Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 01 July 2022
Sec. Pathological Conditions
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.924319

Application of Cognitive Bias Testing in Neuropsychiatric Disorders: A Mini-Review Based on Animal Studies

  • 1CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China

Cognitive biases can arise from cognitive processing under affective states and reflect the impact of emotion on cognition. In animal studies, the existing methods for detecting animal emotional state are still relatively limited, and cognitive bias test has gradually become an important supplement. In recent years, its effectiveness in animal research related to neuropsychiatric disorders has been widely verified. Some studies have found that cognitive bias test is more sensitive than traditional test methods such as forced swimming test and sucrose preference test in detecting emotional state. Therefore, it has great potential to become an important tool to measure the influence of neuropsychiatric disorder-associated emotions on cognitive processing. Moreover, it also can be used in early drug screening to effectively assess the potential effects or side effects of drugs on affective state prior to clinical trials. In this mini-review, we summarize the application of cognitive bias tests in animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders such as depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder, and pain. We also discussed its critical value in the identification of neuropsychiatric disorders and the validation of therapeutic approaches.

Introduction

Emotions can cause the brain to distort the truth, leading to a discrepancy between what we believe is true and reality. Cognitive bias is the tendency of the brain to process information in favor of certain emotional valence (Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). Positive emotions lead to positive cognitive biases, while negative emotions cause negative biases, affecting multiple cognitive processes such as attention, memory, and decision-making (Everaert et al., 2012). The phenomenon of cognitive bias is widespread, especially in neuropsychiatric disorders. The concept of “cognitive bias” was first proposed by Beck in the study of patients with depression (Beck, 1967). Based on Beck’s theory, early adverse experiences can trigger negative cognitive schemas leading to negative views of the self, the world, and the future, which in turn lead to biases in cognitive processing (Segal, 1988). According to Bower’s theory of mood congruity (Bower, 1981), during cognitive processing, individuals tend to focus, process, and recall information that is consistent with their emotional state, resulting in cognitive biases.

Cognitive biases can be divided into three types: attentional bias, interpretation bias, and memory bias. Attentional bias indicates that individuals are more likely to allocate attention to stimuli consistent with their current emotional state (Mennen et al., 2019). In animal research, attentional bias can be investigated by analyzing the behavioral response to threatening stimuli (Lee et al., 2016; Luo et al., 2019). Interpretation bias affects decision-making processes. Individuals are more likely to interpret ambiguous cues to be consistent with their current affective state (Everaert, 2021). Interpretation bias in animal research is often measured using the judgment bias test (JBT) (Nguyen et al., 2020), which relies on certain behaviors (like bar-pressing) and these results are then interpreted with respect to certain human constructs, one of them being “attitude” (see more details in Table 1). For example, animals in a more positive affective state tend to interpret ambiguous cues in a more positive way. Memory bias is most often measured through the affective bias test (ABT) and the modified affective bias test (mABT) in animals (Mitte, 2008). The ABT is based on the assumption that emotional state during the memory coding stage affects the perception of reward value (Stuart et al., 2013), while the mABT examines the ability of an animal to form memory bias based on reward value (Stuart et al., 2015).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Some methodological details of representative cognitive bias paradigms.

Animal experiments are an important complement to human research, especially in the study of neurological and psychological phenomena. Animal research has unique advantages to investigate the underlying mechanisms of these phenomena. For ethical considerations, pharmacological, genetic, and invasive human research is greatly limited, while neurophysiological methods that simulate abnormal states and pharmacological experiments in animals can be conducted to explore specific brain regions, neurons, and even molecules, to better understand the mechanisms behind phenomena, leading to targeted interventions. Harding et al. (2004) were the first to use cognitive bias testing in animals. The presented mini-review briefly summarizes the application of cognitive bias tests in animal research to further explore cognitive bias alterations in neuropsychiatric disorders and the neuropsychological mechanism of cognitive bias, which can ultimately lead to the early identification and treatment of these disorders.

