Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 26 July 2018
Sec. Neurotrauma

Association of Visual Tracking Metrics With Post-concussion Symptomatology

  • 1Brain Trauma Foundation, New York, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Neurosurgery, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, United States

Attention impairment may provide a cohesive neurobiological explanation for clusters of clinical symptoms that occur after a concussion; therefore, objective quantification of attention is needed. Visually tracking a moving target is an attention-dependent sensorimotor function, and eye movement can be recorded easily and objectively to quantify performance. Our previous work suggested the utility of gaze-target synchronization metrics of a predictive visual tracking task in concussion screening and recovery monitoring. Another objectively quantifiable performance measure frequently suggested for concussion screening is simple visuo-manual reaction time (simple reaction time, SRT). Here, we used visual tracking and SRT tasks to assess changes between pre- and within-2-week post-concussion performances and explore their relationships to post-concussion symptomatology. Athletes participating in organized competitive sports were recruited. Visual tracking and SRT records were collected from the recruited athlete pool as baseline measures over a 4-year period. When athletes experienced a concussion, they were re-assessed within 2 weeks of their injury. We present the data from a total of 29 concussed athletes. Post-concussion symptom burden was assessed with the Rivermead Post-Concussion Symptoms Questionnaire and subscales of the Brain Injury Screening Questionnaire. Post-concussion changes in visual tracking and SRT performance were examined using a paired t-test. Correlations of changes in visual tracking and SRT performance to symptom burden were examined using Pearson's coefficients. Post-concussion changes in visual tracking performance were not consistent among the athletes. However, changes in several visual tracking metrics had moderate to strong correlations to symptom scales (r up to 0.68). On the other hand, while post-concussion SRT performance was reduced (p < 0.01), the changes in the performance metrics were not meaningfully correlated to symptomatology (r ≤ 0.33). Results suggest that visual tracking performance metrics reflect clinical symptoms when assessed within 2 weeks of concussion. Evaluation of concussion requires assessments in multiple domains because the clinical profiles are heterogeneous. While most individuals show recovery within a week of injury, others experience prolonged recovery periods. Visual tracking performance metrics may serve as a biomarker of debilitating symptoms of concussion implicating attention as a root cause of such pathologies.

Introduction

Cognitive abilities are often compromised after a concussion, with attention dysfunction being a particularly common sequela (1, 2). Since attention is a vital component of everyday functioning and even minor impairment may be subjectively debilitating, assessment of attention needs to be a key component of outcome measures for concussion. Furthermore, attention impairment may provide a cohesive neurobiological explanation for clusters of clinical symptoms that occur after a concussion (35). For these reasons, objective quantification of attention is needed. Given that eye-movement and attention processes share common neuronal circuitry (6, 7) and that eye movement can be recorded quickly and objectively to quantify performance, an outcome measure for post-concussion attentional functioning may be soundly derived from eye movement testing.

Visual tracking, also known as ocular pursuit, supports perceptual stability of a moving object of interest with a combination of saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements. Visual tracking requires multiple components of attention, including target selection, sustained engagement, spatio-temporal working memory, prediction of the temporal course of the stimulus, and binding of the prediction and the required action (812). In particular, because the visual and motor processing delay of ≈100 ms (8, 13) would preclude visual interception of the target if one was to simply react to the visual input of the target, accurate and precise synchronization must be achieved through shaping anticipatory eye movement. Such shaping is likely to take place with contributions of the fronto-parietal cortical regions and the cerebellum (14), which are important foci of attention processing. Visual tracking performance is known to be, while variable between individuals, normally stable over time periods of weeks to months, and changes detected outside the limit of reliability express alterations in the brain (12, 1517). Our previous work, using a predictive visual tracking task in combination with neuropsychological assessment and diffusion tensor imaging, associated visual tracking synchronization metrics with cognitive functioning and integrity of frontal white matter tracts that are vulnerable to a concussive impact (18). There is also vulnerability of the cerebellum to concussion (1921). Therefore, a predictive visual tracking task would be a useful test paradigm to provide attentional bases for concussion screening and recovery monitoring with objective sensorimotor synchronization metrics (5, 18, 22).

