Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 06 March 2018
Sec. Neurodegeneration

A Longitudinal Study of Neurocognition in Dementia with Lewy Bodies Compared to Alzheimer’s Disease

\r\nMonica H. Breitve,,*Monica H. Breitve1,2,3*Luiza J. Chwiszczuk,,Luiza J. Chwiszczuk1,2,3Kolbjrn Brnnick,Kolbjørn Brønnick4,5Minna J. Hynninen,Minna J. Hynninen6,7Bjrn H. Auestad,Bjørn H. Auestad8,9Dag Aarsland,Dag Aarsland10,11Arvid Rongve,,\r\nArvid Rongve1,2,3
  • 1Department of Research and Innovation, Helse-Fonna Haugesund Hospital, Haugesund, Norway
  • 2Department of Geriatric Psychiatry, Clinic of Psychiatry, Helse-Fonna Haugesund Hospital, Haugesund, Norway
  • 3Faculty of Medicine, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 4TIPS – Centre for Clinical Research in Psychosis, Stavanger University Hospital, Stavanger, Norway
  • 5Network for Medical Sciences, University of Stavanger, Stavanger, Norway
  • 6Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 7NKS Olaviken Gerontopsychiatric Hospital, Erdal, Norway
  • 8Research Department, Stavanger University Hospital, Stavanger, Norway
  • 9Department of Mathematics and Physics, University of Stavanger, Stavanger, Norway
  • 10Center for Age-Related Diseases (SESAM), Stavanger University Hospital, Stavanger, Norway
  • 11Department of Old Age Psychiatry, King’s College, Institute of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom

Introduction: There are relatively few longitudinal studies on the differences in cognitive decline between Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), and the majority of existing studies have suboptimal designs.

Aim: We investigated the differences in cognitive decline in AD compared to DLB over 4 years and cognitive domain predictors of progression.

Methods: In a longitudinal study, 266 patients with first-time diagnosis of mild dementia were included and followed annually. The patients were tested annually with neuropsychological tests and screening instruments [MMSE (Mini-Mental Status Examination), Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), the second edition of California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II), Trail Making Test A & B (TMT A & B), Stroop test, Controlled Oral Word Associations Test (COWAT) animal naming, Boston Naming Test, Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP) Cubes and Silhouettes]. Longitudinal analyses were performed with linear mixed effects (LME) models and Cox regression. Both specific neuropsychological tests and cognitive domains were analyzed.

Results: This study sample comprised 119 AD and 67 DLB patients. In TMT A, the DLB patients had a faster decline over 4 years than patients with AD (p = 0.013). No other longitudinal differences in specific neuropsychological tests were found. Higher executive domain scores at baseline were associated with a longer time to reach severe dementia (CDR = 3) or death for the total sample (p = 0.032). High or low visuospatial function at baseline was not found to be associated with cognitive decline (MMSE) or progression of dementia severity (CDR) over time.

Conclusion: Over 4 years, patients with DLB had a faster decline in TMT A than patients with AD, but this should be interpreted cautiously. Beyond this, there was little support for faster decline in DLB patients neuropsychologically than in AD patients.

Introduction

Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is the second most common type of neurodegenerative dementias with a prevalence of 2.2–24.7% (1). Opposed to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) which is associated with tau and amyloid pathology, DLB is an α-synucleinopathy, characterized with visual hallucinations, parkinsonism, and fluctuations. DLB seems to be a more devastating condition following a more malignant disease course than AD, with a nearly halved time until nursing home admission implying much higher costs and nearly halved survival time. Additionally, persons diagnosed with DLB experience more anxiety and sleep disturbances than persons diagnosed with AD (26).

Previous longitudinal studies have limited evidence base mostly due to methodological weaknesses when addressing the rate of progression of cognitive decline in DLB as compared to AD, and the data are still inconclusive. 6 of 18 studies included in our systematic review and meta-analysis reported significant differences in the rate of cognitive decline between these two conditions. Three studies reported a faster cognitive decline on Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) in patients with mixed DLB and AD compared to pure forms. Two studies reported a faster decline on delayed recall and recognition in AD, and one in DLB on verbal fluency. The meta-analysis of six studies reporting MMSE scores found no significant difference in annual decline between DLB and AD (7). An updated search did not add any information, except from three studies that used MMSE as outcome. For instance, our research group found a faster decline in DLB than AD over 5 years (4.4 vs 3.2 points on average per year) (8). In the second study, patients with mixed AD and DLB were shown to decline faster than AD or DLB, and in the last one, they found some indications of a faster decline in DLB than in AD and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD) (9, 10).

