Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 22 March 2022
Sec. Pathological Conditions
This article is part of the Research Topic Behavioral Outcomes of Traumatic Brain Injury View all 9 articles

The Impact of Exercise and Virtual Reality Executive Function Training on Cognition Among Heavy Drinking Veterans With Traumatic Brain Injury: A Pilot Feasibility Study

\r\nDavid L. Pennington,,*David L. Pennington1,2,3*Jill V. Reavis,,,Jill V. Reavis1,2,3,4Monique T. Cano,Monique T. Cano1,2Erica Walker,,Erica Walker1,2,3Steven L. Batki,,Steven L. Batki1,2,3
  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 2San Francisco Veterans Affairs Health Care System (SFVAHCS), San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 3Northern California Institute for Research and Education (NCIRE), San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA, United States

Executive function (EF) underlies self-control deficits in alcohol use disorder (AUD) and traumatic brain injury (TBI). Cognitive training is a promising adjunctive treatment targeting TBI- and AUD- related cognitive dysfunction. However, major limitations related to compliance and generalizability in the field of cognitive training exist. Physical activity is associated with enhanced cognitive performance across several executive functions and may enhance the benefits of cognitive training. Virtual reality provides multisensory embodied experiences which are likely to engage brain networks more efficiently than standard cognitive training systems, ultimately resulting in greater near- and far-transfer effects. This pilot study aimed to obtain feasibility data and a preliminary assessment of an enriched virtual reality (VR) EF training (EFT) intervention combined with exercise (NCT03786276). Using an 8-week randomized adaptive design study, 30 AUD treatment seeking U.S. Veterans completed nine sessions of exercise-only (n = 15) or gameplay control (n = 15) over 3 weeks, followed by a week-4 repeat assessment in Phase 1. Twenty-three participants completed and moved onto Phase II, where they completed up to nine sessions of VR-EFT plus exercise and completed a week-8 end-of-study assessment. Primary outcomes included feasibility to retain participants, usability, and satisfaction of using VR-EFT. Secondary and exploratory outcomes included within group assessment of change in cognitive function, alcohol use, alcohol craving, and post-concussive symptoms among the three treatment conditions.VR-EFT was feasible with moderate usability and high acceptability ratings.The most common VR-related adverse effect was motion sickness (n = 2/16, 12.5%). The VR-EFT condition was associated with significant improvement in inhibition-switching and visual scanning (both p < 0.05) during Phase II. Exercise-only was associated with significant improvements in cognitive inhibition, cognitive flexibility, reductions in alcohol craving, and number of standard alcohol drinks per week (all p ≤ 0.05). The gaming-control condition was associated with improvement in cognitive flexibility and visuospatial immediate recall (both p < 0.05) during Phase 1. Recruitment and retention of U.S. veterans with AUD and TBI into an exercise plus VR-EFT intervention is feasible, but technological barriers may impact usability. VR-EFT was associated with improvement in executive function domains that were targeted in as little as 3-week and nine sessions of VR-EFT exposure. Results are promising and indicate the need for a larger controlled investigation to assess the efficacy of VR-EFT to enhance treatment outcomes among AUD treatment-seeking U.S. veterans with co-occurring AUD and TBI.

Clinical Trial Registration: www.ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier: NCT03786276.

Introduction

Executive function (EF) underlies successful self-regulation processes (Ilkowska and Engle, 2010; Hofmann et al., 2011) and difficulty with EF is commonly seen among individuals with alcohol use disorder (AUD), a population at disproportionate risk for traumatic brain injury (TBI; Drubach et al., 1993; Corrigan, 1995; Kreutzer et al., 1996; Hibbard et al., 1998; Bogner et al., 2001). U.S. Veterans are at greater risk for TBI and AUD relative to civilian counterparts (Brady et al., 2009; National Center for Health Statistics, 2019), and hazardous drinking can exacerbate ongoing TBI symptoms and increase vulnerability to future head injuries (Schumm and Chard, 2012). Conversely, TBI-related cognitive impairments increase one’s likelihood of engaging in hazardous alcohol use (Corrigan and Cole, 2008; Polusny et al., 2011; Miller, 2018; Jorge et al., 2019). Increased bidirectional risk between AUD and TBI is likely related to disruption of prefrontal cortical functioning in the areas of impulse control, reward expectation, and emotion regulation (Bjork and Grant, 2009). Many other conditions, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Hoge et al., 2004; Corrigan and Cole, 2008; Brady et al., 2009; Gros et al., 2016), anxiety (Hoge et al., 2004), and depression (Hoge et al., 2004, 2008) also commonly co-occur among Veterans with TBI. Although effective interventions for the cognitive sequelae of TBI do exist, most are time-intensive for providers and patients (Cicerone et al., 2019) and very few studies have examined effective treatments specifically for Veterans with co-occurring AUD and TBI (Pennington et al., 2020). The lack of empirically supported treatments highlights the need to enhance interventions for these highly prevalent and costly co-occurring conditions and fill a gap in what’s known about addressing the overlapping cognitive sequalae of AUD and TBI in U.S. Veterans.

Cognitive training strategies that target cognitive dysfunctions have shown promise to restore cognitive alterations and help maintain abstinence in substance use disorders (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019) by enhancing neuroplasticity and supporting more adaptive decision-making ability (Houben et al., 2011; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019). Working-memory training (WMT) has been the most studied EF-based cognitive training tool (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019) and has demonstrated promise for near-transfer effects, including improved performance on related tasks of working-memory (Lechner et al., 2019) and some far-transfer effects related to reduced alcohol consumption among heavy drinkers following one month of training (Houben et al., 2011). WMT often involves a series of computerized tasks that require participants to repeatedly manipulate and recall stimuli (e.g., shapes, numbers) with increasing challenge. Beyond having limited evidence of far-transfer effects (e.g., robust change in alcohol use behavior), findings indicate that WMT is associated with design and compliance issues, as the tasks are often repetitive and require significant time to achieve the desired effects (Wanmaker et al., 2018). Thus, study retention is often poor, which raises concerns about WMT generalizability and its transferability toward other behaviors, including increased self-control over substance use (Wanmaker et al., 2018). Other 2-dimensional (2D) computer-based cognitive trainings have been shown to improve cognitive performance but failed to show transferability to real-world tasks (Ball et al., 2002) and there is little evidence to suggest that they offer an advantage compared to traditional paper methodologies, such as solving crossword puzzles or when compared to non-specific video games (Owen et al., 2010; Kable et al., 2017; Stanmore et al., 2017; Hampshire et al., 2019). These research gaps illuminate the need for more treatment studies on cognitive training overall, as well as the development of innovations to enhance cognitive training acceptability and potential long-term effectiveness.

Virtual reality (VR) technology has evolved substantially in the past several years, leading to the creation of low-cost, lightweight high-resolution headsets with sophisticated head tracking sensors to create a realistic sense of physical presence in virtual environments. Human cognition has been proposed to be deeply rooted in the body’s interactions with the world for the purpose of initiating and controlling physical action (Wilson, 2002; Raichlen and Alexander, 2017). While 2D screen-based systems fail to provide body-related sensory information (e.g., proprioception), VR environments provide multisensory embodied experiences which are likely to engage brain networks more efficiently than 2D systems, ultimately resulting in greater near- and far-transfer effects.

Emerging evidence supports the use of innovative interventions like VR to enhance cognitive functioning among individuals with AUD and cognitive impairment secondary to alcohol use (Gamito et al., 2021). Findings suggest that the utilization of VR technology can improve learning by helping focus attention through engagement in immersive, multisensory, realistic 3-D environments (Hoffman et al., 2004). Among U.S. veterans, it has been well established that simulated VR environments have been effective in promoting readiness and resilience (Parsons and Rizzo, 2008). Less is known, however, about the success of VR-based cognitive training interventions to enhance cognitive function and other outcomes among U.S. veterans with AUD and TBI (e.g., changes in substance use; Wanmaker et al., 2018; Lechner et al., 2019; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019).

Exercise has also been found to demonstrate considerable improvements in cognitive functioning (Hillman et al., 2008). Findings from behavioral (Kramer et al., 1999; Hillman et al., 2006) and neuroimaging (Colcombe et al., 2004) studies indicate that increased levels of physical activity are associated with enhanced cognitive performance across several executive functions, including attentional control, cognitive flexibility, and self-regulation. Exercise may result in even greater improvements in cognition if combined with cognitive training and when both are sufficiently demanding (Lauenroth et al., 2016). Evidence from multiple studies comparing combined cognitive training with physical activity to cognitive training alone supports the hypothesis that combined training produces greater overall benefits than the latter (Anderson-Hanley et al., 2012; Theill et al., 2013; Bamidis et al., 2014; Law et al., 2014; Zolyniak et al., 2014; Ballesteros et al., 2015; Joubert and Chainay, 2018). A study by Rahe et al. (2015) showed that while both cognitive training and combined cognitive and physical training showed significant gains in attention, only the combined training group showed a significant difference in divided attention and sustained improvement in a 1-year follow-up assessment. Physical activity combined with cognitive training may offer a greater potential benefit to cognitive functioning as both involve the recruitment of multiple resources and abilities rather than one alone.