Simple visuo-manual reaction time (simple reaction time, SRT) is another objectively quantifiable measure suggested for concussion screening (23, 24). An SRT task is also considered attention-dependent, invoking vigilance and a top-down control of detecting the target (23, 25). As with visual tracking, SRT performance shows both inter-individual variability and within-individual stability over a normal course of time (2628), bearing biometric characteristics. However, the reactive nature of the behavior contrasts with predictive visual tracking, and the two behaviors may allude to the wellness of separate neural substrates (29).

Concussion clinical profiles are heterogeneous and may require subtyping to facilitate targeted treatments (3032). While we have sought a unified explanation for divergent post-concussion clinical symptoms with attention impairment (4, 5), that attention too is multi-faceted and supported by sub-processes (33, 34) complicates matters. As yet, associations between clinical symptoms and cognitive impairments that follow a concussion are not clear (24, 3537). There is indeed much to be explored beyond mere detection of concussion and toward characterization of heterogeneous traits. Here, we used visual tracking and SRT tasks to assess changes between pre- and within-2-week post-concussion performances and explore their relationships to post-concussion symptomatology.

Materials and Methods

Subject Enrollment

The subject enrollment and testing protocols were approved by the institutional review boards of Weill Cornell Medical College (WCMC) in New York, and Stanford University in California. In collaboration with school, university, and community athletic organizations in respective local areas, middle-school through adult athletes were enrolled and baseline tested. Prior to data collection, written informed consent by adult subjects, or legal guardians of minor subjects with the minors' assent, was obtained in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Testing was conducted inside a parked recreational vehicle outfitted as a mobile testing site, or at the Citigroup Biomedical Imaging Center at WCMC. Data collection spanned from September of 2012 through September of 2016.

By research design, the athletes were retrospectively screened after the baseline testing for inclusion in the current analysis. Pre-screening for eligibility was deemed inappropriate because of the likelihood of singling out ineligible members from others belonging to same teams. The inclusion criteria were participation in organized competitive athletic activity, being of age 12–30, normal or corrected to normal vision, and for athletes over the age 18, a high school diploma or equivalent, or expected timely high school graduation. The exclusion criteria were a prior history of traumatic brain injury (including concussion), alcohol or substance abuse, a known neurologic disorder, or a psychiatric condition previously known or identified using questionnaires for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, depression, and anxiety disorders, and a known vision-related disease or abnormality. Of a total of 3,091 athletes initially enrolled, 1,442 were selected for the study. More than one half of baselined sample (903 athletes) was excluded for a prior injury to the head.

During the baseline consent process, the athletes were given the option of allowing their athletic director, trainer, coach, or school nurse to contact the researchers if they sustain a concussion, as well as the research staff to contact the athletic staff to check on injury status. A diagnosis by a physician was not required. Prospective acute post-concussion enrollment was based on inclusion criteria consisting of an experience within 2 weeks of a concussion that resulted in loss of consciousness, post-traumatic amnesia, dizziness, nausea, headaches, balance problems, blurred or double vision, or daze and confusion, and on an exclusion criterion of intoxication at the time of injury.

Visual Tracking Test

The details of the eye movement testing methods were described previously (12). Briefly, subjects performed a visual tracking task on a video-based eye tracker integrated with stimulus-presentation (EyeLink 1000, SR Research Ltd., Mississauga, Ontario, Canada). The stimulus was presented on a 120 Hz LCD monitor (SyncMaster 2233RZ, Samsung, Seoul, South Korea). The stimulus consisted of a red target of 0.5° of visual angle that was presented against a black background. The target traveled at a constant speed of 25.1°/s in a clockwise direction along a circular path with a radius of 10°. The task was performed in a normally lit room while subjects sat with their head stabilized by a chin-head rest. The semi-automated testing sequence lasted ~5 min, and included text and recorded audio instructions, a 5-s practice run, calibration, validation, and two 15-s test runs. The visual acuity of each subject was confirmed to be normal or corrected-to-normal prior to testing using a handheld vision chart.

