relational aggression) more often than men (Ackermann et al., 2019). Men less often report having a close confident other than the spouse (Antonucci, 1994), spend less time involved in responding to requests from others (Kessler et al., 1985; Troisi, 2001) and on online social networks (Bouna-Pyrrou et al., 2015, 2018), have a smaller risk to use them pathologically (Bouna-Pyrrou et al., 2015), and are less often the target of online communication (Griffiths et al., 2004). The sex differences may suggest that prenatal exposure to androgens influences aggressive and social behaviors in adulthood. Due to ethical reasons and the long time interval between the prenatal window and adulthood, it is hardly possible to directly investigate the effects of intrauterine sex hormones. Hence, biomarkers have been established. The second-to-fourth finger length ratio (2D:4D) is widely used to study prenatal sex hormone exposure. Reinforced prenatal androgen signaling causes lower 2D:4D in mice (Zheng and Cohn, 2011) and indirect effects of such organizational properties have been also found in humans (Manning et al., 2014), for critical review see Berenbaum et al. (2009), Del Giudice et al. (2018). E.g., human maternal plasma testosterone during pregnancy shows a negative correlation with new borns’ digit ratio in both sexes (Ventura et al., 2013), amniotic fluid testosterone is negatively related to 2 year olds’ 2D:4D (Lutchmaya et al., 2004), and females with exposure to excessive prenatal testosterone levels due to congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) have lower 2D:4D values than normal controls (Brown et al., 2002; Buck et al., 2003). Hence, lower 2D:4D is indicative of higher prenatal androgen load in humans.

A meta-analysis reported that lower 2D:4D relates to more aggression in men (Hönekopp and Schuster, 2010), although these effects have been found to be small. Furthermore, situational factors, and adult hormone levels play a moderating role (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011). From an evolutionary perspective, one could also expect that social behaviors involving approach and bonding might be related to biological factors such as prenatal sex hormone exposure. Studies investigating social behavior and prenatal testosterone exposure have been conducted under varying contexts and with the use of different methods, ranging from economic games to observation of interactions. The findings have been inconsistent, perhaps due to the complexity of human behavior and its interplay with environmental factors (Millet and Buehler, 2017). Indeed, contextual factors such as the presence of aggressive (Kilduff et al., 2013) or sexual cues (Van den Bergh and Dewitte, 2006), adult hormone levels (Millet and Dewitte, 2008; van Honk et al., 2012; Manning et al., 2014; Portnoy et al., 2015; Millet and Buehler, 2017), cognitive reflection (Millet and Aydinli, 2019), and time-pressure (Bird et al., 2019) moderate the relationship between 2D:4D and prosocial behavior in economic games. Furthermore, the relationships might differ across sex (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011). However, the evidence seems to be more consistent at least at the level of achievements in adults. Within men, more prenatal androgen (lower 2D:4D) is associated with higher academic grade (Nye et al., 2017; Tektas et al., 2019), larger reproductive success (Manning et al., 2000), and higher trading outcome in financial traders (Coates et al., 2009). Thus, in contrast to what one might expect due to the above reported sex differences, men with lower (more masculine) 2D:4D perform better in tasks that require networking or bonding. Accordingly, in men, lower 2D:4D has been related to more fairness (Millet and Dewitte, 2006), stronger cognitive reflection (Bosch-Domenèch et al., 2014), and higher betweenness centrality, i.e., they connect separated parts of the social structure (Kovářík et al., 2017). Moreover, males with lower 2D:4D show more courtship behavior in social interactions with women (Roney and Maestripieri, 2004).

How much people enjoy interacting with others or need to be in company (two factors of sociability) and how many people they know to rely on (social capital) are important determinants of human well-being and health. For example, a low social capital has been associated with negative health outcomes (Murayama et al., 2012) including depression, pain, and psychosomatic symptoms (Åslund et al., 2010). Thus, associations between 2D:4D and health further highlight the importance to understand the role of prenatal androgen exposure in adult social behavior. For example, in males, lower 2D:4D has been associated with lower anxiety (Evardone and Alexander, 2009), a higher risk for conduct problems during childhood (Eichler et al., 2018), addictive and substance use disorders (Kornhuber et al., 2011, 2013; Canan et al., 2017, 2019; Lenz et al., 2017, 2018a, 2019a; Siegmann et al., 2019), suicide (Lenz et al., 2016, 2019b), and reduced life expectancy in adulthood (Lenz and Kornhuber, 2018).

Given the complexity of behavior in experimental tasks or hypothetical trading situations, relatively stable indicators of social behavior, like the personality trait sociability, personal social capital, and personal social network size provide a suitable approach to investigate the link between social behavior and organizational effects of prenatal androgens. Furthermore, the Alternative Five Model (measuring sociability as one of five factors) has been established for traits with a strong biological-evolutionary basis and increases the comparability of our results with animal models (Zuckerman et al., 1993).

Here, we tested whether 2D:4D relates to sociability, personal social capital, and personal social network size in a large population based cohort of 4998 young males. We also explored whether 2D:4D is associated with aggression and anti-social personality characteristics.

Materials and Methods

Study Sample

The data analyzed here originate in the third survey wave of the longitudinal Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors (C-SURF)1. From 2010 to 2012, 7556 young males, who attended their mandatory recruitment for the Swiss army, gave written informed consent and 5987 participated in the first wave. Data for this study were derived from the third wave which has been conducted between April 2016 and March 2018 and which has included 5516 males (see2 for Questionnaire No. 3).