Although not fully understood, physical activity may improve cognitive performance through a change in metabolic activity in the brain (cerebral blood flow resulting in increased oxygen and metabolism (Smith and Ainslie, 2017; Barnes and Corkery, 2018) or possibly through enhancing cerebral plasticity associated with increased brain-derived neurotrophic factor activation (Szuhany et al., 2015; Phillips, 2017; Walsh and Tschakovsky, 2018). Multiple reviews note the significant benefits of exercise in improving mood and decreasing anxiety (Giménez-Meseguer et al., 2020; Hu et al., 2020; Yoo, 2020; Ashdown-Franks et al., 2020; Smith and Merwin, 2021). In fact, physical activity has been helpful in reducing depression symptoms (Ashdown-Franks et al., 2020), alcohol consumption (including heavy drinking), and alcohol-related cravings among individuals with AUD (Roessler et al., 2017; Jensen et al., 2019; Hallgren et al., 2021). Pairing exercise with cognitive training may aid in improving alcohol treatment outcomes by impacting mechanisms related to mood, pleasure, and self-efficacy (Ashdown-Franks et al., 2020), in addition to neurological activation (Lauenroth et al., 2016).

Since improvement of cognitive function has the potential for enhancing AUD treatment effectiveness, we explored the potential benefit of a novel intervention that combines exercise with an enriched VR environment for delivering an executive function cognitive training intervention for heavy drinking U.S. Veterans with TBI. Our VR executive function training (VR-EFT) targets key aspects of EF—i.e., visual scanning, cognitive flexibility, cognitive inhibition, and processing speed—domains known to underlay significant EF deficits among individuals with AUD and TBI (Bates et al., 2013; Herrold et al., 2015; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019). The primary objectives of this study were to advance knowledge regarding the feasibility of delivering an exercise and VR-EFT intervention to understudied U.S. Veterans with co-occurring AUD and TBI, and to establish preliminary indication that VR-EFT is associated with EF improvement in this complex population. We also explore the extent to which exercise and VR-EFT generalize to other psychological and AUD treatment outcomes.

Materials and Methods

Participants

All participants provided written informed consent prior to the study and underwent procedures approved by the University of California, San Francisco, and the San Francisco Veterans Affairs Health Care System (SFVAHCS). All study procedures took place at SFVAHCS in San Francisco, CA. Participants were recruited from the SFVAHCS Addiction and Recovery Treatment Program via clinician referral or through research study fliers posted at the SFVAHCS between February 2019 and February 2020. The final sample included 30 Veterans with a documented history of TBI in the chronic stable phase of recovery (>6 months post-injury; American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (ACRM), Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Committee of the Head Injury Interdisciplinary Special Interest Group, 1993) and current (past year) moderate to severe AUD as defined by the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force, 2013). In addition to meeting AUD diagnostic criteria, all Veterans reported “heavy” drinking based on NIH/NIAAA criteria (i.e., at least 15 standard drinks for men and at least eight drinks per week for women; Willenbring et al., 2009) for at least 1 week in the 90 days prior to consent. All participants were cleared for physical activity readiness by a study physician, expressed a desire to reduce or stop alcohol use and were receiving treatment for AUD at the SFVAHCS. Participants were free to access any standard psychosocial or pharmacologic treatments for AUD, TBI, or any other psychiatric or medical conditions.

The history of TBI was established using a structured clinical interview adapted from the Veterans Health Administration Comprehensive TBI Evaluation (previously the TBI Second Level Evaluation; Belanger et al., 2009; Pennington et al., 2020) to include a comprehensive assessment of lifetime history of head injury (in addition to most recent and most severe) and occurrence and duration of loss of consciousness, alteration of consciousness, and posttraumatic amnesia for each event. A history of TBI was considered present if a participant endorsed VA/Department of Defense and the ACRM (American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (ACRM), Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Committee of the Head Injury Interdisciplinary Special Interest Group, 1993) criteria of having a traumatically-induced physiological disruption of brain function as a result of an external force resulting in at least one of the following: a loss of consciousness, memory loss for events immediately before or after the event, alteration in mental state at the time of the event (e.g., disorientation, confusion, slowed thinking), or neurological deficit(s) (e.g., loss of balance/coordination, change in vision, weakness). Veterans were excluded if they were pregnant or attempting to conceive, had any unstable psychiatric (e.g., active psychotic symptoms) or medical issues (e.g., acute alcohol withdrawal) judged by study clinicians to pose unacceptable risks, had physical disability making it impossible to use a stationary recumbent bicycle (e.g., leg amputation), or concurrent participation in another AUD or TBI treatment studies. All assessments were administered by the Study PI (Clinical Psychologist) or by trained study staff under direct supervision of the Study PI. Random observations across time of cognitive assessments were conducted to ensure fidelity.

Procedure

The primary goal of this study was to establish the feasibility of delivering an exercise and VR-EFT intervention to a difficult to recruit population of Veterans with TBI and AUD. Secondarily, we sought preliminary indication that VR-EFT is associated with improvement in targeted EF domains, and that this pattern of improvement would vary from within-group change observed in exercise-only or tablet-based gameplay conditions. Therefore, we chose to maximize the sample size within each of the three conditions and conducted an 8-week, randomized, adaptive design pilot study of a VR-based EFT combined with exercise compared to either exercise-only or tablet-based gameplay alone. Screening consisted of two to three visits during which participants completed the measures and interviews described below (Baseline Assessment). Veterans who met the study criteria were randomly assigned in a 1:1 ratio into a tablet-based gameplay condition or an exercise-only condition. Randomization was stratified on the total lifetime occurrence of TBIs (1 TBI vs. ≥ 2 TBIs) and balanced using computer-generated block randomization with permuted block sizes of six. Veterans were asked to complete nine 30-min sessions of the intervention they were assigned over the course of 3 weeks. These 3 weeks were followed by a 1-week washout period, which included repeat cognitive and other psychological assessments (week-4 assessment). Then, participants from both conditions were assigned to complete an exercise plus VR-EFT condition, which included up to nine 30-min VR-EFTsessions over a 3-week period. A final repeat post-treatment assessment occurred during the final week (i.e., week-8 assessment) after VR-EFT condition (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Virtual reality executive function training study design.

Materials

VR-EFT Exercise Condition. The VR-EFT exercise station consisted of a stationary recumbent bicycle, a set of gameplay controllers mounted on the recumbent bicycle handlebars, and an Oculus Rift VR headset. The VR-EFT task was developed by Blue Goji, a company that produces games designed to enhance neuroplasticity and promote user physical activity and engagement in VR gameplay. We utilized Blue Goji’s GoWings Safari, which engages and challenges cognitive domains of attentional scanning, processing speed, and cognitive inhibition and switching. Thus, it was considered appropriate to use as the VR-EFT intervention. This “game” is a path-based adventure game in which the user explores a wildlife environment. Users navigate through the virtual environment by pedaling a stationary bike. While doing so, they must scan and target animals and press a button on the handlebar-mounted controller to take the animal’s picture before they flee. If they see a “poacher,” they then alternate between the camera and net blaster to target and shoot the net blaster to capture the “poacher.” The faster the user pedals, the higher elevation they can gain in the environment enhancing field of view, and the faster they can move through the path. Certain animals are more difficult to spot, thus allotted higher scores. “Poachers” are associated with visual cues in the environment. Each environment includes several different collectible rewards, i.e., bananas (allows players to move faster in the environment without pedaling at a higher rate), camera film, and net blaster ammunition. Collecting items allows for the participant to increase the overall higher scores. Each session is randomly generated, spawning different animals and “poachers” in various locations. Participants can stay in the environment for an unlimited amount of time. The only limiting factors are film and ammunition, which can only be extended by finding collectibles. Participants’ overall score is a calculation of the photos and the number of poachers captured. There is feedback on achievement (score, distance traveled, photographs taken, and poachers captured) at the end of each session, tracked over multiple sessions. During the screening and prior to VR-EFT, each participant was given a tutorial demonstrating how to use the equipment in a non-specific VR environment. During the first training session, participants were oriented to the goals and gameplay. All sessions were conducted at the SFVAHCS under the supervision of a study staff member who was available to answer questions and provide guidance.

Gameplay Control Condition. To provide a non-VR gameplay control to our VR-EFT condition, participants could choose to play up to three non-specific 2D Blue Goji “racing” games, Speedbump, Super Moto X, or Row Your Boat, delivered on a mobile tablet. Each game required the user to manipulate a vehicle in 2-dimensional space down a “racetrack” using game controls presented on the touch screen tablet. The object of the gameplay generally included hitting targets with the user’s “vehicle” to increase speed and avoiding obstacles to win the race. These tasks targeted visual scanning and processing speed, but not higher-level components of executive function including cognitive inhibition or flexibility. During the screening, each participant was given a tutorial demonstrating how to play the games on the mobile tablet. During the first training session, participants were oriented to the goals and gameplay. Progress and advancement of gameplay were not tracked over time. All sessions were conducted at the SFVAHCS under the supervision of a study staff member who was available to answer questions and provide guidance.