Application of Animal Cognitive Bias Testing

Cognitive Bias in Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Many neuropsychiatric disorders are accompanied by emotional alterations which in turn can lead to cognitive biases. One application of cognitive bias testing is to reflect the affective state under different disorders. Currently, cognitive bias tests have been applied in animal models of depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder, and pain (see more details in Table 2). The next section briefly discusses the application of cognitive bias tests in some disorders.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Cognitive bias in animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders.

Depression

Depression is a mood disorder accompanied by low self-esteem, impaired cognitive function, and decreased pleasure (Monroe and Anderson, 2015). In human studies of cognitive bias, it was found that depressed subjects are more inclined to focus on negative stimuli (Armstrong and Olatunji, 2012), choose more negative words as self-descriptive (Dainer-Best et al., 2018), and recall more negative items and less positive items on memory tests (Bianchi et al., 2020). Harding et al. were the first to apply the judgment bias paradigm to investigate the cognitive bias of rats (Harding et al., 2004), demonstrating that the JBT can be used to detect negative emotions in animals.

Animal models of depression include chronic stress, learned helplessness, deficits in the serotonin system, and adverse experiences in early life (Czéh et al., 2016). Rats exposed to chronic physical stress or chronic psychosocial stress negatively interpret ambiguous cues, approach rewards more slowly, and experience a series of long-term cognitive and behavioral changes (Salmeto et al., 2011; Hymel and Sufka, 2012; Chaby et al., 2013; Papciak et al., 2013). Compared with congenitally non-helpless rats, congenitally helpless rats showed decreased positive responses and increased negative responses to ambiguous cues (Enkel et al., 2010; Richter et al., 2012). A study found that inhibiting serotonin synthesis through para-chlorophenylalanine (pCPA) dosing in pigs leads to a shift to more pessimistic judgments of ambiguous stimuli (Stracke et al., 2017). Results from early adverse experience models have shown lowered expectation of reward in response to ambiguous information (Bateson et al., 2015). Of particular interest, Stuart et al. (2019) found that rats experiencing maternal separation were more prone to corticosterone-induced negative bias and showed a deficit in reward-associated positive bias in mABT, whereas no significant difference was found in the sucrose preference test. This finding indicates that cognitive bias testing is a sensitive and important tool in depression-like state assessment.

Forced swimming test, sucrose preference test, and open-field test are widely used in animal studies to detect depression-like behaviors such as behavioral despair, anhedonia, and exploratory behaviors (Hu et al., 2017). These tests do not require training, while cognitive biased tasks require long-term and complex conditional training, as shown in Table 1. Although the cognitive bias test needs more experimental efforts, the affective bias measured by it could not be replaced by other tests (Robinson, 2018). Therefore, cognitive bias test can be used as a good supplement to the commonly used depression-like behavior test and plays a unique role in mechanism research (Stuart et al., 2015) and drug screening (Stuart et al., 2017).

Anxiety

Negative cognitive biases induced by anxiety can help an organism attend to threatening stimuli quickly, leading to an avoidance of potential danger. In a human study, it was found that anxious subjects exhibit an exaggerated attentional bias toward threats and overestimate detrimental consequences of events (Aue and Okon-Singer, 2015). In a JBT study of chicks under anxiety-like state, more pessimistic-like approach behaviors were exhibited to ambiguous aversive cues (Salmeto et al., 2011; Hymel and Sufka, 2012). Using pharmacological methods, one study found that sheep injected with the anxiety-stimulating drug 1-methyl-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP) show increased attention toward threats accompanied by increased vigilance (Lee et al., 2016), leading to negative attentional bias. Other studies found that acute injection of anxiogenic drug FG7142 in rats led to negative cognitive bias in both judgment bias tests (Hales et al., 2016) and affective bias tests (Stuart et al., 2013, 2015; Hinchcliffe et al., 2017).

Studies have shown that high-intensity light and white light are aversive to rodents, while dim light and red light are more neutral (Burman et al., 2009; Boleij et al., 2012) and therefore, alterations in lighting can be used to manipulate anxiety level in rodents. There is strong evidence that rats trained in dim lighting conditions but tested in bright lighting conditions have longer approach latencies when exposed to ambiguous cues (Burman et al., 2009; Boleij et al., 2012), indicating that acute increase in anxiety leads to negative judgment bias.