While AD is predominantly characterized by impaired episodic memory, patients with DLB have more impaired executive, attentional, and visuospatial functioning. For example, visuospatial dysfunction is found in 71% of DLB and in 40% of AD, when controlled for motor function (11), and is found to be predictive for global cognitive impairment (12). In two studies, patients with DLB were divided into groups with high or low visuospatial functioning. Patients with low baseline visuospatial functioning had a faster decline on the Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) and activities of daily living (ADL). No differences were found in AD subgroups (12, 13). In our baseline study, we found that visuoconstructional functions in DLB were worse than in AD, but we did not find any differences between AD and DLB regarding visuoperception (14).

There is a lack of robust and reliable longitudinal studies that compare the cognitive decline in AD and DLB. Studies are needed in order to have a thorough knowledge about prognosis, a very important issue for the patients and their caregivers, as well for the community that plans caregiving. To complement previous research, we investigated (a) differences in rate of decline in neuropsychological test over 4 years in AD and DLB, (b) the association of cognitive domains at baseline and dementia progression, and (c) the associations of high and low visuospatial function at baseline and dementia progression.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Totally, 266 outpatients in clinics of old age psychiatry and geriatric medicine in Western Norway with a first-time mild dementia diagnosis [MMSE score of at least 20 or Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 1] were recruited from 2005 and followed annually. Patients with acute delirium or confusion, terminal illness, current or previous bipolar disorder or psychotic disorder, or who were recently diagnosed with a major somatic illness were excluded (15). Follow-ups were conducted at the clinic or in nursing homes. The study protocol was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Western Norway as well as in the Norwegian Social Science Data Services. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Dementia Diagnosis

The diagnosis of dementia was based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria. The diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease was made according to The National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-AD and Related Disorders Association (16). DLB was diagnosed according to the revised consensus criteria (17). Ioflupane single-photon emission computed tomography (123I-FP-CIT SPECT) (DaTSCAN) was conducted for 55 patients. Two independent raters set the diagnosis, and the diagnoses were evaluated after 2 and 5 years, based on all available information from follow-ups. Neuropathological confirmations are consecutively sampled. For further diagnostic information see Aarsland et al. (15).

Neuropsychological Measures

A battery of standardized and established rating scales and tests was used for investigating patients’ cognitive functioning. MMSE (18) is a brief test used for screening cognitive impairment. The CDR (19) is used for assessing the severity of dementia, on a global scale from 0 to 3. The second edition of California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) (20) was used for assessing verbal learning and memory, in addition to MMSE delayed recall. Visual scanning, psychomotor speed, and attention were assessed with Trail Making Test A & B (TMT A & B) (21). Executive functions were also measured by MMSE attention and calculation, Stroop test (word, color, color-word, number of correct words read in 45 s) (22), and Controlled Oral Word Associations Test, using the semantic fluency task of naming animals (COWAT) (23). Naming was measured with Boston Naming Test (BNT) 15 items (24). Visuospatial function was measured by Silhouettes and Cubes on the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP) (25) and the pentagon on MMSE. The memory domain was composed of CVLT-II List A 1–5 total, CVLT-II delayed recall and MMSE delayed recall. The executive domain of TMT A, Stroop test, COWAT naming animals and MMSE attention and calculation, and the visuospatial domain of VOSP Silhouettes and Cubes and MMSE pentagon.

