Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 11 August 2022
Sec. Behavioral Endocrinology
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.943759

Prenatal valproic acid-induced autism marmoset model exhibits higher salivary cortisol levels

  • 1Department of Cognitive and Psychological Sciences, Graduate School of Informatics, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
  • 2Laboratory for Ultrastructure Research, National Institute of Neuroscience, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan
  • 3Department of Psychology, Japan Women’s University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 4Academy of Emerging Science, Chubu University, Kasugai, Japan

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are exposed to a variety of stressors owing to their behavioral traits. Cortisol is a hormone typically associated with stress, and its concentration and response to stress are higher in individuals with ASD than in controls. The mechanisms underlying cortisol dysregulation in ASD have been explored in rodents. Although rodent models have successfully replicated the major symptoms of autism (i.e., impaired vocal communication, social interaction deficits, and restricted/repetitive patterns of behavior), evidence suggests that the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis system differs between rodents and primates. We developed an ASD model in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), a New World monkey, utilizing prenatal exposure to valproic acid (VPA). In this study, we collected the salivary cortisol levels in VPA-exposed and unexposed marmosets in the morning and afternoon. Our results revealed that both VPA-exposed and unexposed marmosets showed similar diurnal changes in cortisol levels, which were lower in the afternoon than in the morning. However, heightened cortisol levels were observed throughout the day in VPA-exposed marmosets. These results are consistent with those of ASD in humans. Our results suggest that VPA-exposed marmosets show similarities not only in their behavioral patterns and brain pathologies, which we have reported previously, but also in hormonal regulation, validating the usefulness of VPA-exposed marmosets also as a tool for ASD stress research.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a prevalence of 1 in 44 (Maenner et al., 2021). The major symptoms of ASD are impaired vocal communication, deficits in social interaction, and restricted/repetitive behavior patterns (American Psychological Association [APA], and American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Individuals with ASD are exposed to a variety of stressors owing to their behavioral traits, and overwhelming stress in ASD can trigger secondary deficits, such as depression and sleep disorders. Cortisol is a hormone typically associated with stress, and its concentration and response to stress are higher in individuals with ASD than in controls. The mechanisms underlying cortisol dysregulation in ASD have been explored in rodent models; however, differences in the regulatory system of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in rodents and primates have been reported (Gibbs, 1986; Broadbear et al., 2004; Goursaud et al., 2006; Bertani et al., 2010). For example, rats are nocturnal and marmosets are diurnal like humans, and the circadian rhythms of serum hormone levels in rats and marmosets are different (Bertani et al., 2010). Cellular and molecular properties in hypothalamus are largely conserved but still show some differences in gene expression patterns (van Eerdenburg and Rakic, 1994; Zhou et al., 2020). Furthermore, in mice, cortisol suppresses heat production in adipose tissue, but the same is unlikely to occur in primates (Luijten et al., 2019). Considering translational research, the primate ASD model appears to be preferable for studying the hormonal stress response in ASD.

We have previously developed an ASD model for a New World monkey, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) (Yasue et al., 2015, 2018; Watanabe et al., 2021). The marmosets were prenatally exposed to valproic acid (VPA), which is often used in rodent models of ASD. VPA epigenetically alters gene expression in the developing fetal brain. VPA-exposed marmosets demonstrated all three core symptoms of ASD: (1) biased usage of vocal repertoires, (2) weak social attention to unfamiliar conspecifics, and (3) deficits in reversal learning (Watanabe et al., 2021; Nakagami et al., 2022). In addition, when unfair rewards were given for the same task between two marmosets (Yasue et al., 2018), VPA-exposed marmosets continued the task even when it was unfavorable to them, suggesting a lack of attention to the coproducing species (weak inequity aversion). They also failed to recognize differences between third-party reciprocal and non-reciprocal exchanges, whereas VPA-unexposed marmosets (UE marmosets) discriminated against these exchanges (Kawai et al., 2014, 2019; Yasue et al., 2015). Furthermore, transcriptome analyses have revealed that VPA-exposed marmosets replicate a broad range of gene dysregulation in human idiopathic ASD, whereas rodent models generally replicate only a smaller part of the pathology (Watanabe et al., 2021). Especially, VPA-exposed marmosets recapitulate human ASD well in the four major cell types of the brain, consisting of neurons and three types of glia, but less well in glia in rodents. Thus, VPA-exposed marmosets appear to provide a suitable model for translational research on ASD.