Exercise-Only Condition. To provide an exercise condition that did not involve any cognitive challenge, participants used the identical stationary recumbent bicycle utilized for the VR-EFT condition, except without any VR-EFT equipment. The stationary recumbent bicycle tracked calories burned and distance traveled. During the first training session, participants were oriented to the use of the stationary recumbent bicycle. All sessions were conducted at the SFVAHCS under the supervision of a study staff member who was available to answer questions and provide guidance.

Measures

Alcohol Use and Craving Measures

Alcohol use was assessed using the Time Line Follow Back (TLFB; Sobell et al., 1985; Sobell and Sobell, 1992) interview, which yields a total number of standard alcohol drinks per week. Alcohol craving-related obsessive thoughts and compulsions were measured using the Obsessive Compulsive Drinking Scale (OCDS; Anton et al., 1995) at baseline assessment, week-4 assessment, and week-8 end of study assessment. The OCDS is a 14-item self-report scale with a total score which can range from 0 to 56, an obsessive thoughts subscale that can range from 0 to 24, and a compulsive drinking subscale which can range from 0 to 32, with higher scores indicating a greater level of obsessive-compulsive drinking.

Alcohol and Substance Use Disorder Diagnosis and Depression/Anxiety Symptom Severity

All participants were administered the Substance Use Disorders sections of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force, 2013) at baseline. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; Babor et al., 2001) was also administered to screen for harmful or hazardous alcohol use. The range of possible scores is from 0 to 40, where 0 indicates abstinence from alcohol, 1 to 7 indicates low-risk consumption, 8 to 14 suggests hazardous or harmful alcohol use, and scores of 15 or more indicate the likelihood of moderate to severe alcohol use disorder. Participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1961), a 21-item inventory that ranges from 0 to 63 to characterize depression symptoms (0–13 minimal, 14–19 mild, 20–28 moderate, and 29–63 severe) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck et al., 1988) a 21 item inventory that ranges from 0 to 63 to characterize anxiety symptoms (0–7 mild, 8–15 mild, 16–25 moderate, 26–63 severe) at baseline.

Post-concussive Syndrome Symptoms

The Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory (NSI; Department of Veterans Affairs, 2007), a 22-item measure designed to evaluate self-reported vestibular, somatic, cognitive, and affective post-concussive symptoms (e.g., headache, balance, nausea, etc.), was administered at baseline assessment, week-4 assessment, and week-8 end of study assessment. The NSI total score ranges from 0 to 88, with higher scores indicated a greater severity of post-concussive related symptoms. Subscale scores range from 0 to 12 for vestibular, 0 to 28 for somatic/sensory, 0 to 16 for cognitive, and 0 to 24 for affective symptoms. The Validity-10 scale was calculated to provide a measure of symptom validity assessment associated with the NSI, with acceptable scores being ≤22 (Vanderploeg et al., 2014).

Neurocognitive Assessment

The battery was developed to assess performance in cognitive domains commonly affected by heavy alcohol use and TBI. The battery contains only well-validated standardized instruments administered at baseline assessment, week-4 washout, and week-8 end of study assessment. Alternate forms were used for repeated administrations that are particularly susceptible to practice effects including the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt and Benedict, 2001) and brief visuospatial memory test—revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997). Raw scores were converted to standardized scores or t-scores via appropriate normative data adjusted for age, ethnicity and/or education. Domains and constituent measures included: Premorbid verbal intelligence: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (Venegas and Clark, 2011); Working Memory: WAIS-IV Arithmetic and Digit Span (Wechsler, 2008); Visual Scanning: D-KEFS Trail Making Test Condition 1 (Delis et al., 2004); Cognitive Flexibility: D-KEFS Trail Making Condition 4 (Letter-Number Sequencing) and Design Fluency Condition 3 (Switching Dots; Delis et al., 2004); Cognitive Inhibition: D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test Condition 3 (Delis et al., 2004); Cognitive Inhibition-Switching: D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test Condition 4 (Delis et al., 2004); Processing Speed: D-KEFS Trail Making Test, Condition 2 (Number Sequencing) and 3 (Letter Sequencing), D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test Condition 1 (Color Naming), 2 (Word Naming), and DKEFS Design Fluency Condition 1 (Filled Dots) and 2 (Empty Dots; Delis et al., 2004); Auditory-Verbal Recall: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt and Benedict, 2001); and Visuospatial Recall: Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997). The Reliable Digit Span (RDS) was calculated by summing the longest string of digits correctly recalled on forward plus backward trials (both trials correct) of WAIS-IV digit span to provide a measure of performance validity, with acceptable scores ≥7 (Schroeder et al., 2012).

Physical Activity and Readiness

All participants completed the physical activity readiness questionnaire for everyone (PAR-Q+), a 7-item screener followed by a two-page questionnaire that assesses for medical conditions that can limit physical readiness (Warburton et al., 2011). If a participant answered yes to one or more medical conditions that can limit physical activity, they were further assessed by a study physician using the physical activity readiness medical examination (PARmed-X). The study physician reviewed both the PAR-Q+ and PARmed-X prior to clearing aparticipant for unrestricted physical activity. Participants also completed the goden leisure-time exercise questionnaire (GLTEQ), a two-item questionnaire that measures the frequency of strenuous, moderate, and mild leisure physical activity performed for periods of 15 min or more over a usual week (Godin, 2011). This scale yields a leisure activity score that ranges from 0 to 13 (insufficiently active/edentary), 14 to 23 (moderately active), and 24 or more (active). Regardless of recent physical activity level, all participants were encouraged to start both the exercise-only and VR-EFT exercise conditions with low intensity and build up gradually to moderate intensity for the 30 min of participation and over the ninesessions of training.

Usability and Acceptability

The System Usability Scale (SUS; Brooke, 1996; Bangor et al., 2008), a validated measure of technology usability was administered at week-8 assessment to assess the usability of active VR-EFT. The client satisfaction questionnaire-8 (Larsen et al., 1979), designed to measure satisfaction with an clinical intervention, was used to assess satisfaction of our VR-EFT system at week-8 assessment.

End of Study Survey

Participants were asked to complete an author-created end of study survey. The survey included an item asking about the extent to which participants found the study’s interventions (e.g., VR plus exercise, iPad games, exercise only) helpful, with possible responses falling on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “Not helpful at all” to “Very helpful”. Afterward, participants were asked in an open-text item to expand upon any aspects of the study that they disliked or found harmful. Participants were also asked in a checklist item to indicate whether they noticed any personal changes during the study (e.g., drank less, felt better mentally, other specified changes). At the end of the survey, we asked participants if they would recommend this study to other people (yes/no), followed by an open-text item to expand upon this response. Lastly, participants were encouraged to provide suggestions for future VR and exercise studies.

Statistical Analysis

This pilot study was designed primarily to establish the feasibility of enrolling and retaining heavy drinking Veterans with TBI in an 8-week, randomized adaptive design trial of VR-EFT. To establish feasibility, we calculated study adherence and completion rate for each study condition. Results from the SUS and CSQ-8 were used to assess the usability and satisfaction of our VR-EFT system. Our secondary aim was to explore changes in performance in WM and other domains of cognitive function negatively affected by heavy alcohol use and TBI. All analyses are considered preliminary and hypothesis-generating. Therefore, the sample size was set for practical reasons and not driven by estimated effect sizes. However, power calculations based on a 0.05 alpha level reveal that 80% power (1-β) will detect a significant within group effect (VR-EFT condition) of change in cognitive function via F-testing of a medium to large effect size (0.15 ≤ Effect size f2 ≤ 0.35) and would require a total sample size of 7–33 (G-Power 3.1.9.2). In exploratory analyses, we examine if VR-EFT is associated with reduction in alcohol use (i.e., drinks per week), alcohol craving, and post-concussive symptoms.

All statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics Version 27 (IBM Corp, 2020). Baseline characteristics were compared using a one-way analysis of variance for continuous variables and the chi-square test (or Fisher’s exact test when cell sizes were small) for categorical variables. Continuously scaled repeated measures were analyzed with random-intercept linear mixed models using restricted maximum likelihood by SPSS’s MIXED procedure. An auto-regressive correlation structure was used in general linear models to adjust for repeated observations within participants. SPSS’s MIXED procedure allows for the inclusion of all available data. Therefore, no imputation methods were used to attempt to account for missing data among primary outcome variables in these modeling procedures.