Bipolar Disorder and Mania

Depression and mania are the two core components of bipolar disorder. The cognitive and emotional correlates of depression have been extensively studied, but related research on mania is relatively lacking. Chronic administration of the psychostimulant d-amphetamine has been used to cause manic-like symptoms in animals (Valvassori et al., 2019). Some studies have shown that acute d-amphetamine administration can induce an optimistic bias in rats (Rygula et al., 2014; Hales et al., 2017), while another study found that two consecutive weeks of amphetamine treatment does not cause significant positive bias (Rygula et al., 2015b). However, it is not clear whether acute administration of amphetamines induces a manic-like state or simply a state of hyperactivity (Minassian et al., 2016).

In clinics, the mood stabilizers lithium and valproate are the most commonly used drugs to treat bipolar disorder (Geddes and Miklowitz, 2013). They can help patients find a balance between depression and mania (McIntyre et al., 2020). An animal study found that acute administration of lithium induced optimistic bias in rats that were generally pessimistic, while no significant bias was observed after injection of valproic acid in rats that were more neutral at baseline, which suggests that the effect direction of lithium may be affected by the valence of cognitive bias (Rygula et al., 2015a). Although such studies are rare, it still suggests that cognitive bias tests have the potential to be applied to the animal study of pharmacological mechanisms associated with bipolar disorder.

Pain

Pain includes not only physiological components but emotional and cognitive components as well (Price, 2000). Pain in humans can lead to decreased quality of life, anxiety, and depression (Kendig et al., 2000), while pain in animals can lead to reduced water and food intake and abnormal grooming, nesting, and burrowing behaviors (Jirkof, 2017). Previous studies have frequently used conditioned place avoidance (CPA) to examine emotion and avoidance associated with pain (Tappe-Theodor et al., 2019). However, the emotional and cognitive components of pain may be more complex. Cognitive bias tests, such as the JBT, focus on animals’ interpretation of ambiguous information, while the ABT includes reward value. Therefore, cognitive bias tests will help to explore the emotion-motivation and cognition-evaluation dimensions of pain from diverse perspectives.

Dairy calves experiencing postoperative pain associated with hot-iron disbudding to prevent horn growth exhibited a negative interpretation of ambiguous cues (Neave et al., 2013). A study on rats with chronic inflammatory pain as a result of 5-fluorouracil (5-FU) injection to simulate chemotherapy-induced intestinal mucositis, found that 72 h after injection, optimistic decision-making was significantly reduced (George et al., 2018), while 120 h after injection, optimistic decision-making increased as the damaged intestine gradually recovered (George et al., 2018). Chronic neuropathic pain caused by saphenous nerve injury leads to a negative bias which can be corrected by gabapentin as tested by the ABT, and a reward deficit in developing value-based memory bias in the mABT (Phelps et al., 2021), suggesting that rats with chronic neuropathic pain experience negative emotions and deficits in sensitivity to reward value. In addition, a study using the JBT to examine cancer pain and discomfort in mice with tumors found that tumor-bearing male mice were more pessimistic than healthy controls (Resasco et al., 2021). In sum, these studies indicate that cognitive bias tests can effectively measure the negative emotional state caused by pain in animals from acute pain to chronic pain and that analgesics can partially correct this state, therefore can be used in the validation of therapeutic approaches.

Cognitive Bias Tests in Assessing the Effect of Drugs on Affective State

Cognitive bias tests have shown good validity in the assessment of drug-induced affective changes (Robinson, 2018), providing a new approach for preclinical drug screening. Studies using the ABT found that acute administration of the antidepressants such as fluoxetine, reboxetine, venlafaxine, and mirtazapine induced positive biases in animals (Hales et al., 2017). However, one problem with the ABT and other preclinical testing methods, such as forced swimming, is the inability to distinguish between acute and delayed onset of antidepressant action. For example, fluoxetine was found to act quickly in preclinical trials using forced swimming, but with delayed clinical onset (Cryan and Holmes, 2005). The JBT can help to resolve this issue. Acute administration of the conventional antidepressants fluoxetine, reboxetine, or venlafaxine did not cause an interpretation bias in animals compared to the clinical fast-acting antidepressant ketamine, and only long-term use of fluoxetine resulted in a positive bias (Hales et al., 2017). These data indicate that the JBT better reflects the time course of antidepressant effects and effectively screens out fast-acting drugs at the preclinical stage.