Behavioral Phenotyping

To measure sociability, we used the subscale sociability of the Alternative Five Factor Model (Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionaire, ZKPQ-50-cc) (Zuckerman et al., 1993) questionnaire, consisting of 10 binary items and its summation score (Aluja et al., 2006). The scale was further divided in the two subscales representing (1) liking lively parties and friends and (2) intolerance of social isolation. Personal social capital with the subscales bridging and bonding was quantified by an adaptation of the Personal Social Capital Scale (Archuleta and Miller, 2011; Chang and Zhu, 2012; Wang et al., 2014) with only the 5 most relevant items per subscale selected in C-SURF and a Likert Scale 1–5 to respond. Bonding social capital refers to how well a person is embedded within their various networks of different types of people (e.g., family members, friends, and former colleagues), and bridging social capital refers to how well a person is embedded within different types of social organizations. Personal social network size was estimated in C-SURF by two items referring to social network size from the Personal Social Capital Scale (Archuleta and Miller, 2011; Wang et al., 2014). The first item refers to perceived number of friends (from the bonding capital subscale) and the second to the perceived number of cultural, recreational, and leisure groups/organizations in the subject’s community (from the bridging capital subscale).

Aggression was quantified using the 10 items scale of the ZKPQ-50-cc (Aluja et al., 2006). The score on the Anti-Social Personality Disorder scale was probed with items from the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.) with ASSIST-WHO (Sheehan et al., 1998; Hergueta et al., 2015).

2D:4D

The participants were instructed to document the lengths of their second and fourth fingers in millimeters separately for their right and left hands (see2, Questionnaire No. 3 ID: J18) similar to the methods described by Reimers (2007) and Lenz et al. (2018a). The instruction was “Hold your left hand in front of you. Look at where your index finger joins the palm of your hand. Find the bottom crease. Go to the middle of this crease. Put the 0 of your ruler exactly on the middle of the bottom crease (see 2a in the picture below). Make sure the ruler runs straight up the middle of your finger. Measure to the tip of your finger (not your nail see 2b in the picture) in millimeters.” Finger lengths under 10 mm or over 100 mm (Reimers, 2007) and, additionally, 2D:4D values outside of the 2.5 and 97.5 percentiles (Hell and Päßler, 2011; Lenz et al., 2018a) separately for the right and left hand were excluded. Subsequent, we calculated the mean of right-hand 2D:4D and left-hand 2D:4D (M2D:4D) which served as our primary predictor. Whereas some studies report that target traits are more strongly related to 2D:4D of the right hand (Manning et al., 1998; Hönekopp and Watson, 2010; Kornhuber et al., 2011; Masuya et al., 2015; Bilgic et al., 2016), other report stronger associations with 2D:4D of the left hand (Muller et al., 2012; Kornhuber et al., 2013; Hong et al., 2014; Lenz et al., 2017, 2019a). As far as we know, there is no reliable explanation for different associations of right- and left-hand 2D:4D with prenatal androgen load. There is also no support for superiority of either side in a meta-analysis on aggression (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011). Separate values for right-hand 2D:4D (R2D:4D), left-hand 2D:4D (L2D:4D), and the difference between R2D:4D and L2D:4D (2D:4Dr-l) were defined as exploratory predictors. Moreover, regarding quality control, we refer to a previous analysis of the same cohort (except for 9 patients with missing data on alcohol-related questions) which showed median values of 2D:4D similar to other studies (Lenz et al., 2019a).

Statistical Analyses

Continuous data are presented as the median and interquartile range (IQR) and nominal data as frequencies (FREQUENCIES function in SPSS). For missing data points, the corresponding study subjects were excluded from the specific analyses and the number of individuals included in these analyses is reported. Correlations were calculated using Spearman’s method, because normal distribution was rejected for all variables. We used the Mann–Whitney U test to compare independent groups. For two-sided tests, p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. All reported p-values are uncorrected for multiple comparisons. Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 21 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States) and Graph Pad Prism 5 (Graph Pad Software Inc., San Diego, CA, United States).

Results

Sample Characteristics

Due to missing values or eliminations resulting from quality control of R2D:4D and L2D:4D, 518 individuals were excluded from the statistical analyses. This resulted in a total cohort of 4998 study subjects and M2D:4D, L2D:4D, and R2D:4D sub-cohorts of 4778, 4898, and 4878 individuals. The total cohort was characterized as follows: age 25 years (IQR 25–26; N = 4998); body mass index 23.5 kg/m2 (IQR 21.8–25.5; N = 4990); 79.6% gainfully employed (N = 4997); 3.0% secondary education, 1.2% basic vocational education, 34.4% secondary vocational/technical education, 4.3% community college, 11.2% vocational high school, 11.8% high school, 23.4% bachelor (university), 6.1% master (university), 4.6% other (N = 4985); 82.9% single, 5.2% married, 0.1% divorced, 11.6% not married, not separated, not divorced but living together with my partner (e.g., in registered partnership), 0.1% married but separated, 0.1% widowed (N = 4989).

Trait Sociability

Lower M2D:4D (indicative of higher levels of prenatal androgen exposure) correlated with higher trait sociability (ρ = −0.043, N = 4755, p = 0.003), and both L2D:4D and R2D:4D correlated similarly with trait sociability (ρ = −0.045, N = 4875, p = 0.002; ρ = −0.032, N = 4855, p = 0.024). 2D:4Dr-l did not correlate with trait sociability (p > 0.05). As shown in Table 1, both subscales of sociability correlated significantly with 2D:4D.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Post hoc analysis Sociability: Spearman correlations at facet level.

Statistics for the M2D:4D differences for the 10 individual binary items (post hoc analysis) are shown in Supplementary Table S1. Specifically, the items “At parties, I enjoy mingling with many people whether I already know them or not.” and “I am a very sociable person.” were significantly associated with lower M2D:4D.