Within group analyses included a fixed predictor of time; week-4 vs. week-8 for VR-EFT analyses, and baseline assessment vs. week-4 assessment for gameplay control and exercise-only conditions. In models assessing change in cognition, pre-morbid verbal intelligence (WTAR) was included as a covariate. Baseline week-0 cognitive performance was also included as a covariate in the respective model when examining VR-EFT related cognitive domain changes between week-4 and week-8 to account for potential changes in cognition during Phase I exercise-only and gameplay control conditions. Weekly drinks per week over the iteration of time associated with each condition (weeks 0–4 for exercise-only and gameplay control condition, or weeks 4–8 for VR-EFT condition) were examined as the primary variable under investigation for the alcohol use model. In the alcohol model, an aggregate of drinks per week over the 90 days prior to consent was modeled as a covariate. All analyses were intent-to-treat and used all possible observations for participants who were enrolled and completed at least one session of each condition. No imputation methods were used to attempt to account for missing data beyond endpoints in which subjects were no longer participating in the study (i.e., withdrawn and lost to follow-up). An alpha level of p ≤ 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Patient Characteristics and Baseline Cognitive Function

Baseline characteristics are shown in Table 1. Of the 30 participants, 15 were randomly assigned to the exercise-only condition and 15 to the gameplay-control condition at study enrollment. Of those, 23 participants completed phase I and entered the VR-EFT condition (phase II). The participants allocated to the three treatment conditions did not differ on any baseline characteristics or baseline cognitive domain scores (Table 2). The baseline VR-EFT scores in Table 2 reflect the 16 participants that completed the VR-EFT condition. As a measure of performance and symptom validity, we calculated the reliable digit span (RDS) and Validity-10 scale, respectively (Schroeder et al., 2012; Vanderploeg et al., 2014). Both scales showed good validity (RDS ≥ 7, Validity-10 ≤ 22, Table 1). Although not a requirement for study inclusion, all participants indicated experience using a mobile tablet for gameplay, and no participants indicated previous use of virtual reality equipment.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics (Means ± Standard Deviation).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Secondary outcome: within group effects of cognitive function.

Study Completion and Adherence

Subject flow is illustrated in Figure 2. Of the 30 participants, 28 had ≥2 lifetime TBIs. The two participants with only 1 TBI were randomized to the gameplay control condition in Phase I. Study completion for Phase I was defined as being present for the week-4 assessment visit and completion for Phase II was defined as being present for the week-8 post-treatment assessment visit. During Phase I, 11/15 (73%) completed the exercise-only bike condition, 1/15 (7%) was lost to follow-up, 1/15 (7%) moved away, and 2/15 (13%) were withdrawn due to COVID-19 outbreak/shelter-in-place restrictions. Also, in Phase I, 12/15 (80%) completed the gameplay-control condition, 2/15 (13%) were lost to follow-up and 1/15 (7%) moved away. In total, 23/30 (77%) participants completed Phase I and began Phase II. During Phase II, 16/23 (70%) completed the VR-EFT condition of the study, 2/23 (9%) dropped out due to “too much time involved” and 5/23 (22%) were withdrawn due to COVID-19 outbreak/shelter-in-place restrictions. No participants dropped out due to reported adverse effects related to study conditions. Of the 30 randomized participants, 16/30 (53%) completed the full study.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Consort flow diagram.

The mean number of sessions completed did not significantly differ between treatment conditions (p = 0.112). Participants completed 7.1 ± 2.1 exercise only sessions, 7.4 ± 2.4 gameplay control sessions, and 5.7 ± 2.8 VR-EFT sessions in each condition. Participants were asked to use the VR-EFT station, stationary bike, or mobile tablet for gameplay for at least 30 minutes per session but were allowed to discontinue or extend use as desired. Participants utilized the VR-EFT station (23.1 ± 7.1 min) for significantly less time (p = 0.02) than the mobile tablet for gameplay (27.5 ± 2.7 min). Duration of use per exercise bike session (26.0 ± 4.5 min) did not differ from VR-EFT or gameplay only conditions. Average calories burned per session were significantly (p < 0.01) higher for the exercise-only condition (97.8 ± 34.0 calories) vs. VR-EFT condition (65.0 ± 25.3 calories).

Usability and Acceptability

The VR-EFT system received a SUS rating of 70% ± 20% (45% minimum and 100% maximum). An acceptable standard is 70%, rating the system at least “moderate” (Brooke, 1996; Bangor et al., 2008). Scores of less than 50% should be cause for significant concern and are judged to be unacceptable, whereas scores 70%–90% are promising, and scores above 90% are viewed as superior (Bangor et al., 2008). Of the 16 Phase II study completers, 2/16 (12.5%) had a SUS rating below 50%, 6/16 (37.5%) rated the system between 50% and 69%, 4/16 (25%) rated the system between 70% and 90%, and 4/16 (25%) rated the system above 90%.

Client satisfaction (CSQ-8) with the VR-EFT system was rated 30.7 ± 1.8 (26 minimum and 32 maximum). A standard rating of 24 is generally considered acceptable (Larsen et al., 1979). Zero items on the CSQ-8 were rated with a score of 1 (e.g., poor) by any participant. The highest-rated was item 4, “If a friend were in need of similar help, would you recommend this intervention to them?” All participants responded, “Yes, Definitely.” No items had a mean score of less than 3.6 ± 0.63 out of a possible total item score of 4. The lowest scored item was item 6, which states “Have the services you received helped you to deal more effectively with your problems?” The average response on item 6 was between “Yes, they helped, ” to “Yes, they helped a great deal.”

End of Study Questionnaire

Responses on the end of study questionnaire indicated overall favorable impressions of VR-EFT (3.64 ± 1.45 on a 5-point scale) and gameplay control condition (3.14 ± 1.56), with the highest ratings found for exercise-only (4.07 ± 1.03). Exercise-only was rated significantly higher than gameplay control condition (p = 0.05). No other significant differences were observed. When asked about any dislikes/harmful aspects of the study, two participants reported VR-related motion sickness and one reported intermittent technical issues (e.g., “the VR was not always working properly”). Among those responding to the item about any personal changes during the study (n = 15), 20% (n = 3) reported no changes, 20% (n = 3) reported that they quit drinking, 26.67% (n = 4) reported that their appearance improved, 33.33% (n = 5) reported that they drank less, 60% (n = 9) reported that they felt better mentally, and 66.67% (n = 10) reported that they felt better physically. All respondents reported that they would recommend the study to other people, with one participant stating they believe it could help reduce drinking and others commenting on it being “interesting,” “fun,” “challenging,” and a “possible learning experience.” When asked about suggestions for future VR and exercise studies, respondents suggested moving the exercise bike to a quieter location, slowing down the speed of the game, more advanced equipment (e.g., VR controllers, exercise bike), and running the study longer than the 8-week period.

Secondary Analyses: Within Group Change in Cognitive Function

Participants in the VR-EFT condition performed significantly better on inhibition switching (F(1,14) = 7.18, p = 0.018, beta [β] = −0.90; 95% CI = −1.63 to −0.18) and visual scanning (F(1,13) = 8.05, p = 0.014, beta [β] = −1.58; 95% CI = −2.79 to −0.37) at week-8 post assessment compared to week-4 assessment (Table 2).

Participants in the exercise-only condition performed significantly better on cognitive inhibition (F(1,11) = 13.02, p = 0.004, beta [β] = −2.43; 95% CI = −3.91 to −0.95), cognitive flexibility (F(1,10) = 6.69, p = 0.027, beta [β] = −1.30; 95% CI = −2.42 to −0.18), and marginally better on processing speed (F(1, 10) = 4.89, p = 0.051, beta [β] = −0.92; 95% CI = −1.85–0.01) at week-4 assessment compared to baseline assessment (Table 2).

Participants in the gaming-control condition performed significantly better on visuospatial immediate recall (F(1,10) = 19.54, p = 0.001, beta [β] = −8.24; 95% CI = −12.36 to −4.11) and cognitive flexibility (F(1,11) = 4.82, p = 0.050, beta [β] = −1.55; 95% CI = −3.10 to −0.01) at week-4 assessment compared to baseline assessment (Table 2).

Exploratory Analyses: Within Group Change in Alcohol Use, Craving and Post-concussive Symptoms

During the exercise-only condition, participants significantly reduced number of drinks per week during weeks 1–4 of the study (F(1,43) = 8.30, p = 0.006, beta [β] = −4.30; 95% CI = −7.30 to −1.30) and reported significantly less alcohol craving (OCDS Total: F(1,10) = 26.37, p < 0.001, beta [β] = 5.32; 95% CI = 3.03–7.62; Obsessive Subscale: F(1,10) = 12.20, p = 0.006, beta [β] = 2.40; 95% CI = 0.88–3.93; Compulsive Subscale: F(1,10) = 19.85, p = 0.001, beta [β] = 2.84; 95% CI = 1.43–4.25) (Table 3). Participants in the gameplay control condition also reported less alcohol craving, but only on the compulsive subscale of the OCDS (F(1,10) = 10.20, p = 0.010, beta [β] = 1.57; 95% CI = 0.48–2.67) (Table 3). There were no significant changes in alcohol use or alcohol craving associated with the VR-EFT condition. There were no significant changes in post-concussive symptoms associated with any of the three conditions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Exploratory outcome: within group effects of alcohol use, craving, and postconcussive symptoms.

Discussion

This is the first study to examine the effects of a virtual reality cognitive training and exercise intervention on executive function, alcohol use, and post-concussive symptoms in Veterans with traumatic brain injury and alcohol use disorder. Delivering an exercise and VR-EFT intervention to U.S. Veterans with co-occurring AUD and TBI was feasible with moderate usability and high acceptability ratings. We observed a preliminary indication that VR-EFT is associated with cognitive improvement in domains related to the specific cognitive challenges of our VR-EFT condition, i.e., visual scanning and cognitive inhibition-switching. In addition, non-specific gameplay was associated with improvements in cognitive flexibility and visuospatial recall. We also observed significant improvements in cognitive flexibility and reductions in the total number of drinks per week and alcohol craving in our exercise-only condition. Although these changes may have been partially driven by natural recovery, practice effects, and nonspecific effects, such as participant expectancy, interaction with study staff, and close monitoring, these findings lend support for further investigation into the beneficial effects of VR-based cognitive training and exercise for cognitive improvement and alcohol use reduction in this understudied population of U.S. Veterans with AUD and TBI.