Negative emotional side effects caused by drugs can greatly reduce a patient’s quality of life, affect medication compliance, and even cause the original therapeutic regimen to be broken down (George et al., 2018). Therefore, it is critical to assess potential emotional side effects of medication during preclinical studies. Cognitive bias tests have been used to study the emotional side effects of medications. One study used ABT to test some drugs that can increase the risk of depression in clinical patients and found that lipopolysaccharides (LPS), interferons-alpha (IFN-α), and tetrabenazine (a drug for the treatment of chorea in Huntington’s disease) (Frank, 2010) can induce negative deviation in rats, but varenicline (a smoking cessation drug) (Tonstad et al., 2020), carbamazepine (an anticonvulsant) (Israel and Beaudry, 1988), or montelukast (an anti-asthma drug) (Markham and Faulds, 1998) did not induce significant bias (Stuart et al., 2017). At present, the JBT has not been widely used in the preclinical screening of emotional side effects of drugs due to its long training time and complexity. It is necessary to further develop a more sensitive, fast, and simple animal experimental paradigm for cognitive bias in future research.

Discussion

An important interpretation for the behavioral results of cognitive bias test is to reflect the emotional state of animals and its effectiveness has been widely verified (Nguyen et al., 2020), indicating potential application in animal studies associated with neuropsychiatric disorders. Compared to the forced swimming test, the JBT is more sensitive to the clinical onset time of antidepressants, while the ABT is more sensitive in the assessment of reward deficits than the sucrose preference test. Therefore, cognitive bias tests may be used for the early identification of neuropsychiatric disorders and validation of their therapies.

It should be mentioned that in addition to the change of emotional state, motivation factors can also affect cognitive bias. For example, Enkel et al. (2010) noticed that in different depression-like states, a pessimistic judgment bias toward ambiguous cues could result from a decrease in positive response rate coupled with either (1) an increase in negative response rate or (2) an increase in omission rate. The former may reflect increased motivation to avoid potential punishment, whereas the latter may reflect decreased motivation to approach potential reward. This indicates that even in similar affective states, different motivational mechanisms may underlie the formation of bias. Due to the length of the min-review, we cannot discuss more, but we refer interested readers to the review by Lewis et al. (2019) and a recent paper by Neville et al. (2020), both of which provide an in-depth discussion on this topic.

The psychological mechanisms underlying the emergence and transition of cognitive bias remain unclear. One theory explains the emergence of cognitive bias from the perspective of biological evolution and adaptation (Durisko et al., 2015). In everyday life, most information is ambiguous with few explicit cues. Therefore, individuals must use prior experiences to interpret the meaning of current situation ambiguous cues (Norbury et al., 2018). This cognitive process is vital to animal survival and is an adaptive behavior that can be influenced by cognitive bias, which can be advantageous in limiting cognitive resources for faster and more efficient decision-making (Enkel et al., 2010). However, in some disorders, cognitive bias may remain constant, leading to non-adaptive behaviors. For example, negative cognitive biases associated with depression are developed by exposure to persistent stress and other adverse factors. These negative cognitive biases lead to risk-avoidance and loss-reducing behavioral strategies (Durisko et al., 2015) which can be advantageous in an unsafe environment. However, in a safe environment, these behaviors can be non-adaptive. A depressed individual may not have the capacity to alter negative biases in different situations. The ability to alter biases to appropriately address the presented situation needs further research.

Precision medicine is a hot spot in clinical research in recent years (Manchia et al., 2020). The detection of individual emotional characteristics will help to formulate an individualized treatment plan for emotional diseases. Prior studies have shown that the effects of acute manipulation of the dopamine and serotonin systems on cognitive bias may depend on cognitive bias baseline. After acute administration of haloperidol, a dopamine D2 receptor antagonist, or escitalopram, a 5-HT reuptake inhibitor, “optimistic” rats became more pessimistic, while “pessimistic” rats became more optimistic (Golebiowska and Rygula, 2017a). Therefore, cognitive bias tests may serve to formulate therapeutic regimens based on individual patient characteristics and, as such, should be included in future neuropsychiatric drug research.