In this study, we collected the salivary cortisol from marmosets in the morning and afternoon. Salivary measurements of cortisol have been shown to closely mirror those in the serum (Perogamvros et al., 2010; VanBruggen et al., 2011). We found that VPA-exposed marmosets had heightened basal cortisol levels, as do humans with ASD. Our results suggest that VPA-exposed marmosets may be useful for translational research on stress pathophysiology in ASD.

Materials and methods

Subjects

All experimental animal care procedures were conducted under approved protocols according to the regulations of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry (NCNP), Tokyo, Japan. Ten UE marmosets (five males and five females) and nine VPA-exposed marmosets (five males and four females) were included in this study (Table 1). The ages of the experimental animals range from 2- to 8-year-old. The mean age was 4.7 ± 1.62 years (UE marmosets: 4.5 ± 1.43, VPA-exposed marmosets: 4.9 ± 1.79). The subjects were born and raised in family cages. Then, at least 3 months prior to the experiment, they were transferred to individual stainless steel home cages (Natsume Manufacturing Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) in order to avoid the confounding effect on cortisol levels due to interactions with conspecifics of the same cage, which are difficult to control. The subjects were kept at room temperature of 29 ± 2°C and maintained on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle with free access to food and water. The lights in the breeding room were turned on at 7:00 a.m. and turned off at 7:00 p.m. daily. Marmosets in the facility were familiar with human contact and approached experimenters to obtain food rewards without hesitation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject information.

Valproic acid treatment

Valproic acid marmosets were exposed to valproic acid during their fetal stage, whereas UE marmosets were not (Yasue et al., 2015). The dams of VPA-exposed marmosets were housed in their cages. Their blood progesterone levels were monitored periodically to determine the timing of pregnancy, as was done for the UE dams. The VPA group received 200 mg/kg intragastric sodium valproate via an oral catheter daily on days 60 to 66 after conception, for a total of seven treatments. This period was determined based on the administration period (E12 of the rat fetus) used to produce VPA-exposed rodent models of ASD. All VPA dams received the medication without vomiting and showed no signs of abnormal pregnancy or delivery. The dams of UE marmosets were administered neither VPA nor a solvent during this period to prevent miscarriage. VPA marmosets displayed no malformations or body weight differences compared with UE marmosets.

Salivary collection and assay

Saliva was sampled to measure cortisol levels twice daily at 7:30 a.m. and at 6:30 p.m., as described in previous research (Kaplan et al., 2012). Animals were not captured by the experimenter and saliva was collected under free-ranging conditions in their home cages. A thin cotton swab (Matsumotokiyoshi Co., Ltd., Chiba, Japan) was used to collect saliva from the marmosets. The marmosets were fully trained to bite cotton swabs in their home cages before the experiments. The cotton swabs were dipped in powdered sugar to ensure that the bites would last sufficiently long (approximately 3–5 min) (Kaplan et al., 2012). A 2.0-mL Costar Spin-X centrifuge tube with a nylon filter (0.22 μm) was filled with the swabs and centrifuged at 10,000 rpm for 5 min to extract the liquid portion of the sample. The collected samples were stored in a −80°C freezer until further use. Saliva samples were collected three times per individual, with no two collections occurring in the same month. The duration of the experiment was 3 months, from June to August in 2019. Cortisol levels (μg/dl) were measured using an AIA-360 Automated Immunoassay Analyzer with AIA-pack cortisol test cups (Tosoh Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) and averaged per subject across all saliva collections either in the morning or the evening.