Heavy alcohol use can both predispose individuals to TBI and exacerbate existing TBI symptoms, thereby increasing vulnerability to future injuries (Schumm and Chard, 2012). Conversely, the cognitive impairments that accompany TBI can also increase predisposition to heavy alcohol use (Corrigan and Cole, 2008; Polusny et al., 2011; Jorge et al., 2019). The 30 Veteran participants in this study reported 123, mostly mild TBI’s (n = 25, 75%), a notably high TBI count, especially for a sample with minimal combat exposure (n = 5/30, 17%). Only 2/30 (7%) reported a single TBI. A comprehensive TBI evaluation includes reviewing a history of TBI occurrence over the lifetime. Many of the injuries reported in the current sample are from falls, blunt traumas, and motor vehicle accidents that occurred either pre- or post-military service. In the general population, 30%–50% of patients treated for TBI report intoxication at the time of injury with binge drinking being a major risk factor for falls and accidents that are associated with brain trauma (for review see Weil et al., 2018). In general, the rate of TBIs among those with substance use disorders is high, although the true incidence is unknown because there is no standard practice for evaluating TBI in substance use treatment settings (Miller, 2018). The high rates of TBI in this study sample highlight the importance of assessing for TBI in AUD treatment settings and developing interventions such as VR-EFT to address the unique and overlapping symptoms associated with TBI and AUD in this vulnerable population.

VR-EFT, which incorporated physical exercise, was generally well-tolerated and feasible to use. Although the VR-EFT condition involved the use of additional hardware and required additional steps to set up and use than the other two conditions (gameplay- and exercise-only), the total number of sessions that participants completed did not significantly differ between conditions. However, participants did spend about 4.5 minutes more using games delivered on the mobile tablet than they did utilizing the VR-EFT station, even though VR-EFT was indicated as being more helpful than tablet-delivered games. Participants did not spend significantly more time using the exercise bike than they did using VR-EFT or playing games on the mobile tablet, even though the exercise-only condition was rated as the most helpful of the three interventions. Participants did burn more calories during the exercise-only condition compared to exercise plus VR-EFT. The additional complexity of completing cognitive training in VR while on an exercise bike likely contributed to decreased bike intensity. This was contrary to the intended design, which may have contributed to diminished cognitive improvement during the VR-EFT condition.

VR-EFT was associated with motion sickness in 12.5% of participants (n = 2/16), an adverse event that is not uncommon during VR use (Kolasinski, 2014). Blue Goji’s GoWings Safari game is designed with vertical movement by the user’s game character. As bicycle speed increases and reduces, the user’s gaming character rises and falls in 3-D space from the perspective of the user. Motion sickness in VR is thought to be related to sensory conflicts when a user’s perception of self-motion is incongruent with sensory inputs from the vestibular system (Reason and Brand, 1975; Johnson, 2005; Kolasinski, 2014). Forward movement in Blue Goji’s GoWings Safari is associated with the speed of stationary bike pedaling as well as rising and falling, the latter of which is not congruent with typical biking experience or vestibular input from the user’s perspective. This may have resulted in complaints of motion sickness. Future VR-based cognitive training tasks may want to pay special consideration to motions/movements that can be disorienting to the user to improve user satisfaction and desire to engage with the training task.

Twenty percent or more of respondents that completed the VR-EFT reported that they quit drinking or drank less, their appearance improved, and that they felt better mentally and physically. All respondents noted that they would recommend exercise plus VR-EFT to other people, stating that it was “interesting,” “fun,” and “challenging.” In fact, the system received a rating of 70% on the system usability scale which is considered “promising” by industry standards, and a score of 30.7 on the client satisfaction questionnaire, which is close to the highest possible score of 32 (Larsen et al., 1979; Brooke, 1996; Bangor et al., 2008). Intermittent technical issues, distractions in the physical environment, speed of the game, and issues with equipment (e.g., VR controllers not always working and exercise bikes not easily syncing with the game) all contributed to negative aspects of the participants’ experience. Improving user experience and reducing the occurrence of technical issues may likely enhance engagement with VR-based cognitive training interventions.

The VR-EFT condition was associated with near-transfer effects to two cognitive domains that our training targeted, visual scanning and cognitive inhibition-switching. Neither of these cognitive domains was associated with improvement during Phase I of the study that exposed participants to exercise-only or the gameplay control condition. The primary tasks in GoWings Safari are to scan the visual environment, inhibit and switch, or activate a tool (e.g., camera or net blaster) to capture a target (e.g., animal or “poacher”). Improvements in both visual scanning and inhibition-switching during Phase II and not Phase I may reflect the specific training received during our VR-EFT condition and are less likely related to practice or other non-specific effects of the study. Particularly as the measures used in the study are well-validated standardized instruments that utilize alternative forms when necessary. Still, given the lack of direct group comparison, the cognitive gains observed during Phase II may be due to other non-specific effects and cannot be ruled out.

Visual scanning was measured using a visual attention task (Delis et al., 2007) and studies consistently demonstrate that many people with SUD exhibit cognitive deficits in attention (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019). Long-term alcohol use is associated with deficits in visual scanning (Ciesielski et al., 1995), and accuracy during attention is a consistent predictor of treatment retention (Dominguez-Salas et al., 2016) and relapse (Rolland et al., 2019) in SUD studies. Inhibition-switching is also consistently associated with deficits in SUD (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019) and these deficits are associated with lower quality of life improvement during early treatment (Rubenis et al., 2018). There have been a small number of studies that have focused on inhibition control training (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019) that have shown modest reductions in alcohol use, but none have been conducted among treatment seekers (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2019). Our study represents the first VR-EFT study that targets inhibition-switching among a sample of treatment-seeking U.S. Veterans.

Similarly, cognitive bias modification (CBM) studies have targeted attentional bias training for substance-related cues. One attentional bias alcohol study has shown a delay in time to relapse (but not relapse rates) relative to controls who received sham training (Schoenmakers et al., 2010) and a second study showed reduced alcohol relapse rates at a one-year follow-up relative to a sham training condition in a sample of AUD treatment seekers (Rinck et al., 2018). These studies were attempting to train attentional bias specific to alcohol-related cues and were different from the current study in that the target of VR-EFT was a general attentional skill not associated with specific alcohol-related imagery. Studies of attentional bias modification have thus far shown evidence for reduced AUD symptoms, but not evidence for reductions in attentional bias (Heitmann et al., 2018). Our study provides preliminary evidence showing that attentional scanning can be targeted with a VR-based general attentional training approach vs. an attentional bias targeting a specific substance. However, our small pilot study did not show far transfer effects, i.e., an indication for reduction in alcohol use or alcohol craving during and immediately following VR-EFT.

During Phase I, we also saw significant improvement in cognitive inhibition associated with the exercise-only condition, but not gameplay control. A recent meta-analysis reported small to moderate effects of exercise on executive function, including inhibitory control in healthy populations (Ludyga et al., 2016). We also saw a significant reduction in alcohol use and alcohol craving associated with exercise-only condition in our study. Among people with methamphetamine use disorder exercise has been shown to improve inhibitory control and reduce craving (Wang et al., 2015), but findings for the benefits of exercise for AUD have only consistently demonstrated improved mood and physical health outcomes, but not reduced alcohol use, alcohol craving, or improved cognitive function (Zschucke et al., 2012; Hallgren et al., 2017). This lack of effect may be due to the limited number of longitudinal, well-controlled studies on the impact of exercise on AUD symptom outcomes, including cognition (Hallgren et al., 2017). As such, there is still enthusiasm for continued work in understanding the benefits and potential mechanisms underlying neurological improvement associated with exercise in AUD (Colledge et al., 2018). Our findings lend enthusiasm and support for this continued evaluation.

Significant improvement in visuospatial immediate recall was only evident in the gameplay-control condition during Phase I. Although this condition was not designed to necessarily enhance executive function, it still required utilizing visuospatial skills to use and improve gameplay across sessions. It is not unusual for videogames which rely on the utilization of complex visuospatial skills to result in improvement in visuospatial memory (Basak et al., 2008), and our gameplay control condition likely had a similar effect. In fact, improvement in visuospatial recall during the gameplay control condition may have washed out any expected VR-EFT effects on visuospatial recall during Phase II. Improvement in visuospatial recall was not the target of VR-EFT, but nonetheless should be examined in future trials that compare these conditions in head-to-head challenge vs. a phased approach that may wash out VR-EFT specific findings.

Finally, we also observed significant improvement in cognitive flexibility for all participants during Phase I. Given the non-specific effect of improvement by condition in cognitive flexibility, it is likely that improvement in this domain was related to practice effects rather than true improvement associated with the condition (Lemay et al., 2004; Pietrzak et al., 2010; Lubrini et al., 2020; Sicard et al., 2020). Managing practice effects is a major issue for future cognitive training trials to contend with.