Collected eye movement data were screened with an automated algorithm for >10% of missing data, artifacts associated with inadequate quality of calibration, or poor head stabilization during recording as evidenced by a large change in visual fixation records and were also visually inspected. Validated eye movement data were processed using a standardized method to derive performance metrics. To characterize the stability of the gaze on the target, we evaluated the variability of gaze position error along axes orthogonal (radial) and parallel (tangential) to target movement (standard deviation of radial and tangential errors—SDRE, SDTE). The smaller the SDRE or SDTE value, the more precise the tracking. To characterize the central tendency of gaze position relative to the target, we evaluated the mean radial error and the mean phase error. A negative radial error indicated the gaze drawing a smaller circular trajectory than the target. A negative phase error indicated the gaze trailing the target or phase lag. We also computed the horizontal and vertical smooth pursuit velocity gains (H and V gains), which were the ratios between the amplitudes of modulation of smooth pursuit eye velocity and target velocity. A smaller gain indicated less accurate tracking.

Simple Reaction Time Test

We collected simple visuo-manual reaction time data using the SRT component of the Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics Version 4 (ANAM4) (38). The SRT test was added to the testing protocol after the initiation of the study; therefore subjects who were enrolled in the study at an early stage did not receive this test. Other components of the ANAM4 library were not deployed. The stimulus was a large asterisk symbol presented at the center of a blank computer screen. The subjects were instructed to press a response key as quickly as possible each time the stimulus was presented. There were 40 trials in a test and the results were automatically analyzed by the software. The software discarded a response made in <130 ms after the cue presentation in analysis. We chose the median and throughput outputs to characterize the subjects' performance. Median was used to describe each subject's central tendency of reaction times, the intra-individual distribution of which is typically skewed. The throughput metric was defined as the number of proper responses per min.

Symptom Assessment

The Rivermead Post-Concussion Symptoms Questionnaire (RPQ) (39) measures the severity of 16 problems that commonly follow a concussion. The subjects were asked to rate changes in symptoms from 0 to 4 in comparison to the pre-injury state, corresponding to “not experienced,” “no more of a problem,” “mild problem,” “moderate problem,” and “severe problem,” and total scores were calculated as the sum of all symptom scores with a range from 0 to 64. Types of symptoms were not analyzed based on sub-test scores of the RPQ (40).

The Brain Injury Screening Questionnaire (BISQ) (41) is a screening tool for documenting lifetime history of traumatic brain injury and the presence of current symptoms and for ruling out alternative explanations for reported symptoms. A section of BISQ is an inventory of 100 four-point-rated symptoms that characterize a comprehensive range of transient and chronic problems after brain injury. A factor analysis of these symptoms revealed four main factors (42), which were labeled as memory-attention, depression-anxiety-mood, aggression-impulsivity, and physical. Symptom factor scores are scaled from 0 to 3, with 0 indicating no symptoms.

Statistical Analysis

We analyzed changes in visual tracking and SRT performances and their relationship to post-concussion symptomatology. Within-individual changes in visual tracking and SRT performances before and after injury were tested with paired t-tests. Relationships between visual tracking and SRT performances and those between changes in performance and post-concussion symptoms were examined with Pearson's correlation coefficient. Analyses were based on all observed data without case deletion or replacing missing data with substituted values. Findings are presented with a statistical significance defined by the alpha level of 0.05, and without correction for multiple comparisons because of the exploratory nature of the study.

Results

In the baseline-tested 1,442 athletes selected for the study, the research staff was made aware of and was able to test 29 cases of concussion meeting the enrollment criteria. There were 14 female and 15 male subjects. The subjects' ages spanned from 14 years and 2 months to 22 years and 5 months, with a mean (SD) of 18.4 (2.3) years. The post-concussion testing took place with the mean (SD) of 5.3 (3.3) days after the injury, and 2.8 (2.5) months after the baseline testing. Valid visual tracking scores from both baseline and acute post-injury assessments were available from 20 of the 29 subjects. Most of the eye tracking technical failures were associated with poor head stabilization. Four of the 29 subjects did not undergo the SRT test at baseline as they were enrolled before the test was implemented in the testing protocol. A maximum of five anticipatory responses and no timeouts were recorded in any subject. Most tests were completed with 40 valid trials with a mean (SD) of 39.4 (1.0) trials. Altogether, valid baseline and acute post-injury SRT scores were available from 25 subjects. All 29 subjects had RPQ and BISQ scores.