Finally, the neural mechanisms of cognitive biases are understudied. The prefrontal area plays an important role in decision-making under ambiguity and risk (Rouault et al., 2019). A study in rats found that lesions to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) but not to the medial PFC (mPFC) decreased the proportion of positive lever presses and increased the proportion of negative lever presses in response to ambiguous tones, indicating increased pessimism (Golebiowska and Rygula, 2017b). The basolateral amygdala is closely associated with prefrontal regions and is also involved in the assessment of ambiguity and uncertainty (Davis and Whalen, 2001). One study found that unpredictability increased c-Fos expression in the lateral amygdala of mice (Herry et al., 2007). Likewise, the lateral septum is an important area for the integration of cognitive and affective information that compares known information with unknown and inferred ambiguous cues (Wirtshafter and Wilson, 2021). A study has shown a decrease in c-Fos expression in the lateral septum in response to ambiguous cues (Boleij et al., 2012). Further research using surgery, electrophysiology, optogenetics, in vivo calcium imaging, and other techniques to study the neural correlates of cognitive bias is necessary to identify key brain regions and molecular targets of potential therapeutics.

Author Contributions

NW, J-YW, and FL contributed to the conception of the review. Y-HZ wrote the original draft of the manuscript. NW and X-XL wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NNSF) grants to NW (31671140), FL (31970926), and J-YW (31271092).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Armstrong, T., and Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 704–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aue, T., and Okon-Singer, H. (2015). Expectancy biases in fear and anxiety and their link to biases in attention. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 42, 83–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M., Emmerson, M., Ergün, G., Monaghan, P., and Nettle, D. (2015). Opposite Effects of Early-Life Competition and Developmental Telomere Attrition on Cognitive Biases in Juvenile European Starlings. PLoS One 10:e0132602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Laurent, E., Schonfeld, I. S., Bietti, L. M., and Mayor, E. (2020). Memory bias toward emotional information in burnout and depression. J. Health Psychol. 25, 1567–1575. doi: 10.1177/1359105318765621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boleij, H., van’t Klooster, J., Lavrijsen, M., Kirchhoff, S., Arndt, S. S., and Ohl, F. (2012). A test to identify judgement bias in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 233, 45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. Am. Psychol. 36, 129–148.