Raw scores were used when analyzing neuropsychological tests. When analyzing cognitive domains, raw scores were standardized into z-scores, and then computed into a composite score for each domain. TMT A was reversed.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analysis was made with SPSS 22.0. and R (26). Differences between AD and DLB at baseline were analyzed with Mann–Whitney U-test, T-test for normally distributed data, and by Pearson chi-square for categorical data. Linear mixed effects (LME) models were employed to analyze the longitudinal data to study possible differences between AD and DLB in neuropsychological tests. The LME model is to a certain extent capable of handling longitudinal data with dropout (27). Models with random intercept and random slope were used. Cox regression analyses were performed to find out if cognitive domains (memory, executive, visuospatial) at baseline were associated with survival/dementia severity. Outcome was defined as reaching CDR 3 or death. Finally, we used random effects logistic regression, finding the association between probability for CDR >1.5 (dementia severity) or lower MMSE score (cognitive decline) and as function of time and visuospatial function at baseline. High or low function was defined by a domain score above or below the mean.

Results

Baseline

Of 266 recruited patients, 186 were included in the analysis, 119 AD and 67 DLB, see Figure 1 for flow chart. There were no significant differences between the groups in age, education, depression, MMSE, and CDR scores at baseline. The AD group had significantly more women included and the DLB group had significantly longer duration of symptoms before inclusion (see Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of study group.

Follow-up

An association between time, diagnosis, and neuropsychological tests was found on Trails A (p = 0.013), where the DLB group had a significantly faster decline compared to the AD group (see Figure 2). No statistical differences were found in CVLT-II, VOSP, BNT, Stroop, COWAT, and TMT A. Analyses were adjusted for sex, age, and education (see Table 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differences in Trail Making Test A in Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies over 4 years. Reported in square root transformed data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Longitudinal changes in cognitive tests over 4 years.

Higher executive domain scores at baseline were associated with a longer time to reach severe dementia (CDR 3) or death for the total sample (p = 0.032), but type of dementia were not. No such associations were observed for the visuospatial or memory domain. The analyses were adjusted for age and sex (see Table 3). We also analyzed if high or low visuospatial function at baseline in AD and DLB were associated with cognitive decline (MMSE) (p = 0.108) or dementia severity CDR (p = 0.654), but no differences were found. The analyses were adjusted for sex, age, and education.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between cognitive domains and negative outcome.

At one-year follow-up, only 11.9% (n = 8) of the DLB group were able to complete TMT B, compared to 27.7% (n = 33) in the AD group, therefore, not included in the analyses.

Discussion

Patients with DLB had a faster decline on TMT A compared to patients with AD over four years. Higher executive function at baseline was associated with a slower progression of dementia and longer survival in the total group, but no differences between AD and DLB were observed. We did not find any association between high or low visuospatial function at baseline and the rate of decline.

The TMT A task is relatively undemanding cognitively and motor tempo determines the performance to a degree. The faster decline in the performance of the DLB group could be due to more deficiencies in motor and visuospatial functions, which are more prominent in DLB than in AD. On the other hand, the two groups were progressing without any significant difference on other neuropsychological test, some of which were also depending on such skills. Previous studies have not found differences in decline over time between the two groups in TMT A, and due to missing data and the risk of familywise statistical error, our findings should be cautiously interpreted. In our baseline study, we found that DLB patients performed worse than AD on tests correlating with parkinsonism, not with visual hallucinations or cognitive fluctuation (14).

High executive functions at baseline were associated with a slower progression of reaching severe dementia or death for the total sample, but there were no differences in progression between AD and DLB. In the memory or visuospatial domains, we found no significant differences. Chunking single neuropsychological tests that are assumed to measure more or less the same cognitive factor into one cognitive domain has pros and cons. We can rely on more tests and presumably a more robust measure, nevertheless the domains, especially the executive, are not entities, but can be broken into several functions (28). This could be the reason why studies of mild cognitive impairment have shown mixed results regarding the link between executive dysfunctions and prediction of conversion to dementia (29). On the other hand, we have not found any studies that have investigated the difference in cognitive domains over time in AD and DLB. As executive functions also encompass several forms of attention, included fluctuations, one of the core symptoms of DLB, it would be interesting to find out if patients with DLB and relatively preserved executive functions have milder fluctuations and a slower disease progression.