A degradation in visual tracking performance after experiencing a concussion was not observed consistently, with only H gain showing a change at p < 0.05 (Table 1; Figure 1). However, changes in SDRE, SDTE, and mean phase error moderately correlated with RPQ scores, with increased gaze position variability and a phase lead being associated with reports of more severe symptomatology that presented after a concussion (Table 2; Figure 2, leftmost column). The RPQ scores ranged from 0 to 26 (mean 12.2) among the 29 subjects, and from 0 to 26 (mean 11.1) among the 20 subjects with valid baseline and post-injury visual tracking scores. The same visual tracking metrics also correlated with the memory-attention and physical symptom subscale scores of the BISQ at a moderate to strong level (Figure 2, middle and rightmost columns). Table 3 shows a sample of questionnaire items factorized together under these subscales. Correlations between visual tracking metrics and the BISQ depression-anxiety-mood and aggression-impulsivity symptom subscales were weak at best and not statistically significant (Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Within-individual changes from the baseline in visual tracking and reaction time performances within 2 weeks after a concussion.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Baseline and acute post-concussion performance scores. Circle markers in each panel indicate individual subjects. The diagonal lines indicate no change from the baseline measurement. Also see Table 1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson correlations between performance changes and symptom scores.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Visual tracking performance changes are associated with post-concussion symptomatology. Circle markers in each panel indicate individual subjects. A regression line is drawn to indicate a trend. A positive change in SDRE and SDTE indicates an increased gaze position variability during tracking of a moving target. A positive change in mean phase error indicates a phase advancement. Also see Table 2.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Example symptom items factorized together under memory-attention and physical subscales of BISQ.

SRT performance was degraded after experiencing a concussion, with the throughput metric showing a statistically significant change (p = 0.008, Table 1). Measured in terms of median, reaction times were also longer overall, although a t-test failed to show a statistically significant change because one subject representing an extreme, rather than a reverse, of the trend contributed to the overall increase in the sample variance (Figure 1, top right panel). An inspection of this subject's raw reaction time records showed 40 valid responses ranging from 434 to 1,404 ms. While this subject also had the lowest throughput after the concussion, with the throughput metric being reciprocally related to the mean of reaction times, the deviation associated with slowed response was less exaggerated. The visual tracking performance of this subject was not exceptional either at baseline or post-injury, or in terms of changes between baseline and post-injury scores. The SRT metrics did not meaningfully correlate with any of the symptom scores (Table 2; Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. SRT performance changes are associated with post-concussion symptomatology. Circle markers in each panel indicate individual subjects. A regression line is drawn to indicate a trend. A positive change in median or a negative change in throughput indicates slowed reaction. Also see Table 2.

Normal interdependence between visual tracking and SRT metrics is low (29), but changes in the overall integrity of the attention system may impact across its different components (18, 43). We thus tested for correlations between visual tracking and SRT metrics in terms of their post-concussion changes from the baseline. However, among the 19 subjects who had valid baseline and post-concussion data for both visual tracking and SRT tests, the correlation between changes were very weak at best (r < 0.20) and not statistically significant.

Discussion

The normally expected within-individual stability of visual tracking performance can be disrupted by a concussion. We found that when baseline-tested athlete subjects were reassessed within 2 weeks of a concussion, changes in visual tracking performance reflected clinical symptoms. Post-concussion symptom burden was assessed with the RPQ, which evaluates differences from the pre-injury state, and with the BISQ, which quantifies symptom severity in four separate dimensions. Symptomatic subjects had reduced precision and accuracy of gaze-target synchronization. Specifically, these visual tracking performance changes were linked to memory-attention and physical symptoms, but not with depression-anxiety-mood or aggression-impulsivity symptoms.