Google Scholar

Brydges, N. M., Hall, L., Nicolson, R., Holmes, M. C., and Hall, J. (2012). The effects of juvenile stress on anxiety, cognitive bias and decision making in adulthood: a rat model. PLoS One 7:e48143. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burman, O. H., Parker, R. M., Paul, E. S., and Mendl, M. T. (2009). Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals. Physiol. Behav. 98, 345–350. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaby, L. E., Cavigelli, S. A., White, A., Wang, K., and Braithwaite, V. A. (2013). Long-term changes in cognitive bias and coping response as a result of chronic unpredictable stress during adolescence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:328. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cryan, J. F., and Holmes, A. (2005). The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and anxiety. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 4, 775–790. doi: 10.1038/nrd1825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czéh, B., Fuchs, E., Wiborg, O., and Simon, M. (2016). Animal models of major depression and their clinical implications. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 293–310. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dainer-Best, J., Lee, H. Y., Shumake, J. D., Yeager, D. S., and Beevers, C. G. (2018). Determining optimal parameters of the self-referent encoding task: a large-scale examination of self-referent cognition and depression. Psychol. Assess. 30, 1527–1540. doi: 10.1037/pas0000602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M., and Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol. Psychiatry 6, 13–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B. H., and Andrews, P. W. (2015). An adaptationist perspective on the etiology of depression. J. Affect. Disord. 172, 315–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enkel, T., Gholizadeh, D., von Bohlen und Halbach, O., Sanchis-Segura, C., Hurlemann, R., Spanagel, R., et al. (2010). Ambiguous-Cue Interpretation is Biased Under Stress- and Depression-Like States in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 1008–1015. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everaert, J. (2021). Interpretation of ambiguity in depression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 41, 9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everaert, J., Koster, E. H. W., and Derakshan, N. (2012). The combined cognitive bias hypothesis in depression. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 413–424. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, S. (2010). Tetrabenazine: the first approved drug for the treatment of chorea in US patients with Huntington disease. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 6, 657–665. doi: 10.2147/ndt.S6430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geddes, J. R., and Miklowitz, D. J. (2013). Treatment of bipolar disorder. Lancet 381, 1672–1682. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(13)60857-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, R. P., Barker, T. H., Lymn, K. A., Bigatton, D. A., Howarth, G. S., and Whittaker, A. L. (2018). A Judgement Bias Test to Assess Affective State and Potential Therapeutics in a Rat Model of Chemotherapy-Induced Mucositis. Sci. Rep. 8:8193. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26403-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golebiowska, J., and Rygula, R. (2017a). Effects of acute dopaminergic and serotonergic manipulations in the ACI paradigm depend on the basal valence of cognitive judgement bias in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 327, 133–143. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golebiowska, J., and Rygula, R. (2017b). Lesions of the Orbitofrontal but Not Medial Prefrontal Cortex Affect Cognitive Judgment Bias in Rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:51. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hales, C. A., Bartlett, J. M., Arban, R., Hengerer, B., and Robinson, E. S. J. (2020). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the effects of rapid acting antidepressants on decision-making biases in rodents. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 2278–2288. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00797-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hales, C. A., Houghton, C. J., and Robinson, E. S. J. (2017). Behavioural and computational methods reveal differential effects for how delayed and rapid onset antidepressants effect decision making in rats. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 27, 1268–1280. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hales, C. A., Robinson, E. S., and Houghton, C. J. (2016). Diffusion Modelling Reveals the Decision Making Processes Underlying Negative Judgement Bias in Rats. PLoS One 11:e0152592. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harding, E. J., Paul, E. S., and Mendl, M. (2004). Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427, 312–312. doi: 10.1038/427312a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herry, C., Bach, D. R., Esposito, F., Di Salle, F., Perrig, W. J., Scheffler, K., et al. (2007). Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala. J. Neurosci. 27, 5958–5966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5218-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinchcliffe, J. K., Stuart, S. A., Mendl, M., and Robinson, E. S. J. (2017). Further validation of the affective bias test for predicting antidepressant and pro-depressant risk: effects of pharmacological and social manipulations in male and female rats. Psychopharmacology 234, 3105–3116. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4687-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, C., Luo, Y., Wang, H., Kuang, S., Liang, G., Yang, Y., et al. (2017). Re-evaluation of the interrelationships among the behavioral tests in rats exposed to chronic unpredictable mild stress. PLoS One 12:e0185129. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hymel, K. A., and Sufka, K. J. (2012). Pharmacological reversal of cognitive bias in the chick anxiety-depression model. Neuropharmacology 62, 161–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Israel, M., and Beaudry, P. (1988). Carbamazepine in psychiatry: a review. Can. J. Psychiatry 33, 577–584. doi: 10.1177/070674378803300701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jirkof, P. (2017). Side effects of pain and analgesia in animal experimentation. Lab. Anim. 46, 123–128. doi: 10.1038/laban.1216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S., Neville, V., Higgs, L., Paul, E. S., Dayan, P., Robinson, E. S. J., et al. (2018). Assessing animal affect: an automated and self-initiated judgement bias task based on natural investigative behaviour. Sci. Rep. 8:12400. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30571-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendig, H., Browning, C. J., and Young, A. E. (2000). Impacts of illness and disability on the well-being of older people. Disabil. Rehabil. 22, 15–22. doi: 10.1080/096382800297088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kloke, V., Schreiber, R. S., Bodden, C., Mollers, J., Ruhmann, H., Kaiser, S., et al. (2014). Hope for the best or prepare for the worst? Towards a spatial cognitive bias test for mice. PLoS One 9:e105431. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakenberg, V., Woigk, I., Garcia Rodriguez, L., Kastner, N., Kaiser, S., Sachser, N., et al. (2019). Technology or ecology? New tools to assess cognitive judgement bias in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 362, 279–287. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C., Verbeek, E., Doyle, R., and Bateson, M. (2016). Attention bias to threat indicates anxiety differences in sheep. Biol. Lett. 12:20150977. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, L. R., Benn, A., Dwyer, D. M., and Robinson, E. S. J. (2019). Affective biases and their interaction with other reward-related deficits in rodent models of psychiatric disorders. Behav. Brain Res. 372:112051. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, L., Reimert, I., de Haas, E. N., Kemp, B., and Bolhuis, J. E. (2019). Effects of early and later life environmental enrichment and personality on attention bias in pigs (Sus scrofa domesticus). Anim. Cogn. 22, 959–972. doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01287-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manchia, M., Pisanu, C., Squassina, A., and Carpiniello, B. (2020). Challenges and Future Prospects of Precision Medicine in Psychiatry. Pharmgenomics Pers. Med. 13, 127–140. doi: 10.2147/pgpm.S198225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markham, A., and Faulds, D. (1998). Montelukast. Drugs 56, 251–256. doi: 10.2165/00003495-199856020-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntyre, R. S., Berk, M., Brietzke, E., Goldstein, B. I., López-Jaramillo, C., Kessing, L. V., et al. (2020). Bipolar disorders. Lancet 396, 1841–1856. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)31544-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mennen, A. C., Norman, K. A., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2019). Attentional bias in depression: understanding mechanisms to improve training and treatment. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29, 266–273. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minassian, A., Young, J. W., Cope, Z. A., Henry, B. L., Geyer, M. A., and Perry, W. (2016). Amphetamine increases activity but not exploration in humans and mice. Psychopharmacology 233, 225–233. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4098-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitte, K. (2008). Memory bias for threatening information in anxiety and anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 134, 886–911. doi: 10.1037/a0013343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroe, S. M., and Anderson, S. F. (2015). Depression: the shroud of heterogeneity. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 227–231. doi: 10.1177/0963721414568342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neave, H. W., Daros, R. R., Costa, J. H. C., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G., and Weary, D. M. (2013). Pain and pessimism: dairy calves exhibit negative judgement bias following hot-iron disbudding. PLoS One 8:e80556. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neville, V., King, J., Gilchrist, I. D., Dayan, P., Paul, E. S., and Mendl, M. (2020). Reward and punisher experience alter rodent decision-making in a judgement bias task. Sci. Rep. 10:11839. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68737-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, H. A. T., Guo, C., and Homberg, J. R. (2020). Cognitive Bias Under Adverse and Rewarding Conditions: A Systematic Review of Rodent Studies. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:14. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norbury, A., Robbins, T. W., and Seymour, B. (2018). Value generalization in human avoidance learning. eLife 7:e34779. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novak, J., Bailoo, J. D., Melotti, L., Rommen, J., and Wurbel, H. (2015). An Exploration Based Cognitive Bias Test for Mice: Effects of Handling Method and Stereotypic Behaviour. PLoS One 10:e0130718. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papciak, J., Popik, P., Fuchs, E., and Rygula, R. (2013). Chronic psychosocial stress makes rats more ‘pessimistic’ in the ambiguous-cue interpretation paradigm. Behav. Brain Res. 256, 305–310. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, C. E., Lumb, B. M., Donaldson, L. F., and Robinson, E. S. (2021). The partial saphenous nerve injury model of pain impairs reward-related learning but not reward sensitivity or motivation. Pain 162, 956–966. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, D. D. (2000). Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 288, 1769–1772. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resasco, A., MacLellan, A., Ayala, M. A., Kitchenham, L., Edwards, A. M., Lam, S., et al. (2021). Cancer blues? A promising judgment bias task indicates pessimism in nude mice with tumors. Physiol. Behav. 238:113465. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, S. H., Schick, A., Hoyer, C., Lankisch, K., Gass, P., and Vollmayr, B. (2012). A glass full of optimism: enrichment effects on cognitive bias in a rat model of depression. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 527–542. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0101-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E. S. J. (2018). Translational new approaches for investigating mood disorders in rodents and what they may reveal about the underlying neurobiology of major depressive disorder. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 373:20170036. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouault, M., Drugowitsch, J., and Koechlin, E. (2019). Prefrontal mechanisms combining rewards and beliefs in human decision-making. Nat. Commun. 10:301. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08121-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rygula, R., Szczech, E., Kregiel, J., Golebiowska, J., Kubik, J., and Popik, P. (2015b). Cognitive judgment bias in the psychostimulant-induced model of mania in rats. Psychopharmacology 232, 651–660. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3707-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rygula, R., Golebiowska, J., Kregiel, J., Holuj, M., and Popik, P. (2015a). Acute administration of lithium, but not valproate, modulates cognitive judgment bias in rats. Psychopharmacology 232, 2149–2156. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3847-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rygula, R., Papciak, J., and Popik, P. (2013). Trait pessimism predicts vulnerability to stress-induced anhedonia in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 2188–2196. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rygula, R., Papciak, J., and Popik, P. (2014). The effects of acute pharmacological stimulation of the 5-HT, NA and DA systems on the cognitive judgement bias of rats in the ambiguous-cue interpretation paradigm. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 24, 1103–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salmeto, A. L., Hymel, K. A., Carpenter, E. C., Brilot, B. O., Bateson, M., and Sufka, K. J. (2011). Cognitive bias in the chick anxiety–depression model. Brain Res. 1373, 124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychol. Bull. 103:147.