Statistical analysis

Two-factor repeated-measures ANOVA and student-t test were performed using JMP 16 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, United States) when the data were determined to be normally distributed by the Shapiro–Wilk test or by the F-test. If we found that the data showed a non-normal distribution, a mixed design ANOVA by a downloadable program (ARTool) (Wobbrock et al., 2018) was performed using R. P-values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Salivary samples were successfully collected by experimenters while the marmosets were in their home cages. They showed no signs of aggression or aversion during sampling. No significant effect of sex on salivary cortisol levels was observed in both UE and VPA-exposed groups in the morning and evening. Therefore, we analyzed the data for male and female together.

Figure 1 shows group salivary cortisol levels (mean ± SD) measured in the morning (7:30 a.m.) and afternoon (6:30 p.m.). Two-factor repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant difference in the group factor [F(1,17) = 9.3153, p = 0.0072] and time factor [F(1,17) = 100.8143, p < 0.0001]. We did not find any significant difference in interaction effects [F(1,17) = 4.0257, p = 0.0610]. These results suggested that the VPA-exposed marmosets maintained diurnal changes with high cortisol levels in the morning that fell throughout the afternoon, and that salivary cortisol levels in the VPA-exposed group were significantly higher than those in the UE group at the times examined in this study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Salivary cortisol levels in the morning (7:30 a.m.) and afternoon (6:30 p.m.) in the valproic acid-exposed (VPA group) and unexposed (UE group) marmosets. Values are expressed as mean ± standard deviation (n = 10 for UE group, n = 9 for VPA group, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).

We have also examined the correlation between age and salivary cortisol level. Although no statistically significant correlation between age and cortisol level was found at any time of day or in any group, there was a positive correlation in UE marmosets in the afternoon (r = 0.58) and a weak positive correlation in VPA-exposed marmosets (r = 0.32). In the morning, no correlation between age and salivary cortisol level was found for both groups.

Unfortunately, we found that among the VPA-exposed marmosets used in the current experiment, only three VPA-exposed marmosets were involved in the task presented in the previous papers (Yasue et al., 2015, 2018; Nakagami et al., 2022). Among these three animals, one marmoset showed the lowest morning salivary cortisol level (23.1 μg/dL). This level was half that of the other two VPA-exposed marmosets and was similar to the average level of the UE marmoset. This VPA-exposed marmoset had the best performance in three social tasks (social gazing, inequity aversion, and third-party reciprocity) among these three VPA-exposed marmosets. These anecdotal observations suggest that there may be a relationship between cortisol levels and levels of social impairment in VPA-exposed marmosets.

Of the animals in this experiment, there were two pairs of siblings in UE marmosets and two pairs in VPA marmosets (Table 1). None of these pairs had cortisol levels biased in one direction relative to the mean in both morning and evening (data not shown), suggesting that the results in this study were not biased due to the inclusion of siblings.

Note that saliva sampling in this study was from marmosets that were in individual cages, so the results of this study may include isolation-related stress. We considered the possibility that the time between the animals’ transfer to the single cage and the sampling start point could affect the results of this study. First, there was no significant difference in the time from isolation to the experiment between UE and VPA-exposed marmosets (p = 0.8659, student t-test). We also examined temporal changes of cortisol level in samples at three time points taken from each animal separately in the morning and afternoon. A mixed design ANOVA showed a significant difference only in the group factor [F(1,16) = 7.6731, p = 0.0131] in the morning cortisol level, but not in the time factor and interaction effects in the morning or afternoon. The results indicate that there is no specific pattern of variation in cortisol level among three samples over 3 months in either UE or VPA, suggesting the effect of isolation time before the experiment is minimal in both groups.

Discussion

This study revealed that both UE- and VPA-exposed marmosets showed a similar diurnal change in cortisol levels, which was lower in the afternoon than in the morning. This is consistent with the circadian rhythm of cortisol levels in humans (Smyth et al., 1997). However, heightened cortisol levels were observed in VPA-exposed marmosets throughout the day. Previous studies have shown that VPA-exposed marmosets use phee calls (isolation call) more frequently than do UE marmosets (Yamaguchi et al., 2010; Watanabe et al., 2021). This suggests that VPA-exposed marmosets may be in a constant state of stress, such as anxiety.