Personal Social Capital

Lower M2D:4D correlated with bigger personal social capital (ρ = −0.040, N = 5762, p = 0.005), and both L2D:4D and R2D:4D correlated similarly with bigger personal social capital (ρ = −0.036, N = 4882, p = 0.012; ρ = −0.013, N = 4861, p = 0.039). 2D:4Dr-l did not correlate with personal social capital (p > 0.05). Table 2 shows the results of the post hoc analysis on subscale level.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Post hoc analysis Personal social capital: Spearman correlations at subscale level.

Item level analysis revealed significant correlations with the items “interacting with people makes me feel like a part of a large community,” “the people I interact with would be good job references for me” and “if I needed an emergency loan, I know someone I can turn to”, for details see Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Post hoc analysis Personal social capital: Spearman correlations at item level.

Personal Social Network Size

2D:4D correlated negatively with the personal social network size (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The M2D:4D (A), L2D:4D (B), and R2D:4D (C) ratios [but not 2D:4Dr-l (D)] were negatively correlated with self-reports for the number of friends and the number of cultural, recreational, and leisure groups/organizations/associations in one’s community. 2D:4D, second-to-fourth-finger length ratio; primary predictor: M2D:4D, mean of R2D:4D and L2D:4D; exploratory predictors: L2D:4D, left-hand 2D:4D; R2D:4D, right-hand 2D:4D; 2D:4Dr-l, difference between R2D:4D and L2D:4D. P < 0.05 (uncorrected) in bold.

Aggression and Anti-social Personality

M2D:4D, L2D:4D, R2D:4D, or 2D:4Dr-l did not correlate with aggression or anti-social personality disorder score (p > 0.05, Supplementary Table S2).

Discussion

Here, we report that higher sociability and bigger personal social capital are correlated with lower 2D:4D in a population-based cohort of young Swiss men. Notably, both factors of sociability, liking lively parties and friends and intolerance of social isolation (Zuckerman et al., 1993), correlated independently with 2D:4D across both hands. Furthermore, we provide preliminary evidence for an association between bigger personal social network size and lower 2D:4D. These results suggest that, in men, higher prenatal androgen exposure improves sociability and leads to a bigger social capital and social network size in adulthood. Our observation is consistent with a study showing that prenatal testosterone as measured in amniotic fluid during 13–20 weeks of gestation is associated with approach behavior and reactivity to happy faces in brain reward areas of boys (Lombardo et al., 2012). The large sample size of nearly 5000 study participants analyzed here is a major strength of this project. It is limited by the 2D:4D self-measurement method which is related to reduced reliability in comparison to expert measured 2D:4D (Hönekopp and Watson, 2010).

Sociability involves the opioid system of the brain (Knowles et al., 1989; Kalin et al., 1995). In animal experiments, prenatal androgen receptor inhibition by flutamide down-regulates cerebral expression of the μ opioid receptor 1 in adulthood (Huber et al., 2018). In line with this association between prenatal sex hormone effects and opioid signaling, R2D:4D in men has been related to genetic polymorphisms in opioid receptors (Pearce et al., 2018). During social laughter – related to the sociability factor “party and friends” – endogenous opioids are released, and the depletion during social isolation motivates to seek company – related to the sociability factor “isolation intolerance” (Knowles et al., 1989; Kalin et al., 1995). The minor G-allele of the μ-opioid receptor 1 polymorphism rs1799971 is associated with more pleasure experienced in social situations (Troisi et al., 2011), and mice with this variant have increased motivation for non-aggressive social interactions and show less avoidance after social defeat (Briand et al., 2015). Taken together, prenatal androgen exposure may organize cerebral opioid signaling with behavioral effects on sociability. Future research should investigate how prenatal influences might interact with genetics to affect sociability.

We found lower 2D:4D to be associated with higher sociability. Our findings are in line with previous reports on higher betweenness centrality in men with lower 2D:4D, i.e., these subjects connect separated parts of the social structure (Kovářík et al., 2017). Furthermore, academic, reproductive, and trading success, all negatively correlated with 2D:4D (Manning et al., 2000; Coates et al., 2009; Nye et al., 2017; Tektas et al., 2019), have networking as an essential common mechanism to success. Hence, higher sociability might mediate the relationship between lower 2D:4D and successfulness in men.

In our adult cohort, we did not find any significant correlation between 2D:4D and aggression, which might be explained by the low precision due to the employed self-measurement technique and the fact that correlations of aggression and 2D:4D in adults are mainly found in challenging situations (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011) and in other situations are small at the best (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011).

By contrast, we found lower 2D:4D to be associated with higher sociability. At the first glance, our findings may contradict that lower 2D:4D (indicative of higher prenatal androgen exposure) relates to behavioral symptoms in boys (Williams et al., 2003; Eichler et al., 2018), which entails problems in social interaction. Aggression, fighting, and lacking obedience are characteristics of conduct disorder. However, the frontal lobe and cognitive reflection are still developing in children. Frontal lobe development and cognitive reflection inhibit aggressive outbursts and the shift of neural regulation to prefrontal areas takes place during puberty (Cubillo et al., 2012; Rubia et al., 2013; Tyborowska et al., 2016). In adulthood, cognitive reflection is higher in individuals with lower 2D:4D (Bosch-Domenèch et al., 2014) and probably explains the moderating role of sexual and aggressive cues on the relationship between 2D:4D and aggressive behavior (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011). Without a situationally triggered testosterone surge, aggression as a trait is less evident in daily life and cognitive reflection might counteract aggressive trends in men with low 2D:4D. Boys with higher sociability (following higher prenatal androgen load) may be involved into fights more often due to the increased total frequency of interactions with others and given the fact that physical aggression is used instrumentally in healthy young children.