Our study had some major limitations. Namely, this was a relatively small pilot study that was designed primarily to evaluate the feasibility and usability of a VR-EFT plus exercise intervention and was not designed to definitively evaluate the effect of VR-EFT vs. other interventions on cognitive function or symptoms of AUD and TBI. As such, the associations observed in cognitive function and symptom improvement are considered preliminary and hypotheses generating only. Our small sample size also did not allow for the consideration of gender identity, sex, age, or race/ethnicity in the analyses or include a longitudinal analysis of cognition or TBI/AUD related symptoms, both important areas for future research. An additional limitation of this report is the reliance on self-report measures to assess TBI symptoms and alcohol use—although self-report at present remains the standard for alcohol use outcome measurement in clinical trials (Falk et al., 2010; Fertig et al., 2012; Litten et al., 2012). Of note, we did not include a measure of the quality of life related to TBI, a valuable and common assessment among TBI studies.

There is also a high likelihood that this population may have one or more of many common co-occurring conditions in addition to AUD and TBI, including PTSD, depression, anxiety, chronic pain, and problematic substance use (Corrigan and Cole, 2008; Stein et al., 2015; Kulas and Rosenheck, 2018; Miller, 2018), which can also increase the risk for alcohol-related concerns (Taylor et al., 2012; Wisco et al., 2014; Roberts et al., 2016; Miller, 2018). We did not evaluate symptoms associated with these common co-occurring conditions on intervention feasibility, usability, satisfaction, or other exploratory outcomes (e.g., cognitive function, alcohol use), as it fell outside the scope of this small pilot study. Understanding the high co-occurrence of PTSD and TBI among Veterans, future studies should examine the impact of comorbid PTSD on these outcomes. Finally, we did not require a specific fitness level or stratify randomization on fitness level at study entry. As seen in Table 1, the exercise-only group had much lower Leisure Activity Scores than the gameplay control condition. Although this likely did not impact the results of the current study, future studies examining the benefits of exercise combined with cognitive training should take great care in addressing activity levels at study entry.

Despite these limitations, the study was successful in establishing the feasibility, acceptability, and usability of conducting a VR-EFT and exercise study among Veterans with co-occurring TBI and AUD. VR-related adverse events were minimal, usability was at least acceptable, satisfaction was high, and adherence and retention were generally good among all conditions. We expect that retention would have been greatly improved had it not been for COVID-19 related withdrawals. All of this lends support for the successful completion of a larger VR-EFT trial examining the additional benefits that physical activity may have on cognitive training.

In sum, we established preliminary indication that exercise combined with VR-EFT is associated with improvement in executive function domains that were targeted in as little as 3 weeks and nine sessions of exposure. In addition, there was an indication that exercise-only was associated with improvement in executive function and reductions in alcohol use and craving. These findings can be used to inform the development of a larger definitive study, one that also includes a head-to-head challenge vs. a phased approach, and one that examines longitudinal far-transfer effects (i.e., 6–12-month rates of alcohol use and alcohol craving). We would also recommend that future studies of physically active VR-based cognitive training preemptively plan to assess pre-treatment cognitive function, baseline fitness level, and examine the effect of delivering personalized VR-EFT focusing on each participant’s unique needs. In sum, the results of this study indicate the need for a larger controlled investigation to more definitively assess the efficacy of physically active VR-EFT to enhance treatment outcomes among treatment-seeking individuals with co-occurring AUD and TBI.

Data Availability Statement

Restrictions apply to the datasets: the datasets for this manuscript are not publicly available, because they were created as part of Veteran’s Administration approved research. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the primary author DP, david.pennington2@va.gov.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of California, San Francisco Institutional Review Board, San Francisco Veterans Affairs Healthcare System, VA Research and Development Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors have contributed to design and preparation of the manuscript. DP for conceptualization, funding acquisition, resources, supervision, writing (initial draft, review, and editing), clinical data management, and data analysis. JR for writing (initial draft, review, and editing) and clinical data management. MC, EW, and SB for clinical data management and completion of the RCT. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by a Grant Support Investigators Award: UCSF Under-Represented Faculty in Clinical and Translational Research: (Pennington); Department of VA Clinical Science Research & Development: IK2CX001510 (Pennington); National Institutes of Health (NIH), National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA): DA039903 (Pennington); NIH, NIDA: T32DA007250 (Cano); and by San Francisco VA Healthcare System resources.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We extend our appreciation to all who volunteered for this research. We thank the Addiction and Recovery Treatment Service personnel at the San Francisco VA Health Care System for critical help with participant recruitment. We also wish to thank Brooke Lasher, Nicole Bautista, Hala El Bahraoui, Edie Harris, and Siqi Song for participant recruitment and assessment as well as Dr. Ellen Herbst for reviewing and conducting assessments for physical readiness for exercise.

References

American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (ACRM), Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Committee of the Head Injury Interdisciplinary Special Interest Group (1993). Definition of mild traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 8, 86–87. doi: 10.1097/00001199-199309000-00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force (2013). Diagnostic Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5TM. (5th Edn). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson-Hanley, C., Arciero , P. J., Westen, S. C., Nimon, J. and Zimmerman, E. (2012). Neuropsychological benefits of stationary bike exercise and a cybercycle exergame for older adults with diabetes: an exploratory analysis. J. Diabetes Sci. Technol. 6, 849–857. doi: 10.1177/193229681200600416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anton, R. F., Moak, D. H., and Latham, P. (1995). The obsessive compulsive drinking scale: a self-rated instrument for the quantification of thoughts about alcohol and drinking behavior. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 92–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01475.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashdown-Franks, G., Firth, J., Carney, R., Carvalho, A. F., Hallgren, M., Koyanagi, A., et al. (2020). Exercise as medicine for mental and substance use disorders: a meta-review of the benefits for neuropsychiatric and cognitive outcomes. Sports Med. 50, 151–170. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01187-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babor, T. F., Higgins-Biddle, J. C., Saunders, J. B., and Monteiro, M. G. (2001). AUDIT: The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288, 2271–2281. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Kraft, E., Santana, S., and Tziraki, C. (2015). Maintaining older brain functionality: a targeted review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 55, 453–477. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamidis, P. D., Vivas, A. B., Styliadis, C., Frantzidis, C., Klados, M., Schlee, W., et al. (2014). A review of physical and cognitive interventions in aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 206–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangor, A., Kortum, P. T., and Miller, J. T. (2008). An empirical evaluation of the System Usability Scale. Int. J. Hum. Comp. Interact. 24, 574–594. doi: 10.1080/10447310802205776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, J. N., and Corkery, A. T. (2018). Exercise improves vascular function, but does this translate to the brain? Brain Plast. 4, 65–79. doi: 10.3233/BPL-180075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol. Aging 23, 765–777. doi: 10.1037/a0013494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, M. E., Buckman, J. F., and Nguyen, T. T. (2013). A role for cognitive rehabilitation in increasing the effectiveness of treatment for alcohol use disorders. Neuropsychol. Rev. 23, 27–47. doi: 10.1007/s11065-013-9228-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belanger, H. G., Uomoto, J. M., and Vanderploeg, R. D. (2009). The Veterans Health Administration system of care for mild traumatic brain injury: costs, benefits and controversies. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 24, 4–13. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181957032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedict, R. (1997). Brief Visual Memory Test Revised. Odessa, FL: PAR Inc.

Google Scholar

Bjork, J. M., and Grant, S. J. (2009). Does traumatic brain injury increase risk for substance abuse? J. Neurotrauma 26, 1077–1082. doi: 10.1089/neu.2008.0849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogner, J. A., Corrigan, J. D., Mysiw, W. J., Clinchot, D., and Fugate, L. (2001). A comparison of substance abuse and violence in the prediction of long-term rehabilitation outcomes after traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 82, 571–577. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.22340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, K. T., Tuerk, P., Back, S. E., Saladin, M. E., Waldrop, A. E., and Myrick, H. (2009). Combat posttraumatic stress disorder, substance use disorders and traumatic brain injury. J. Addict. Med. 3, 179–188. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3181aa244f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, J., and Benedict, R. (2001). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Services.

Google Scholar

Brooke, J. (1996). “SUS—a quick and dirty usability scale,” in Usability Evaluation in Industry, eds P. W. Jordan, B. Thomas, B. A. Weerdmeester and I. L. McClelland (London: Taylor & Francis), 189–194.

Google Scholar

Cicerone, K. D., Goldin, Y., Ganci, K., Rosenbaum, A., Wethe, J. V., Langenbahn, D. M., et al. (2019). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: systematic review of the literature from 2009 through 2014. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 100, 1515–1533. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2019.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciesielski, K. T., Waldorf, A. V., and Jung, R. E., Jr. (1995). Anterior brain deficits in chronic alcoholism: cause or effect? J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 183, 756–761. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199512000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 3316–3321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400266101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colledge, F., Ludyga, S., Mücke, M., Pühse, U., and Gerber, M. (2018). The effects of an acute bout of exercise on neural activity in alcohol and cocaine craving: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials 19:713. doi: 10.1186/s13063-018-3062-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigan, J. D. (1995). Substance abuse as a mediating factor in outcome from traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 76, 302–309. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(95)80654-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigan, J. D., and Cole, T. B. (2008). Substance use disorders and clinical management of traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder. JAMA 300, 720–721. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.6.720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J., Op den Buysch, H. O., Joris Noens, I. L., and van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2007). D-KEFS: Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Trail Making Test. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Test Publishers.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., and Holdnack, J. (2004). Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan executive function system: an update. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 301–303. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Department of Veterans Affairs. (2007). Screening and Evaluation of Possible Traumatic Brain Injury in Operation Enduring Freedom (OEF) and Operation Iraqi Freedom (OIF) Veterans. Washington, DC: Department of Veterans Affairs, Veterans Health Administration.