Studies of cortisol levels in humans with ASD have shown inconsistent results (Tordjman et al., 2014). The discrepancies in their results may be related to study methods (plasma cortisol measures vs. urinary or salivary measures), and sample sizes. We thus compared our results with those of five studies that used a sufficient number of cases with salivary sampling in humans (more than 20 cases in both control and ASD groups). Four of those reports showed elevated cortisol concentrations (Corbett et al., 2008; Kidd et al., 2012; Spratt et al., 2012; Tordjman et al., 2014). One report commented on the high variability of cortisol concentration in ASD cases (Corbett et al., 2009). In studies examining diurnal variation, two reports observed cortisol increases in the morning and evening (Corbett et al., 2008; Tordjman et al., 2014), while another observed cortisol increases only in the morning (Kidd et al., 2012). Thus, our results are consistent with these reports. VPA-exposed marmosets replicated the abnormal endocrine function observed in people with ASD.

Among rodent models of ASD, BTBR mice and VPA-exposed male rats showed heightened serum cortisol levels (Schneider et al., 2008; Benno et al., 2009; Frye and Llaneza, 2010; Silverman et al., 2010; Ferraro et al., 2021), similar to VPA-exposed marmosets and people with idiopathic ASD. In contrast, transgenic ASD model mice (MeCP2 and FMR1 mutant) showed no elevated levels of cortisol (McGill et al., 2006; Qin and Smith, 2008). It can be difficult, however, to collect specimens for cortisol measurement in rodents without stressing the animals. To avoid this problem, blood is often collected from the heart immediately after euthanasia via acute decapitation. Collection of salivary cortisol or plasma cortisol using the tail cutting technique also requires restraint and is unsuitable for measuring basal cortisol levels (Fenske, 1997; Kim et al., 2018). Blood sampling through an intravenous catheter in large vessels can be applicable for plasma cortisol measurement on freely moving rodents (Nyuyki et al., 2012). However, this technique is still invasive and not suitable for long-term cortisol level monitoring. In this study, we established a method to collect saliva from marmosets after acclimatization by training. The current procedure using marmosets will allow the repeated examination of cortisol levels in ASD models in the same individuals, both at basal levels and in the stress response, and will contribute to a reduction in the number of experimental animals.

This study revealed that VPA-exposed marmosets reproduced the variability of cortisol levels in human ASD. Marmosets are cooperative and highly social primates and are considered a suitable model animal to study stress in social life, and they also show higher social function deficits as in cases of ASD (Yasue et al., 2015, 2018; Watanabe et al., 2021; Nakagami et al., 2022). Further examination of cortisol levels in VPA-exposed marmosets would provide a new avenue for studying the biology of stress faced by individuals with ASD and for developing novel therapeutic interventions.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

This animal study was reviewed and approved by the regulations of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry (NCNP), Tokyo, Japan.

Author contributions

NK, NI, MN, and AN designed this study. MN, AN, and MY performed salivary sampling. MN analyzed the data. KN managed the production and physical condition of the animals. NK, NI, and MN wrote the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by an Intramural Research Grant (Grant Number: 2-7 to NI) for Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders from the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, by Brain Mapping by Integrated Neurotechnologies for Disease Studies (Brain/MINDS), the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED) (Grant Number: 22 dm0207066h0004 to NI), and by JPSP KAKENHI (Grant Numbers: JP24600020 and JP15K01791 to AN and 16H02058, 19K22870, 21H04421, and 21K18552 to NK).

Acknowledgments

We thank Akiko Tsuchiya for providing technical support and Satoshi Watanabe for advice on statistical analysis.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

American Psychological Association [APA], and American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, TX: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Benno, R., Smirnova, Y., Vera, S., Liggett, A., and Schanz, N. (2009). Exaggerated responses to stress in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse: an unusual behavioral phenotype. Behav. Brain Res. 197, 462–465. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertani, S., Carboni, L., Criado, A., Michielin, F., Mangiarini, L., and Vicentini, E. (2010). Circadian profile of peripheral hormone levels in Sprague-Dawley rats and in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). In Vivo 24, 827–836.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Broadbear, J. H., Winger, G., and Woods, J. H. (2004). Self-administration of fentanyl, cocaine and ketamine: effects on the pituitary-adrenal axis in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 176, 398–406. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1891-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbett, B. A., Mendoza, S., Wegelin, J. A., Carmean, V., and Levine, S. (2008). Variable cortisol circadian rhythms in children with autism and anticipatory stress. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 33, 227–234.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Corbett, B. A., Schupp, C. W., Levine, S., and Mendoza, S. (2009). Comparing cortisol, stress, and sensory sensitivity in children with autism. Autism Res. 2, 39–49.