In support of this developmental view on aggression, we also did not find a correlation of prenatal testosterone with anti-social personality disorder (ASPD) items. Whereas conduct disorder increases the risk for ASPD (Olsson and Hansson, 2009), other factors like intelligence, parent psychopathology, parent-child relation, and peer-rejection are known to moderate this risk essentially (Olsson and Hansson, 2009).

In this study, lower 2D:4D correlated with bigger personal social capital and a larger personal social network. Here, we will argue that negotiation strategies, which are conceptually related to social networking, change from children to adulthood into more functional behavior in people with lower 2D:4D. In adult men, lower 2D:4D is associated with more uncooperative behavior, but only when they act intuitively or less reflected (Millet and Buehler, 2017; Millet and Aydinli, 2019) and as already mentioned, men with lower 2D:4D have stronger cognitive reflection skills (Bosch-Domenèch et al., 2014). In general, adult men with low 2D:4D prefer fair from either altruistic or egoistic choices (Millet and Dewitte, 2006), even though their faces appear more dominant to others (Neave et al., 2003). In children, however, a lower 2D:4D is still unrelated to fair choices and correlates with less altruistic choices instead (Millet and Dewitte, 2006). In adults, social status relevance (potentially leading to a surge in testosterone) within a given context moderates the impact of 2D:4D on cooperative behavior, aggression, and dominance in economic games (Millet and Buehler, 2017). Taken together, evidence on negotiation strategies of lower 2D:4D subjects supports our findings on the relationship between 2D:4D, social capital, and network size.

Furthermore, children with a higher status – as measured in number of friends/interaction partners – choose the prosocial option less often (Horn et al., 2018). In contrast to our data from adults, in which a bigger social capital and a larger social network are associated with lower 2D:4D, in boys the strategies to gain status may still be dysfunctional, as a link with number of friends/interaction partners and 2D:4D was not found (Horn et al., 2018).

The relationship between sociability, aggression, and behavioral strategies to gain status or bond might change from childhood to adulthood, when cognitive reflection and the frontal lobe have fully developed. As a consequence, normative behavior, learned cooperation, and fairness may be utilized by adult men with low 2D:4D, at least in unchallenging situations. Furthermore, experiences from frequent social interactions (sociability) and from testing the limits with others during childhood (instrumental aggression) might in the end help to bond with others and make these subjects more resilient, explaining the long term positive outcomes of men with lower 2D:4D in academia (Nye et al., 2017; Tektas et al., 2019), reproduction (Manning et al., 2000) and trading (Coates et al., 2009).

Although we found that low 2D:4D in men is associated with higher trait sociability and possibly more social bonds to rely on, there is evidence for a more avoidant attachment style (Del Giudice and Angeleri, 2016) and lesser quality of relationships in people with low 2D:4D (Knickmeyer et al., 2005). Furthermore, intimate partner violence is actually higher in low 2D:4D men (Romero-Martínez et al., 2013). Thus, sociability and a bigger social capital in men do not necessarily mean that intimate or close relationships are better on the long term. They might even be worse as subjects are more directed at social status than intimacy.

G-allele carriers of the μ opioid receptor 1 polymorphism rs1799971 experience more pleasure in social situations (Troisi et al., 2011) and alcohol-dependent G-allele carriers show increased cue-reactivity to alcohol stimuli in certain brain regions which correlates with craving (Bach et al., 2015). As endogenous opioids contribute to the punishing effects of social isolation and rejection (Knowles et al., 1989; Kalin et al., 1995; Briand et al., 2015), it is interesting that an interaction between 2D:4D and the rs1799971 polymorphism has been reported for alcohol dependence (Gegenhuber et al., 2018). Both aspects of sociability, the interest in parties and friends und isolation intolerance, which correlated with 2D:4D in our study, might influence the development of alcohol dependency. This study’s results indicate that the pleasure to bond with others and enjoy social laughter is increased in people with low 2D:4D which might lead to more reward (opioid release) experienced during these situations. This mechanism might potentiate the rewarding effect of consumption (again opioid release) by increased chances of social laughter and bonding. Finally, also isolation intolerance might play a role, as it might induce drinking behavior to cope with loneliness. However, further research is needed to test these hypotheses.

At first glance, the observed negative correlation between 2D:4D and social network might contradict the fact that lower 2D:4D has been associated with suicide completion (Lenz et al., 2016) because social connectedness has been shown to be protective against suicidal behaviors (Fässberg et al., 2012). However, for suicide completion, it has been argued that correlations of lower 2D:4D with stronger cognitive reflection (Bosch-Domenèch et al., 2014; Millet and Aydinli, 2019) might play a role, leading to better planned and more successful suicide attempts, as 2D:4D measured independently from cognitive reflection is unrelated to suicidal thoughts and attempts (Lenz et al., 2019b).

Limitations

Self-measured 2D:4D is less reliable than expert-measures and is said to reach only 46% of its reliability (Hönekopp and Watson, 2010). Furthermore, finger deformation was not assessed in this project, which has reduced precision. We are aware of current criticism on 2D:4D as a proxy for prenatal androgen exposure, as the experimental evidence used to support the validity of 2D:4D as a biomarker of prenatal androgen exposure has not been replicated consistently (Berenbaum et al., 2009; Huber et al., 2017; Del Giudice et al., 2018).

Besides correlating our primary independent variable with our dependent variables, we extended the analysis to exploratory testing of left and right hand 2D:4D and asymmetries of left and right hand 2D:4D, but did not correct for multiple hypothesis testing which might have resulted in false positive findings in the exploratory analysis.