Visuo-manual reaction was slowed following a concussion. This finding substantiates that the impact to the head sustained by the athlete subjects in fact had a neurological effect. This affirmation is despite the fact that the testing usually took place days after the injury, but is consistent with a previous report that, even though the greatest recovery from clinical symptoms typically takes place during the first several days after the injury (44, 45), a reaction deficit can persist for a longer period (35). Such apparent dissociation between degraded reaction performance and the contemporaneous subjective symptomatology is consistent with the results of the direct correlation analysis that we conducted. It is not clear whether slowed response becomes apparent to the individual in a subtle way or whether it is coupled to symptoms that we did not quantify. It is also not clear how attention subcomponents that support visual tracking and SRT performances interact in daily activities under normal and post-concussion conditions.

Because of heterogeneity in the presentations of concussion and the possible existence of clinical subtypes (3032), evaluation of concussion requires assessments in multiple domains. As stated in INTRODUCTION, predictive visual tracking and reaction time measurements may allude to the wellness of separate neural substrates (29). The current results with visual tracking and reaction time metrics reflecting different aspects of post-concussion profiles support this view. In particular, we have postulated that impaired predictive timing, as measured by visual tracking synchronization metrics, is the primary basis for the post-concussion attention deficit and that further cognitive, somatic, or affective symptoms can be explained as secondary consequences of impaired predictive timing (35). That visual tracking metrics were linked to memory-attention and physical symptoms are in line with these accounts, and these metrics may serve as an attentional biomarker that quantifies the impact associated with debilitating symptoms within these domains. On the other hand, symptoms in depression-anxiety-mood and aggression-impulsivity domains require alternative explanations because similar links were not found. Assessment of impairments to other sensorimotor systems such as proprioceptive, vestibular, and ocular vergence mechanisms should present further clues to the puzzle (32, 46, 47).

The sample was a prospective cohort from a natural setting of competitive sports. Although an enormous amount of resources was devoted for baseline testing, the culminated sample size was limited. Over one half of the baselined athletes met the exclusion criteria, the primary reason being a prior head injury. Consequently, our sample represents only a selective subgroup of a typical athlete population. The sample size was unfortunately further reduced because the timing of data processing was determined by the prospective study design and we lacked the means to screen for the validity of eye movement records at the time of data collection. Improvements in eye tracking technology supplemented by immediately screening the data with an automated algorithm should reduce the technical failure rate. Also constrained by the study design was the timing of post-concussion assessment, which was variable within the 2-week window. Since trajectories of symptom recovery vary among individuals (35, 37), it was unclear in the present study if individuals with low symptom scores were those who recovered or those who were asymptomatic. A study with an initial assessment timed closer to the injury and with longitudinal follow-ups would inform varying trajectories of recovery following a concussion.

Conclusion

Post-concussion symptoms are subjective but can be debilitating. Evaluation of concussion requires assessments in multiple domains because the clinical profiles are heterogeneous and patients can benefit from targeted treatments. However, it is difficult to determine the root causes of various clinical symptoms. While most concussed individuals are said to subjectively experience recovery within a week of injury, deficits in objective measures may linger for some. Still others experience prolonged recovery periods. We sought to associate objective attention measures with clinical symptoms to elucidate their underlying pathophysiology. Quantification of attention, particularly predictive timing, based on visual tracking performance may render a biological explanation for cognitive and physical symptoms that occur after a concussion as a result of changes in the supporting neural circuit.

Author Contributions

JM and JG designed experiments and oversaw data collection and analysis. JM, LS, and UR contributed to data management and conducted the statistical analyses. JM drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the interpretation of data and to revising the work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

JG is director of Sync-Think, Inc. and the inventor of U.S. patent 7,384,399. JM holds stock option in Sync-Think. LS and UR have a consulting arrangement with Sync-Think. JM, UR, and JG are inventors of pending patent application PCT/US2016/027923 related to the subject matter described in this article.