Previous research has shown that patients with DLB and low visuospatial function have a faster decline on cognitive screening tests or ADL function (13). We could not confirm their findings in our study. This could possibly be due to longer follow-up time, the tests that are used in the analyses, or the lack of neuropathological reports on the total sample. Without the neuropathological confirmation of the diagnosis, we cannot rule out the possibility that there are more DLB cases with neocortical or nigral predominant type of LB, and not the limbic type that is more often associated with more rapid decline in visuospatial function (30). At the same time in AD, the zeitgeist is to move away from using the concept AD, and instead refer to the pathology behind the symptoms, since there are several subtypes and other AD mimicking diseases (31). Lack of systematical differences could hypothetically be due to grouping AD patients with mixed pathologies that are not identical and the expression of the same disease. We also lack information about genetics that could have affected the cognitive decline over time.

In clinical practice, the two most frequent and important questions for people newly diagnosed with dementia are “is there anything that can prevent progression?” followed by “how long will it take before I lose my mind?” This is not easy to predict for the individual patient, but on group levels, for patients with relatively preserved executive functions, the progression seems to be slower. Even though there are few studies on neuropsychological tests that predict the rate of decline in AD (32), our finding is supported from studies that found impaired executive functions to be associated with a faster decline (33, 34).

Thus, there is no persuasive evidence that there is a dissimilar decline in neuropsychological tests and cognitive domains between AD and DLB. On the other hand, recent studies of good quality have found that DLB progress faster than AD and PDD using the cognitive screening test MMSE, as outcome. Our clinical impression of a more rapid decline in patients with DLB is evident in previously mentioned research, probably caused by the α-synuclein pathology and the severe BPSD symptoms that are highly frequent in DLB. The cognitive differences though, is not the dominant predictor of a more rapid disease course (8).

There are some limitations in the study. We have previously argued for longer follow up time than 1 year in these kinds of studies (7). Regardless a large sample size at baseline, missing data is a common issue in longitudinal studies with neurodegenerative diseases due to cognitive decline and death. In our study, LME models were used to minimize this problem. Also, we used neuropsychological test scores at baseline with non-cognitive measures as outcome. Another advantage of this approach is that in the beginning of the dementia, AD and DLB are more separable, since the clinic and brain pathology is more specific, not widespread and mixed with brain pathology that comes as a natural consequence of aging.

Mini-Mental Status Examination was the only test that all the patients could complete at every follow up. MMSE has been criticized for not being sensitive enough for measuring changes in patients with pure DLB since they have a different cognitive profile than AD patients, but Lessig et al. found it valid for Parkinson’s disease patients who share most of the same pathology (35).

The mean score at baseline in CVLT-II delayed memory was low (mean 1.3 for AD and 2.5 for DLB out of a maximum score of 16) and missing statistically significant differences could be due to floor effect. There is also a potential problem with circularity, since the diagnoses are made partially by relying on results on cognitive tests (i.e., memory impairments for AD). However, the autopsy results indicate that the diagnoses are not biased by circularity (36).

Strengths of the study are the longitudinal design with one of the largest samples of DLB patients which has been followed annually, and the AD and DLB groups were similar in age, education, and MMSE score at baseline. The patients are thoroughly evaluated and diagnosed, and the majority of the DLB patients have undergone 123I-FP-CIT SPECT (DaTSCAN), and DLB is, therefore, not likely to be underdiagnosed in the study group. Autopsy has been done in 43 cases with 92% sensitivity and 83% specificity for clinical diagnosis (36).

Conclusion

Over 4 years, there was little support for the hypothesis that patients with DLB had faster decline neuropsychologically than patients with AD. The only exception was TMT A, where patients with DLB had a faster decline than AD patients, but this should be interpreted cautiously due to missing data and the risk of familywise statistical error. Higher executive domain scores at baseline were associated with a longer time to reach severe dementia or death for the total sample, but type of dementia was not. Further research is needed and information about genetics should be included.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Western Norway and the Norwegian Social Science Data Services. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

MB and LC participated in the data acquisition, made the hypothesis, analyzed and interpreted data, and drafted the manuscript. KB made the hypothesis, interpreted data, and revised the manuscript. BA performed and interpreted the longitudinal analysis, and revised the manuscript. MH contributed with the hypothesis, interpretation of the analysis, and revised the manuscript. DA and AR participated in the data acquisition, contributed with the hypothesis, interpretation of the analysis, and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wanted to thank the patients and their caregivers who participated in the study and Mien Parlati for plotting data.