Google Scholar

Dominguez-Salas, S., Díaz-Batanero, C., Lozano-Rojas, O. M., and Verdejo-García, A. (2016). Impact of general cognition and executive function deficits on addiction treatment outcomes: systematic review and discussion of neurocognitive pathways. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 772–801. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drubach, D. A., Kelly, M. P., Winslow, M. M., and Flynn, J. P. (1993). Substance abuse as a factor in the causality, severity and recurrence rate of traumatic brain injury. Md. Med. J. 42, 989–993.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Falk, D., Wang, X. Q., Liu, L., Fertig, J., Mattson, M., Ryan, M., et al. (2010). Percentage of subjects with no heavy drinking days: evaluation as an efficacy endpoint for alcohol clinical trials. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 2022–2034. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01290.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fertig, J. B., Ryan, M. L., Falk, D. E., Litten, R. Z., Mattson, M. E., Ransom, J., et al. (2012). A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial assessing the efficacy of levetiracetam extended-release in very heavy drinking alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 1421–1430. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01716.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamito, P., Oliveira, J., Matias, M., Cunha, E., Brito, R., Lopes, P. F., et al. (2021). Virtual reality cognitive training among individuals with alcohol use disorder undergoing residential treatment: pilot randomized controlled trial. J. Med. Internet Res. 23:e18482. doi: 10.2196/18482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giménez-Meseguer, J., Tortosa-Martínez, J., and Cortell-Tormo, J. (2020). The benefits of physical exercise on mental disorders and quality of life in substance use disorders patients. Systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:3680. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17103680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godin, G. (2011). The Godin-Shephard leisure-time physical activity questionnaire. Health Fitness J. Can. 4, 18–22. doi: 10.14288/hfjc.v4i1.82

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gros, D. F., Korte, K. J., Horner, M. D., Brady, K. T., and Back, S. E. (2016). Co-occurring traumatic brian injury, PTSD symptoms and alcohol use in veterans. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 38, 266–273. doi: 10.1007/s10862-015-9513-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallgren, M., Herring, M. P., Vancampfort, D., Hoang, M. T., Andersson, V., Andreasson, S., et al. (2021). Changes in craving following acute aerobic exercise in adults with alcohol use disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 142, 243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallgren, M., Vancampfort, D., Giesen, E. S., Lundin, A., and Stubbs, B. (2017). Exercise as treatment for alcohol use disorders: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 51, 1058–1064. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampshire, A., Sandrone, S., and Hellyer, P. J. (2019). A large-scale, cross-sectional investigation into the efficacy of brain training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:221. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heitmann, J., Bennik, E. C., van Hemel-Ruiter, M. E., and de Jong, P. J. (2018). The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in adults: a systematic review. Syst. Rev. 7:160. doi: 10.1186/s13643-018-0822-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrold, A. A., Sander, A. M., Wilson, K. V., Scimeca, L. M., Cobia, D. J., and Breiter, H. C. (2015). Dual diagnosis of traumatic brain injury and alcohol use disorder: characterizing clinical and neurobiological underpinnings. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2, 273–284. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0078-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hibbard, M. R., Uysal, S., Kepler, K., Bogdany, J., and Silver, J. (1998). Axis I psychopathology in individuals with traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 13, 24–39. doi: 10.1097/00001199-199808000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Motl, R. W., Pontifex, M. B., Posthuma, D., Stubbe, J. H., Boomsma, D. I., et al. (2006). Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. Health Psychol. 25, 678–687. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Friese, M., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2011). “Working memory and self-regulation,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory and Applications, eds K. D. Vohs and R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 204–225.

Google Scholar

Hoffman, H. G., Patterson, D. R., Magula, J., Carrougher, G. J., Zeltzer, K., Dagadakis, S., et al. (2004). Water-friendly virtual reality pain control during wound care. J. Clin. Psychol. 60, 189–195. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoge, C. W., Castro, C. A., Messer, S. C., McGurk, D., Cotting, D. I., and Koffman, R. L. (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems and barriers to care. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 13–22. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa040603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., Cox, A. L., Engel, C. C., and Castro, C. A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. N. Engl. J. Med. 358, 453–463. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa072972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., Wiers, R. W., and Jansen, A. (2011). Resisting temptation: decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug Alcohol Depend. 116, 132–136. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, S., Tucker, L., Wu, C., and Yang, L. (2020). Beneficial effects of exercise on depression and anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic: a narrative review. Front. Psychiatry 11:587557. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.587557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp. (2020). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Ilkowska, M., and Engle, R. W. (2010). “Working memory capacity and self-regulation,” in Handbook of Personality and Self-Regulation, ed R. H. Hoyle (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell) pp. 263–290. doi: 10.1002/9781444318111.ch12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, K., Nielsen, C., Ekstrøm, C. T., and Roessler, K. K. (2019). Physical exercise in the treatment of alcohol use disorder (AUD) patients affects their drinking habits: a randomized controlled trial. Scand. J. Public Health 47, 462–468. doi: 10.1177/1403494818759842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, D. (2005). Introduction to and Review of Simulator Sickness Research (Research Report 1832). Fort Belvoir, VA: U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences.

Google Scholar

Jorge, R. E., Li, R., Liu, X., McGavin, J. K., Shorter, D. I., Acion, L., et al. (2019). Treating alcohol use disorder in U.S. veterans: the role of traumatic brain injury. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 31, 319–327. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.18110250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joubert, C., and Chainay, H. (2018). Aging brain: the effect of combined cognitive and physical training on cognition as compared to cognitive and physical training alone – a systematic review. Clin. Interv. Aging 13, 1267–1301. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S165399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kable, J. W., Caulfield, M. K., Falcone, M., McConnell, M., Bernardo, L., Parthasarathi, T., et al. (2017). No effect of commercial cognitive training on brain activity, choice behavior, or cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 37, 7390–7402. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2832-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolasinski, E. (2014). Simulator Sickness in Virtual Reality Environments (ARI 1027). Fort Belvoir, VA: U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences.

Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R., et al. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 400, 418–419. doi: 10.1038/22682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreutzer, J. S., Witol, A. D., and Marwitz, J. H. (1996). Alcohol and drug use among young persons with traumatic brain injury. J. Learn. Disabil. 29, 643–651. doi: 10.1177/002221949602900608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulas, J. F., and Rosenheck, R. A. (2018). A comparison of veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder, with mild traumatic brain injury and with both disorders: understanding multimorbidity. Mil. Med. 183, e114–e122. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usx050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, D. L., Attkisson, C. C., Hargreaves, W. A., and Nguyen, T. D. (1979). Assessment of client/patient satisfaction: development of a general scale. Eval. Program Plann. 2, 197–207. doi: 10.1016/0149-7189(79)90094-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauenroth, A., Ioannidis, A. E., and Teichmann, B. (2016). Influence of combined physical and cognitive training on cognition: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 16:141. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0315-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, L. L. F., Barnett, F., Yau, M. K., and Gray, M. A. (2014). Effects of combined cognitive and exercise interventions on cognition in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Ageing Res. Rev. 15, 61–75. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lechner, W. V., Sidhu, N. K., Kittaneh, A. A., and Anand, A. (2019). Interventions with potential to target executive function deficits in addiction: current state of the literature. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 30, 24–28. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemay, S., Bédard, M.-A., Rouleau, I., and Tremblay, P.-L. G. (2004). Practice effect and test-retest reliability of attentional and executive tests in middle-aged to elderly subjects. Clin. Neuropsychol. 18, 284–302. doi: 10.1080/13854040490501718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litten, R. Z., Fertig, J. B., Falk, D. E., Ryan, M. L., Mattson, M. E., Collins, J. F., et al. (2012). A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial to assess the efficacy of quetiapine fumarate XR in very heavy-drinking alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 406–416. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01649.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubrini, G., Viejo-Sobera, R., Periáñez, J. A., Cicuendez, M., Castaño, A. M., González-Marqués, J., et al. (2020). Evolution of cognitive impairment after a traumatic brain injury: is there any improvement after controlling the practice effect? Rev. Neurol. 70, 37–44. doi: 10.33588/rn.7002.2019233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., and Pühse, U. (2016). Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: a meta-analysis: moderate exercise and executive function. Psychophysiology 53, 1611–1626. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, S. (2018). The ASAM Principles of Addiction Medicine. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

National Center for Health Statistics (2019). Estimates of Mental Health Symptomatology, By Month of Interview: United States, 2019. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics, Released 03/2021.

Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., et al. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature 465, 775–778. doi: 10.1038/nature09042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, T. D., and Rizzo, A. A. (2008). Affective outcomes of virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety and specific phobias: a meta-analysis. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 39, 250–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, D. L., Bielenberg, J., Lasher, B., Herbst, E., Abrams, G., Novakovic-Agopian, T., et al. (2020). A randomized pilot trial of topiramate for alcohol use disorder in veterans with traumatic brain injury: effects on alcohol use, cognition and post-concussive symptoms. Drug Alcohol Depend. 214:108149. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C. (2017). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor, depression and physical activity: making the neuroplastic connection. Neural Plast. 2017:7260130. doi: 10.1155/2017/7260130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietrzak, R. H., Snyder, P. J., and Maruff, P. (2010). Amphetamine-related improvement in executive function in patients with chronic schizophrenia is modulated by practice effects. Schizophr. Res. 124, 176–182. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polusny, M. A., Kehle, S. M., Nelson, N. W., Erbes, C. R., Arbisi, P. A., and Thuras, P. (2011). Longitudinal effects of mild traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder comorbidity on postdeployment outcomes in National Guard Soldiers deployed to Iraq. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 79–89. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahe, J., Petrelli, A., Kaesberg, S., Fink, G. R., Kessler, J., and Kalbe, E. (2015). Effects of cognitive training with additional physical activity compared to pure cognitive training in healthy older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 10, 297–310. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S74071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichlen, D. A., and Alexander, G. E. (2017). Adaptive capacity: an evolutionary neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition and brain health. Trends Neurosci. 40, 408–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reason, J. T., and Brand, J. J. (1975). Motion Sickness. London; New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Rinck, M., Wiers, R. W., Becker, E. S., and Lindenmeyer, J. (2018). Relapse prevention in abstinent alcoholics by cognitive bias modification: clinical effects of combining approach bias modification and attention bias modification. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 86, 1005–1016. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, N. P., Roberts, P. A., Jones, N., and Bisson, J. I. (2016). Psychological therapies for posttraumatic stress disorder and comorbid substance use disorder. Cochrane Database Sys. Rev. 4:CD010204. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010204.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roessler, K. K., Bilberg, R., Søgaard Nielsen, A., Jensen, K., Ekstrøm, C. T., and Sari, S. (2017). Exercise as adjunctive treatment for alcohol use disorder: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 12:e0186076. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolland, B., D’Hondt, F., Montègue, S., Brion, M., Peyron, E., D’Aviau de Ternay, J., et al. (2019). A patient-tailored evidence-based approach for developing early neuropsychological training programs in addiction settings. Neuropsychol. Rev. 29, 103–115. doi: 10.1007/s11065-018-9395-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubenis, A. J., Fitzpatrick, R. E., Lubman, D. I., and Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2018). Impulsivity predicts poorer improvement in quality of life during early treatment for people with methamphetamine dependence. Addiction 113, 668–676. doi: 10.1111/add.14058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenmakers, T. M., de Bruin, M., Lux, I. F. M., Goertz, A. G., Van Kerkhof, D. H. A. T., and Wiers, R. W. (2010). Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 109, 30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, R. W., Twumasi-Ankrah, P., Baade, L. E., and Marshall, P. S. (2012). Reliable digit span: a systematic review and cross-validation study. Assessment 19, 21–30. doi: 10.1177/1073191111428764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumm, J. A., and Chard, K. M. (2012). Alcohol and stress in the military. Alcohol Res. 34, 401–407.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sicard, V., Simard, A., Moore, R. D., and Ellemberg, D. (2020). Practice effect associated with the serial administration of the switch task and its implications in the assessment of sports-related concussion. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 42, 965–973. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2020.1828836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. J., and Ainslie, P. N. (2017). Regulation of cerebral blood flow and metabolism during exercise. Exp. Physiol. 102, 1356–1371. doi: 10.1113/EP086249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. J., and Merwin, R. M. (2021). The role of exercise in management of mental health disorders: an integrative review. Annu. Rev. Med. 72, 45–62. doi: 10.1146/annurev-med-060619-022943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobell, L. C., and Sobell, M. B. (1992). “Timeline follow-back,” in Measuring Alcohol Consumption, eds R. Z. Litten, and J. P. Allen (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press), 41–72. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-0357-5_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., Maisto, S. A., and Cooper, A. M. (1985). Time-Line Follow-Back Assessment Method. National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse.

Google Scholar

Stanmore, E., Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., de Bruin, E. D., and Firth, J. (2017). The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, M. B., Kessler, R. C., Heeringa, S. G., Jain, S., Campbell-Sills, L., Colpe, L. J., et al. (2015). Prospective longitudinal evaluation of the effect of deployment-acquired traumatic brain injury on posttraumatic stress and related disorders: results from the army study to assess risk and resilience in service members (Army STARRS). Am. J. Psychiatry 172, 1101–1111. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14121572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szuhany, K. L., Bugatti, M., and Otto, M. W. (2015). A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J. Psychiatr. Res. 60, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, B. C., Hagel, E. M., Carlson, K. F., Cifu, D. X., Cutting, A., Bidelspach, D. E., et al. (2012). Prevalence and costs of cooccurring traumatic brain injury with and without psychiatric disturbance and pain among Afghanistan and Iraq War Veteran VA users. Med. Care 50, 342–346. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e318245a558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theill, N., Schumacher, V., Adelsberger, R., Martin, M., and Jäncke, L. (2013). Effects of simultaneously performed cognitive and physical training in older adults. BMC Neurosci. 14:103. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-14-103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderploeg, R. D., Cooper, D. B., Belanger, H. G., Donnell, A. J., Kennedy, J. E., Hopewell, C. A., et al. (2014). Screening for postdeployment conditions: development and cross-validation of an embedded validity scale in the neurobehavioral symptom inventory. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 29, 1–10. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e318281966e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venegas, J., and Clark, E. (2011). “Wechsler test of adult reading,” in Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology, eds J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, and B. Caplan (New York, NY: Springer), 2693–2694. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lorenzetti, V., Manning, V., Piercy, H., Bruno, R., Hester, R., et al. (2019). A roadmap for integrating neuroscience into addiction treatment: a consensus of the neuroscience interest group of the international society of addiction medicine. Front. Psychiatry 10:877. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, J. J., and Tschakovsky, M. E. (2018). Exercise and circulating BDNF: mechanisms of release and implications for the design of exercise interventions. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 43, 1095–1104. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2018-0192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D., Zhou, C., and Chang, Y.-K. (2015). Acute exercise ameliorates craving and inhibitory deficits in methamphetamine: an ERP study. Physiol. Behav. 147, 38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wanmaker, S., Leijdesdorff, S. M. J., Geraerts, E., van de Wetering, B. J. M., Renkema, P. J., and Franken, I. H. A. (2018). The efficacy of a working memory training in substance use patients: a randomized double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trial. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 473–486. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1372367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warburton, D. E. R., Jamnik, V. K., Bredin, S. S. D., and Gledhill, N. (2011). The physical activity readiness questionnaire (PAR-Q+) and electronic physical activity readiness medical examination (ePARmed-X+). Health Fitness J. Can. 4, 3–23. doi: 10.14288/hfjc.v4i2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. doi: 10.1037/t15169-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weil, Z. M., Corrigan, J. D., and Karelina, K. (2018). Alcohol use disorder and traumatic brain injury. Alcohol Res. 39, 171–180.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Willenbring, M. L., Massey, S. H., and Gardner, M. B. (2009). Helping patients who drink too much: an evidence-based guide for primary care clinicians. Am. Fam. Physician 80, 44–50.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/bf03196322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wisco, B. E., Marx, B. P., Wolf, E. J., Miller, M. W., Southwick, S. M., and Pietrzak, R. H. (2014). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the U.S. veteran population: results from the national health and resilience in veterans study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 75, 1338–1346. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, J.-H. (2020). The psychological effects of water-based exercise in older adults: an integrative review. Geriatr. Nursing 41, 717–723. doi: 10.1016/j.gerinurse.2020.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zolyniak, N., Schulte-Göcking, H., and Kraft, E. (2014). Neuroplasticity in aging: implications for behavioral and lifestyle combined interventions. Top. Geriatr. Rehabil. 30, 15–17. doi: 10.1097/TGR.0000000000000004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zschucke, E., Heinz, A., and Ströhle, A. (2012). Exercise and physical activity in the therapy of substance use disorders. Sci. World J. 2012:901741. doi: 10.1100/2012/901741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual reality, executive function, cognition, traumatic brain injury, alcohol use disorder, cognitive training, exercise, U.S. veterans

Citation: Pennington DL, Reavis JV, Cano MT, Walker E and Batki SL (2022) The Impact of Exercise and Virtual Reality Executive Function Training on Cognition Among Heavy Drinking Veterans With Traumatic Brain Injury: A Pilot Feasibility Study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:802711. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.802711

Received: 27 October 2021; Accepted: 22 February 2022;
Published: 22 March 2022.

Edited by:

Cole Vonder Haar, The Ohio State University, United States

Reviewed by:

M. Alexandra Kredlow, Harvard University, United States
Maya Troyanskaya, Baylor College of Medicine, United States

Copyright © 2022 Pennington, Reavis, Cano, Walker and Batki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David L. Pennington, david.pennington2@va.gov; david.pennington@ucsf.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.