Personal social capital was assessed in C-SURF only with a selection of items from the Personal Social Capital Scale, using only the 5 most relevant items per subscale. Even though we found a good internal consistency of 0.85 Cronbach’s alpha, construct validity remains unknown for this subset of items.

Personality disorder diagnoses like anti-social personality disorder should be assessed by experienced clinicians using structured clinical interviews (Paap et al., 2017). Here, we correlated the summation score of a self-report screening instrument with unknown discriminability for this clinical disorder.

We did not find a correlation between 2D:4D and aggression as a personality factor. In a meta-analysis on 2D:4D and aggressive behavior, it was reported that any correlation found appear to be very small and findings are context dependent (Hönekopp and Watson, 2011). We investigated the personality factor aggression with a questionnaire and did not use an experimental setup with provocative cues or interaction partners. Furthermore, we face a lower reliability of self-measured 2D:4D measures in comparison to expert ratings. Moreover, sex differences in aggression appear to be larger in children than in adults (Campbell, 2006; Archer, 2009) and our adult cohort is rather homogeneous in age.

Exploratory analysis of social network size was only probed by two self-reported items and future research should use more reliable and objective measures to investigate the relationship between 2D:4D and social network size.

Finally, our cohort consisted of mostly Caucasian young men and the results cannot be transferred to other ethnicities, gender, or age groups.

Conclusion

To summarize, our data show that low 2D:4D is associated with higher trait sociability, bigger personal social capital, and larger personal social network size. Given the complexity of human behavior and environmental/nurture effects on personality, it is not surprising that the correlations are small though. Our study provides a better understanding of the link between prenatal influences and social behavior in adulthood. It also leads to an interesting hypothesis on the mediating role of sociability between prenatal environment and life achievements, behavioral problems in adolescence, and other health related aspects.

Members of Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors

Gerhard Gmel: Addiction Medicine, Lausanne University Hospital CHUV, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland; Addiction Switzerland, Lausanne, Switzerland; Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada; University of the West of England, Frenchay Campus, Bristol, United Kingdom (Gerhard.Gmel@chuv.ch). Meichun Mohler-Kuo: La Source, School of Nursing Sciences, HES-SO University of Applied Sciences and Arts of Western Switzerland, Lausanne, Switzerland (m.mohler-kuo@ecolelasource.ch). Simon Foster: Institut für Epidemiologie, Biostatistik und Prävention, Hirschengraben, Zurich, Switzerland (simon.foster@kjpd.uzh.ch). Simon Marmet: Addiction Medicine, Lausanne University Hospital CHUV, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland (simon.marmet@chuv.ch). Joseph Studer: Addiction Medicine, Lausanne University Hospital CHUV, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland (Joseph.Studer@chuv.ch).

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Clinical Research of Lausanne University Medical School (Protocol No. 15/07). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

VB and BL conceived and designed the research, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. GG, MM-K, SM, SF, and JS performed the experiments. CM and JK commented on the manuscript and provided the intellectual input.

Funding

The third C-SURF survey was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant: FN 33CS30_148493). This scientific research was also promoted by the STAEDTLER Foundation and the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (IMAC-Mind project: Improving Mental Health and Reducing Addiction in Childhood and Adolescence through Mindfulness: Mechanisms, Prevention and Treatment; 2018–2022; 01GL1745C). The funders played no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. CM is an associated fellow of the research training group 2162 funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) - 270949263/GRK2162.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the C-SURF participants for their continuing support of the research project. In addition, we acknowledge support by the DFG within the funding program Open Access Publishing.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00246/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.c-surf.ch
  2. ^ www.c-surf.ch/img/questionnaires_pdf/q3_follow_up2_en.pdf

References

Ackermann, K., Kirchner, M., Bernhard, A., Martinelli, A., Anomitri, C., Baker, R., et al. (2019). Relational aggression in adolescents with conduct disorder: sex differences and behavioral correlates. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 47, 1625–1637. doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00541-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aluja, A., Rossier, J., García, L. F., Angleitner, A., Kuhlman, M., and Zuckerman, M. (2006). A cross-cultural shortened form of the ZKPQ (ZKPQ-50-cc) adapted to English, French, German, and Spanish languages. Personal. Individ. Differ. 41, 619–628. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonucci, T. C. (1994). A Life-Span View of Women’s Social Relations, Women Growing Older: Psychological Perspectives (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc), 239–269.