Abbreviations

AD, Alzheimer’s disease; ADL, activities of daily living; BNT, Boston Naming Test; BPSD, behavioural and psychological symptoms; CDR, Clinical Dementia Rating; CVLT-II, the second edition of California Verbal Learning Test; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Associations Test; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; DRS, Dementia Rating Scale; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition; LB, Lewy bodies; LME, linear mixed effects; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; MMSE, Mini-Mental Status Examination; PD, Parkinson’s disease; PDD, Parkinson’s disease dementia; TMT A & B, Trail Making Test A & B; VOSP, Visual Object and Space Perception Battery; 123I-FP-CIT SPECT, ioflupane single-photon emission computed tomography.

References

1. Vann Jones SA, O’Brien JT. The prevalence and incidence of dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review of population and clinical studies. Psychol Med (2014) 44(4):673–83. doi:10.1017/S0033291713000494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Stubendorff K, Hansson O, Minthon L, Londos E. Differences in survival between patients with dementia with Lewy bodies and patients with Alzheimer’s disease – measured from a fixed cognitive level. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2011) 32(6):408–16. doi:10.1159/000335364

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Rongve A, Vossius C, Nore S, Testad I, Aarsland D. Time until nursing home admission in people with mild dementia: comparison of dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2013) 29(4):398. doi:10.1002/gps.4015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Vossius C, Rongve A, Testad I, Wimo A, Aarsland D. The use and costs of formal care in newly diagnosed dementia: a three-year prospective follow-up study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2013) 22(4):381–8. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2012.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hynninen MJ, Breitve MH, Rongve A, Aarsland D, Nordhus IH. The frequency and correlates of anxiety in patients with first-time diagnosed mild dementia. Int Psychogeriatr (2012) 24(11):1771–8. doi:10.1017/S1041610212001020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Chwiszczuk L, Breitve M, Hynninen M, Gjerstad MD, Aarsland D, Rongve A. Higher frequency and complexity of sleep disturbances in dementia with Lewy bodies as compared to Alzheimer’s disease. Neurodegener Dis (2015) 16(3–4):152–60. doi:10.1159/000439252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Breitve MH, Chwiszczuk LJ, Hynninen MJ, Rongve A, Brønnick K, Janvin C, et al. A systematic review of cognitive decline in dementia with Lewy bodies versus Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Res Ther (2014) 6(5–8):53. doi:10.1186/PREACCEPT-1892765652118673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rongve A, Soennesyn H, Skogseth R, Oesterhus R, Hortobágyi T, Ballard CG, et al. Cognitive decline in dementia with Lewy bodies: a 5-year prospective cohort study. BMJ Open (2016) 6(2):e010357. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kramberger MG, Auestad B, Garcia-Ptacek S, Abdelnour C, Olmo JG, Walker Z, et al. Long-term cognitive decline in dementia with Lewy bodies in a large multicenter, International Cohort. J Alzheimers Dis (2017) 57(3):787–95. doi:10.3233/JAD-161109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Blanc F. Does cognitive profile distinguish Lewy body disease from Alzheimer’s disease in the early stages? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2013) 84(12):1299–300. doi:10.1136/jnnp-2012-304694

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Wood JS, Firbank MJ, Mosimann UP, Watson R, Barber R, Blamire AM, et al. Testing visual perception in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2013) 21(6):501–8. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2012.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hamilton JM, Salmon DP, Galasko D, Raman R, Emond J, Hansen LA, et al. Visuospatial deficits predict rate of cognitive decline in autopsy-verified dementia with Lewy bodies. Neuropsychology (2008) 22(6):729–37. doi:10.1037/a0012949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wood JS, Watson R, Firbank MJ, Mosimann UP, Barber R, Blamire AM, et al. Longitudinal testing of visual perception in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2013) 28(6):567–72. doi:10.1002/gps.3860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Brønnick K, Breitve MH, Rongve A, Aarsland D. Neurocognitive deficits distinguishing mild dementia with Lewy bodies from mild Alzheimer’s disease is associated with parkinsonism. J Alzheimers Dis (2016) 53(4):1277–85. doi:10.3233/JAD-160294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Aarsland D, Rongve A, Nore SP, Skogseth R, Skulstad S, Ehrt U, et al. Frequency and case identification of dementia with Lewy bodies using the revised consensus criteria. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2008) 26(5):445–52. doi:10.1159/000165917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology (1984) 34:939–44. doi:10.1212/WNL.34.7.939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. McKeith IG. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with lewy bodies (DLB): report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. J Alzheimers Dis (2006) 9(3 Suppl):417–23.