Google Scholar

Fenske, M. (1997). The use of salivary cortisol measurements for the non-invasive assessment of adrenal cortical function in Guinea pigs. Exp. Clin. Endocrinol. Diabetes 105, 163–168. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1211746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraro, S., De Zavalia, N., Belforte, N., and Amir, S. (2021). In utero exposure to valproic-acid alters circadian organisation and clock-gene expression: implications for autism spectrum disorders. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:711549. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.711549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frye, C. A., and Llaneza, D. C. (2010). Corticosteroid and neurosteroid dysregulation in an animal model of autism, BTBR mice. Physiol. Behav. 100, 264–267. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, D. M. (1986). Vasopressin and oxytocin: hypothalamic modulators of the stress response: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 11, 131–139. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(86)90048-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goursaud, A.-P. S., Mendoza, S. P., and Capitanio, J. P. (2006). Do neonatal bilateral ibotenic acid lesions of the hippocampal formation or of the amygdala impair HPA axis responsiveness and regulation in infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)? Brain Res. 1071, 97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, G., Pines, M. K., and Rogers, L. J. (2012). Stress and stress reduction in common marmosets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 137, 175–182.

Google Scholar

Kawai, N., Nakagami, A., Yasue, M., Koda, H., and Ichinohe, N. (2019). Common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) evaluate third-party social interactions of human actors but Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) do not. J. Comparat. Psychol. 133:488. doi: 10.1037/com0000182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawai, N., Yasue, M., Banno, T., and Ichinohe, N. (2014). Marmoset monkeys evaluate third-party reciprocity. Biol. Lett. 10:20140058.

Google Scholar

Kidd, S. A., Corbett, B. A., Granger, D. A., Boyce, W. T., Anders, T. F., and Tager, I. B. (2012). Daytime secretion of salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase in preschool-aged children with autism and typically developing children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2648–2658. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1522-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Foong, D., Cooper, M. S., Seibel, M. J., and Zhou, H. (2018). Comparison of blood sampling methods for plasma corticosterone measurements in mice associated with minimal stress-related artefacts. Steroids 135, 69–72. doi: 10.1016/j.steroids.2018.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luijten, I. H., Cannon, B., and Nedergaard, J. (2019). Glucocorticoids and brown adipose tissue: do glucocorticoids really inhibit thermogenesis? Mol. Aspects Med. 68, 42–59. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2019.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maenner, M. J., Shaw, K. A., Bakian, A. V., Bilder, D. A., Durkin, M. S., Esler, A., et al. (2021). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2018. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 70:1. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6745a7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGill, B. E., Bundle, S. F., Yaylaoglu, M. B., Carson, J. P., Thaller, C., and Zoghbi, H. Y. (2006). Enhanced anxiety and stress-induced corticosterone release are associated with increased Crh expression in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103, 18267–18272. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608702103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakagami, A., Yasue, M., Nakagaki, K., Nakamura, M., Kawai, N., and Ichinohe, N. (2022). Reduced childhood social attention in an autism model marmoset predicts impaired social skills and inflexible behavior in adulthood. Front. Psychiatry 13:885433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.885433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyuyki, K. D., Maloumby, R., Reber, S. O., and Neumann, I. D. (2012). Comparison of corticosterone responses to acute stressors: chronic jugular vein versus trunk blood samples in mice. Stress 15, 618–626. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2012.655348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perogamvros, I., Keevil, B. G., Ray, D. W., and Trainer, P. J. (2010). Salivary cortisone is a potential biomarker for serum free cortisol. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 95, 4951–4958. doi: 10.1210/jc.2010-1215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, M., and Smith, C. B. (2008). Unaltered hormonal response to stress in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 883–889. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, T., Roman, A., Basta-Kaim, A., Kubera, M., Budziszewska, B., Schneider, K., et al. (2008). Gender-specific behavioral and immunological alterations in an animal model of autism induced by prenatal exposure to valproic acid. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 728–740. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, J. L., Yang, M., Turner, S. M., Katz, A. M., Bell, D. B., Koenig, J. I., et al. (2010). Low stress reactivity and neuroendocrine factors in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Neuroscience 171, 1197–1208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.09.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, J. M., Ockenfels, M. C., Gorin, A. A., Catley, D., Porter, L. S., Kirschbaum, C., et al. (1997). Individual differences in the diurnal cycle of cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 22, 89–105.