Google Scholar

Stracke, J., Otten, W., Tuchscherer, A., Puppe, B., and Düpjan, S. (2017). Serotonin depletion induces pessimistic-like behavior in a cognitive bias paradigm in pigs. Physiol. Behav. 174, 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.02.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, S. A., Butler, P., Munafo, M. R., Nutt, D. J., and Robinson, E. S. (2013). A translational rodent assay of affective biases in depression and antidepressant therapy. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1625–1635. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.69

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, S. A., Butler, P., Munafo, M. R., Nutt, D. J., and Robinson, E. S. (2015). Distinct Neuropsychological Mechanisms May Explain Delayed- Versus Rapid-Onset Antidepressant Efficacy. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2165–2174. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, S. A., Hinchcliffe, J. K., and Robinson, E. S. J. (2019). Evidence that neuropsychological deficits following early life adversity may underlie vulnerability to depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 1623–1630. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0388-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, S. A., Wood, C. M., and Robinson, E. S. J. (2017). Using the affective bias test to predict drug-induced negative affect: implications for drug safety. Br. J. Pharmacol. 174, 3200–3210. doi: 10.1111/bph.13972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tappe-Theodor, A., King, T., and Morgan, M. M. (2019). Pros and Cons of Clinically Relevant Methods to Assess Pain in Rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 100, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonstad, S., Arons, C., Rollema, H., Berlin, I., Hajek, P., Fagerström, K., et al. (2020). Varenicline: mode of action, efficacy, safety and accumulated experience salient for clinical populations. Curr. Med. Res. Opin. 36, 713–730. doi: 10.1080/03007995.2020.1729708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valvassori, S. S., Mariot, E., Varela, R. B., Bavaresco, D. V., Dal-Pont, G. C., Ferreira, C. L., et al. (2019). The role of neurotrophic factors in manic-, anxious- and depressive-like behaviors induced by amphetamine sensitization: implications to the animal model of bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 245, 1106–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.10.370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirtshafter, H. S., and Wilson, M. A. (2021). Lateral septum as a nexus for mood, motivation, and movement. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 126, 544–559. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.03.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive bias test, animal research, affective state, application, memory bias, interpretation bias

Citation: Zhang Y-H, Wang N, Lin X-X, Wang J-Y and Luo F (2022) Application of Cognitive Bias Testing in Neuropsychiatric Disorders: A Mini-Review Based on Animal Studies. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:924319. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.924319

Received: 20 April 2022; Accepted: 13 June 2022;
Published: 01 July 2022.

Edited by:

Jiaojian Wang, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China

Reviewed by:

Rainer Schwarting, University of Marburg, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Zhang, Wang, Lin, Wang and Luo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ning Wang, wangn@psych.ac.cn

Download