Google Scholar

Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: a meta-analytic review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 8, 291–322. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 249–266. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09990951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archuleta, A. J., and Miller, C. R. (2011). Validity evidence for the translated version of the personal social capital scale among people of Mexican descent. J. Soc. Soc. Work Res. 2, 39–53. doi: 10.5243/jsswr.2011.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Åslund, C., Starrin, B., and Nilsson, K. W. (2010). Social capital in relation to depression, musculoskeletal pain, and psychosomatic symptoms: a cross-sectional study of a large population-based cohort of Swedish adolescents. BMC Public Health 10:715. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach, P., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Kirsch, M., Hoffmann, S., Jorde, A., Frank, J., et al. (2015). Increased mesolimbic cue-reactivity in carriers of the mu-opioid-receptor gene OPRM1 A118G polymorphism predicts drinking outcome: a functional imaging study in alcohol dependent subjects. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 1128–1135. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berenbaum, S. A., Bryk, K. K., Nowak, N., Quigley, C. A., and Moffat, S. (2009). Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure. Endocrinology 150, 5119–5124. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-0774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilgic, Ö, Altınyazar, H. C., Eryılmaz, D., and Tuğrul, Z. A. (2016). Are 2D:4D finger-length ratios an indicator of androgenetic alopecia in males? An. Bras. Dermatol. 91, 156–159. doi: 10.1590/abd1806-4841.20164622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, B. M., Geniole, S. N., Procyshyn, T. L., Ortiz, T. L., Carré, J. M., and Watson, N. V. (2019). Effect of exogenous testosterone on cooperation depends on personality and time pressure. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 538–545. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0220-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch-Domenèch, A., Brañas-Garza, P., and Espín, A. M. (2014). Can exposure to prenatal sex hormones (2D:4D) predict cognitive reflection? Psychoneuroendocrinology 43, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Aufleger, B., Braun, S., Gattnar, M., Kallmayer, S., Wagner, H., et al. (2018). Cross-sectional and longitudinal evaluation of the social network use disorder and internet gaming disorder criteria. Front. Psychiatry 9:692. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Mühle, C., Kornhuber, J., and Lenz, B. (2015). Internet gaming disorder, social network disorder and laterality: handedness relates to pathological use of social networks. J. Neural. Transm. 122, 1187–1196. doi: 10.1007/s00702-014-1361-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briand, L. A., Hilario, M., Dow, H. C., Brodkin, E. S., Blendy, J. A., and Berton, O. (2015). Mouse model of OPRM1 (A118G) polymorphism increases sociability and dominance and confers resilience to social defeat. J. Neurosci. 35, 3582–3590. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4685-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, E. C. Z., Steadman, C. J., Lee, T. M., Padmanabhan, V., Lehman, M. N., and Coolen, L. M. (2015). Sex differences and effects of prenatal exposure to excess testosterone on ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons in adult sheep. Eur. J. Neurosci. 41, 1157–1166. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, W. M., Hines, M., Fane, B. A., and Breedlove, S. M. (2002). Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm. Behav. 42, 380–386. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buck, J. J., Williams, R. M., Hughes, I. A., and Acerini, C. L. (2003). In-utero androgen exposure and 2nd to 4th digit length ratio-comparisons between healthy controls and females with classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hum. Reprod. 18, 976–979. doi: 10.1093/humrep/deg198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, A. (2006). Sex differences in direct aggression: what are the psychological mediators? Aggress. Violent Behav. 11, 237–264. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2005.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canan, F., Karaca, S., Düzgün, M., Erdem, A. M., Karaçayl∣, E., Topan, N. B., et al. (2017). The relationship between second-to-fourth digit (2D:4D) ratios and problematic and pathological Internet use among Turkish university students. J. Behav. Addict. 6, 30–41. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canan, F., Tegin, C., and Gecici, O. (2019). The second to fourth digit (2D:4D) ratios, smoking, and problem drinking in a young adult university student sample. Neurol. Psychiatry Brain Res. 32, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.npbr.2019.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y. P., and Zhu, D. H. (2012). The role of perceived social capital and flow experience in building users’ continuance intention to social networking sites in China. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28, 995–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, J. M., Gurnell, M., and Rustichini, A. (2009). Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 623–628. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810907106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Smith, A., Taylor, E., and Rubia, K. (2012). A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. Cortex 48, 194–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Giudice, M., and Angeleri, R. (2016). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and attachment styles in middle childhood: indirect evidence for an organizational effect of sex hormones. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s40750-015-0027-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Giudice, M., Barrett, E. S., Belsky, J., Hartman, S., Martel, M. M., Sangenstedt, S., et al. (2018). Individual differences in developmental plasticity: a role for early androgens? Psychoneuroendocrinology 90, 165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichler, A., Heinrich, H., Moll, G. H., Beckmann, M. W., Goecke, T. W., Fasching, P. A., et al. (2018). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and behavioral symptoms in primary-school aged boys. Early Hum. Dev. 119, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evardone, M., and Alexander, G. M. (2009). Anxiety, sex-linked behaviors, and digit ratios (2D:4D). Arch. Sex. Behav. 38, 442–455. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9260-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fässberg, M. M., van Orden, K. A., Duberstein, P., Erlangsen, A., Lapierre, S., Bodner, E., et al. (2012). A systematic review of social factors and suicidal behavior in older adulthood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 9, 722–745. doi: 10.3390/ijerph9030722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gegenhuber, B., Weinland, C., Kornhuber, J., Mühle, C., and Lenz, B. (2018). OPRM1 A118G and serum beta-endorphin interact with sex and digit ratio (2D:4D) to influence risk and course of alcohol dependence. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 28, 1418–1428. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, M. D., Davies, M. N. O., and Chappell, D. (2004). Online computer gaming: a comparison of adolescent and adult gamers. J. Adolesc. 27, 87–96. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hell, B., and Päßler, K. (2011). Are occupational interests hormonally influenced? The 2D:4D-interest nexus. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 376–380. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hergueta, T., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, D., and Weiller, E. (2015). Mini international neuropsychiatric interview French current DSM-IV. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.1.2792.9440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hönekopp, J., and Schuster, M. (2010). A meta-analysis on 2D:4D and athletic prowess: substantial relationships but neither hand out-predicts the other. Personal. Individ. Differ. 48, 4–10. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hönekopp, J., and Watson, S. (2010). Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 22, 619–630. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.21054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hönekopp, J., and Watson, S. (2011). Meta-analysis of the relationship between digit-ratio 2D:4D and aggression. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 381–386. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, L., Zhan-Bing, M., Zhi-Yun, S., Xiao-Xia, S., Jun-Li, Z., and Zheng-Hao, H. (2014). Digit ratio (2D:4D) in Chinese women with breast cancer. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 26, 562–564. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, L., Hungerländer, N. A., Windhager, S., Bugnyar, T., and Massen, J. J. M. (2018). Social status and prenatal testosterone exposure assessed via second-to-fourth digit ratio affect 6-9-year-old children’s prosocial choices. Sci. Rep. 8:9198. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27468-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, S. E., Lenz, B., Kornhuber, J., and Müller, C. P. (2017). Prenatal androgen-receptor activity has organizational morphological effects in mice. PLoS One 12:e0188752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, S. E., Zoicas, I., Reichel, M., Mühle, C., Büttner, C., Ekici, A. B., et al. (2018). Prenatal androgen receptor activation determines adult alcohol and water drinking in a sex-specific way. Addict. Biol. 23, 904–920. doi: 10.1111/adb.12540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., and Lynn, D. E. (1995). Opiate systems in mother and infant primates coordinate intimate contact during reunion. Psychoneuroendocrinology 20, 735–742. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(95)00023-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., McLeod, J. D., and Wethington, E. (1985). The costs of caring: a perspective on the relationship between sex and psychological distress. Theory Res. Appl. 24, 491–506. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-5115-0_25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilduff, L. P., Hopp, R. N., Cook, C. J., Crewther, B. T., and Manning, J. T. (2013). Digit ratio (2D:4D), aggression, and testosterone in men exposed to an aggressive video stimulus. Evol. Psychol. 11, 953–964. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., and Taylor, K. (2005). Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 198–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00349.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowles, P. A., Conner, R. L., and Panksepp, J. (1989). Opiate effects on social behavior of juvenile dogs as a function of social deprivation. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 33, 533–537. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90382-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornhuber, J., Erhard, G., Lenz, B., Kraus, T., Sperling, W., Bayerlein, K., et al. (2011). Low digit ratio 2D:4D in alcohol dependent patients. PLoS One 6:e19332. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornhuber, J., Zenses, E. M., Lenz, B., Stoessel, C., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Rehbein, F., et al. (2013). Low 2D:4D values are associated with video game addiction. PLoS One 8:e79539. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovářík, J., Brañas-Garza, P., Davidson, M. W., Haim, D. A., Carcelli, S., and Fowler, J. H. (2017). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and social integration: an effect of prenatal sex hormones. Netw. Sci. 5, 476–489. doi: 10.1017/nws.2017.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Mühle, C., and Kornhuber, J. (2018a). Low digit ratio (2D:4D) and late pubertal onset indicate prenatal hyperandrogenziation in alcohol binge drinking. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 86, 370–378. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Eichler, A., Schwenke, E., Buchholz, V. N., Hartwig, C., Moll, G. H., et al. (2018b). Mindfulness-based stress reduction in pregnancy: an app-based programme to improve the health of mothers and children (MINDFUL/PMI Study). Geburtshilfe Frauenheilkd 78, 1283–1291. doi: 10.1055/a-0677-2630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., and Kornhuber, J. (2018). Cross-national gender variations of digit ratio (2D:4D) correlate with life expectancy, suicide rate, and other causes of death. J. Neural. Transm. 125, 239–246. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1815-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Mühle, C., Braun, B., Weinland, C., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Behrens, J., et al. (2017). Prenatal and adult androgen activities in alcohol dependence. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 136, 96–107. doi: 10.1111/acps.12725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., and Mühle, C., Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors, and Kornhuber, J. (2019a). Lower digit ratio (2D:4D) in alcohol dependence: confirmation and exploratory analysis in a population-based study of young men. Addict. Biol. doi: 10.1111/adb.12815 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Röther, M., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Mühle, C., Tektas, O. Y., and Kornhuber, J. (2019b). The androgen model of suicide completion. Prog. Neurobiol. 172, 84–103. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Thiem, D., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Mühle, C., Stoessel, C., Betz, P., et al. (2016). Low digit ratio (2D:4D) in male suicide victims. J. Neural. Transm. 123, 1499–1503. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1608-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lombardo, M. V., Ashwin, E., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., Lai, M.-C., Taylor, K., et al. (2012). Fetal programming effects of testosterone on the reward system and behavioral approach tendencies in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 839–847. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Knickmeyer, R., and Manning, J. T. (2004). 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early. Hum. Dev. 77, 23–28. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J., Kilduff, L., Cook, C., Crewther, B., and Fink, B. (2014). Digit ratio (2D:4D): a biomarker for prenatal sex steroids and adult sex steroids in challenge situations. Front. Endocrinol. 5:9. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., Barley, L., Walton, J., Lewis-Jones, D. I., Trivers, R. L., Singh, D., et al. (2000). The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success: evidence for sexually antagonistic genes. Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 163–183. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00029-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., Wilson, J., and Lewis-Jones, D. I. (1998). The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum. Reprod. 13, 3000–3004. doi: 10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masuya, Y., Okamoto, Y., Inohara, K., Matsumura, Y., Fujioka, T., Wada, Y., et al. (2015). Sex-different abnormalities in the right second to fourth digit ratio in Japanese individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Mol. Autism 6:34. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0028-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millet, K., and Aydinli, A. (2019). Cognitive reflection, 2D:4D and social value orientation. PLoS One 14:e0212767. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millet, K., and Buehler, F. (2017). A context dependent interpretation of inconsistencies in 2d:4d findings: the moderating role of status relevance. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:254. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millet, K., and Dewitte, S. (2006). Second to fourth digit ratio and cooperative behavior. Biol. Psychol. 71, 111–115. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millet, K., and Dewitte, S. (2008). A subordinate status position increases the present value of financial resources for low 2D:4D men. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 20, 110–115. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, D. C., Baglietto, L., Manning, J. T., McLean, C., Hopper, J. L., English, D. R., et al. (2012). Second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D), breast cancer risk factors, and breast cancer risk: a prospective cohort study. Br. J. Cancer 107, 1631–1636. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2012.418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murayama, H., Fujiwara, Y., and Kawachi, I. (2012). Social capital and health: a review of prospective multilevel studies. J. Epidemiol. 22, 179–187. doi: 10.2188/jea.je20110128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neave, N., Laing, S., Fink, B., and Manning John, T. (2003). Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 270, 2167–2172. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nye, J. V. C., Bryukhanov, M., and Polyachenko, S. (2017). 2D:4D and lifetime educational outcomes: evidence from the Russian RMLS survey. Personal. Individ. Differ. 112, 62–66. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsson, M., and Hansson, K. (2009). A long-term follow-up study of adolescents with conduct disorder: can outcome be predicted from self-concept and intelligence? Nord. J. Psychiatry 63, 454–461. doi: 10.3109/08039480903009100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paap, M. C. S., Braeken, J., Pedersen, G., Urnes, Ø., Karterud, S., Wilberg, T., et al. (2017). A psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder: dimensionality, local reliability, and differential item functioning across gender. Assessment doi: 10.1177/1073191117745126 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). Associations between neurochemical receptor genes, 2D:4D, impulsivity and relationship quality. Biol. Lett. 14:20180642. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portnoy, J., Raine, A., Glenn, A. L., Chen, F. R., Choy, O., and Granger, D. A. (2015). Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the relationship between cortisol reactivity and self-reported externalizing behavior in young adolescent males. Biol. Psychol. 112, 94–106. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimers, S. (2007). The BBC internet study: general methodology. Arch. Sex. Behav. 36, 147–161. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9143-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero-Martínez, Á., Lila, M., Catalá-Miñana, A., Williams, R. K., and Moya-Albiol, L. (2013). The contribution of childhood parental rejection and early androgen exposure to impairments in socio-cognitive skills in intimate partner violence perpetrators with high alcohol consumption. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 10, 3753–3770. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10083753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roney, J. R., and Maestripieri, D. (2004). Relative digit lengths predict men’s behavior and attractiveness during social interactions with women. Hum. Nat. 15, 271–282. doi: 10.1007/s12110-004-1009-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubia, K., Lim, L., Ecker, C., Halari, R., Giampietro, V., Simmons, A., et al. (2013). Effects of age and gender on neural networks of motor response inhibition: from adolescence to mid-adulthood. Neuroimage 83, 690–703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20), 22–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Siegmann, E.-M., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Lenz, B., and Kornhuber, J. (2019). Digit ratio (2D:4D) in relation to substance and computer use: a meta-analysis. J. Neural. Transm. (Vienna) 126, 623–636. doi: 10.1007/s00702-019-02002-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tektas, O. Y., Kapsner, L., Lemmer, M., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Lewczuk, P., Lenz, B., et al. (2019). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and academic success as measured by achievement in the academic degree “Habilitation”. PLoS One 14:e0212167. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troisi, A. (2001). Gender differences in vulnerability to social stress: a Darwinian perspective. Physiol. Behav. 73, 443–449. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00459-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troisi, A., Frazzetto, G., Carola, V., Di Lorenzo, G., Coviello, M., D’Amato, F. R., et al. (2011). Social hedonic capacity is associated with the A118G polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) in adult healthy volunteers and psychiatric patients. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 88–97. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.482786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyborowska, A., Volman, I., Smeekens, S., Toni, I., and Roelofs, K. (2016). Testosterone during puberty shifts emotional control from pulvinar to anterior prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 36, 6156–6164. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Bergh, B., and Dewitte, S. (2006). Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the impact of sexual cues on men’s decisions in ultimatum games. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 273, 2091–2095. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Honk, J., Montoya, E. R., Bos, P. A., van Vugt, M., and Terburg, D. (2012). New evidence on testosterone and cooperation. Nature 485:E4. doi: 10.1038/nature11136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, T., Gomes, M. C., Pita, A., Neto, M. T., and Taylor, A. (2013). Digit ratio (2D:4D) in newborns: influences of prenatal testosterone and maternal environment. Early Hum. Dev. 89, 107–112. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2012.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, P., Chen, X., Gong, J., and Jacques-Tiura, A. J. (2014). Reliability and validity of the personal social capital scale 16 and personal social capital scale 8: two short instruments for survey studies. Soc. Indic. Res. 119, 1133–1148. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0540-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. H. G., Greenhalgh, K. D., and Manning, J. T. (2003). Second to fourth finger ratio and possible precursors of developmental psychopathology in preschool children. Early Hum. Dev. 72, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(03)00012-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Z., and Cohn, M. J. (2011). Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit ratios. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 16289–16294. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108312108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., and Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: the big three, the big five, and the alternative five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 757–768. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.4.757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: 2D:4D, digit ratio, sociability, aggression, opioid receptor, social behavior, isolation intolerance

Citation: Buchholz VN, Mühle C, Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors, Kornhuber J and Lenz B (2019) Lower Digit Ratio (2D:4D) Indicative of Excess Prenatal Androgen Is Associated With Increased Sociability and Greater Social Capital. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:246. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00246

Received: 09 August 2019; Accepted: 07 October 2019;
Published: 05 December 2019.

Edited by:

Ekrem Dere, Sorbonne University, France

Reviewed by:

Dirk Scheele, University of Bonn, Germany
Ramón Sotomayor-Zárate, University of Valparaíso, Chile

Copyright © 2019 Buchholz, Mühle, Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors, Kornhuber and Lenz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Verena N. Buchholz, VerenaNadine.Buchholz@uk-erlangen.de

Download