Google Scholar

Spratt, E. G., Nicholas, J. S., Brady, K. T., Carpenter, L. A., Hatcher, C. R., Meekins, K. A., et al. (2012). Enhanced cortisol response to stress in children in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 75–81. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1214-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tordjman, S., Anderson, G. M., Kermarrec, S., Bonnot, O., Geoffray, M.-M., Brailly-Tabard, S., et al. (2014). Altered circadian patterns of salivary cortisol in low-functioning children and adolescents with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 50, 227–245. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Eerdenburg, F. J., and Rakic, P. (1994). Early neurogenesis in the anterior hypothalamus of the rhesus monkey. Dev. Brain Res. 79, 290–296. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(94)90134-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanBruggen, M. D., Hackney, A. C., McMurray, R. G., and Ondrak, K. S. (2011). The relationship between serum and salivary cortisol levels in response to different intensities of exercise. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 6, 396–407. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.6.3.396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, S., Kurotani, T., Oga, T., Noguchi, J., Isoda, R., Nakagami, A., et al. (2021). Functional and molecular characterization of a non-human primate model of autism spectrum disorder shows similarity with the human disease. Nat. Commun. 12:5388. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-25487-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wobbrock, J. O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., Higgins, J. J., and Kay, M. (2018). ARTool Align-and-Rank Data for A Nonparametric ANOVA. Available online at: https://deptswashingtonedu/aimgroup/proj/art/ (accessed July 1, 2022)

Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, C., Izumi, A., and Nakamura, K. (2010). Time course of vocal modulation during isolation in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Am. J. Primatol. 72, 681–688. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasue, M., Nakagami, A., Banno, T., Nakagaki, K., Ichinohe, N., and Kawai, N. (2015). Indifference of marmosets with prenatal valproate exposure to third-party non-reciprocal interactions with otherwise avoided non-reciprocal individuals. Behav. Brain Res. 292, 323–326. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasue, M., Nakagami, A., Nakagaki, K., Ichinohe, N., and Kawai, N. (2018). Inequity aversion is observed in common marmosets but not in marmoset models of autism induced by prenatal exposure to valproic acid. Behav. Brain Res. 343, 36–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Zhong, S., Peng, H., Liu, J., Ding, W., Sun, L., et al. (2020). Cellular and molecular properties of neural progenitors in the developing mammalian hypothalamus. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–16.

Google Scholar

Keywords: VPA marmoset, cortisol, diurnal change, stress, autism spectrum disorder

Citation: Nakamura M, Nakagami A, Nakagaki K, Yasue M, Kawai N and Ichinohe N (2022) Prenatal valproic acid-induced autism marmoset model exhibits higher salivary cortisol levels. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:943759. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.943759

Received: 14 May 2022; Accepted: 14 July 2022;
Published: 11 August 2022.

Edited by:

Marie-Pierre Moisan, INRAE Nouvelle-Aquitaine Bordeaux, France

Reviewed by:

Leandro Franco Vendruscolo, National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIH), United States
Suresh Sundram, Monash University, Australia
Rafael S. Maior, University of Brasília, Brazil

Copyright © 2022 Nakamura, Nakagami, Nakagaki, Yasue, Kawai and Ichinohe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nobuyuki Kawai, kawai@is.nagoya-u.ac.jp; Noritaka Ichinohe, nichino@ncnp.go.jp

Download