Google Scholar

18. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res (1975) 12(3):189–98. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Morris JC. Clinical dementia rating: a reliable and valid diagnostic and staging measure for dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int Psychogeriatr (1997) 9(Suppl 1):173–6; discussion 177–8. doi:10.1017/S1041610297004870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test – Second Edition. Adult Version. Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation (2000).

Google Scholar

21. Armitage SG. An analysis of certain psychological tests used for the evaluation of brain injury. Psychol Monogr (1946) 60(1):1–48. doi:10.1037/h0093567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Golden CJ. Stroop Colour and Word Test. Chicago: Stoelting (1979).

Google Scholar

23. Benton AL, Hamsher K. Multilingual Aphasia Examination Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: University of Iowa Press (1976).

Google Scholar

24. Mack WJ, Freed DM, Williams BW, Henderson VW. Boston naming test: shortened versions for use in Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol (1992) 47(3):154–8. doi:10.1093/geronj/47.3.P154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Warrington EK, James M. The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Bury St Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company (1991).

Google Scholar

26. Team RC. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2014).

Google Scholar

27. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-plus. Statistics and Computing (1978).

Google Scholar

28. Metzler-Baddeley C. A review of cognitive impairments in dementia with Lewy bodies relative to Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Cortex (2007) 43(5):583–600. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70489-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Aretouli E, Tsilidis KK, Brandt J. Four-year outcome of mild cognitive impairment: the contribution of executive dysfunction. Neuropsychology (2013) 27(1):95–106. doi:10.1037/a0030481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Yu L, Boyle PA, Leurgans SE, Bennett DA. Cognitive impairment, decline and fluctuations in older community-dwelling subjects with Lewy bodies. Brain (2012) 135(10):3005–14. doi:10.1093/brain/aws234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gauthier SS, Albert M, Fox N, Goedert M, Kivipelto M, Mestre-Ferrandiz J, et al. Why has therapy development for dementia failed in the last two decades? Alzheimers Dement (2016) 12(1):60–4. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Parikh M, Hynan LS, Weiner MF, Lacritz L, Ringe W, Cullum CM. Single neuropsychological test scores associated with rate of cognitive decline in early Alzheimer disease. Clin Neuropsychol (2014) 28(6):926–40. doi:10.1080/13854046.2014.944937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Buccione I, Perri R, Carlesimo GA, Fadda L, Serra L, Scalmana S, et al. Cognitive and behavioural predictors of progression rates in Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurol (2007) 14(4):440–6. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2007.01693.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Marra C, Silveri MC, Gainotti G. Predictors of cognitive decline in the early stage of probable Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2000) 11(4):212–8. doi:10.1159/000017239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lessig S, Nie D, Xu R, Corey-Bloom J. Changes on brief cognitive instruments over time in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord (2012) 27(9):1125–8. doi:10.1002/mds.25070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Skogseth R, Hortobagyi T, Soennesyn H, Chwiszczuk L, Ffytche D, Rongve A, et al. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies versus neuropathology. J Alzheimers Dis (2017) 59(4):1139–52. doi:10.3233/JAD-170274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, cognitive decline, longitudinal, neuropsychology

Citation: Breitve MH, Chwiszczuk LJ, Brønnick K, Hynninen MJ, Auestad BH, Aarsland D and Rongve A (2018) A Longitudinal Study of Neurocognition in Dementia with Lewy Bodies Compared to Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Neurol. 9:124. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00124

Received: 08 September 2017; Accepted: 20 February 2018;
Published: 06 March 2018

Edited by:

Marjan Jahanshahi, UCL Institute of Neurology, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Gianfranco Spalletta, Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS), Italy
Angie A. Kehagia, King’s College London, United Kingdom

Copyright: © 2018 Breitve, Chwiszczuk, Brønnick, Hynninen, Auestad, Aarsland and Rongve. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Monica H. Breitve, monica.breitve@helse-fonna.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.