The authors declare no other potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Brain Trauma Foundation's clinical research staff for assistance with data collection and maintenance. This work was supported by U.S. Department of Defense contracts W911QY-12-C-0005 and W911QY-14-C-0086.

Abbreviations

BISQ, Brain Injury Screening Questionnaire; RPQ, Rivermead Post-Concussion Symptoms Questionnaire; SD, standard deviation; SDRE, SD of radial errors; SDTE, SD of tangential errors; SRT, simple reaction time.

References

1. Broglio SP, Pontifex MB, O'Connor P, Hillman CH. The persistent effects of concussion on neuroelectric indices of attention. J Neurotrauma (2009) 26:1463–70. doi: 10.1089/neu.2008-0766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Howell D, Osternig L, Van Donkelaar P, Mayr U, Chou LS. Effects of concussion on attention and executive function in adolescents. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2013) 45:1030–7. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3182814595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ghajar J, Ivry RB. The predictive brain state: timing deficiency in traumatic brain injury? Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2008) 22:217–27. doi: 10.1177/1545968308315600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Maruta J, Lee SW, Jacobs EF, Ghajar J. A unified science of concussion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2010) 1208:58–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05695.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Maruta J, Tong J, Lee SW, Iqbal Z, Schonberger A, Ghajar J. EYE-TRAC: monitoring attention and utility for mTBI. Proc SPIE (2012) 8371:83710L. doi: 10.1117/12.927790

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Drury HA, et al. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron (1998) 21:761–73. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80593-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Ohlendorf S, Kimmig H, Glauche V, Haller S. Gaze pursuit, ‘attention pursuit’ and their effects on cortical activations. Eur J Neurosci. (2007) 26:2096–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05824.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lisberger SG, Morris EJ, Tychsen L. Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. Annu Rev Neurosci. (1987) 10:97–129. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.000525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Chen Y, Holzman PS, Nakayama K. Visual and cognitive control of attention in smooth pursuit. Prog Brain Res. (2002) 140:255–65. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(02)40055-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Krauzlis RJ. The control of voluntary eye movements: new perspectives. Neuroscientist (2005) 11:124–37. doi: 10.1177/1073858404271196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Barnes GR. Cognitive processes involved in smooth pursuit eye movements. Brain Cogn. (2008) 68:309–26. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Maruta J, Heaton KJ, Kryskow EM, Maule AL, Ghajar J. Dynamic visuomotor synchronization: quantification of predictive timing. Behav Res Methods (2013) 45:289–300. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0248-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tavassoli A, Ringach DL. Dynamics of smooth pursuit maintenance. J Neurophysiol. (2009) 102:110–8. doi: 10.1152/jn.91320.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Metcalf NV, Rengachary J, MacDonald CL, Harrington DL, et al. Abnormal white matter blood-oxygen-leveldependent signals in chronic mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2015) 32:1254–71. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Roy-Byrne P, Radant A, Wingerson D, Cowley DS. Human oculomotor function: reliability and diurnal variation. Biol Psychiatry (1995) 38:92–7. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00225-R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ettinger U, Kumari V, Crawford TJ, Davis RE, Sharma T, Corr PJ. Reliability of smooth pursuit, fixation, and saccadic eye movements. Psychophysiology (2003) 40:620–8. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bargary G, Bosten JM, Goodbourn PT, Lawrance-Owen AJ, Hogg RE, Mollon JD. Individual differences in human eye movements: an oculomotor signature? Vision Res. (2017) 141:157–69. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Maruta J, Suh M, Niogi SN, Mukherjee P, Ghajar J. Visual tracking synchronization as a metric for concussion screening. J Head Trauma Rehabil. (2010) 25:293–305. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181e67936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fukuda K, Aihara N, Sagar SM, Sharp FR, Pitts LH, Honkaniemi J, et al. Purkinje cell vulnerability to mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (1996) 13:255–66. doi: 10.1089/neu.1996.13.255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Alhilali LM, Yaeger K, Collins M, Fakhran S. Detection of central white matter injury underlying vestibulopathy after mild traumatic brain injury. Radiology (2014) 272:224–32. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14132670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wang Z, Wu W, Liu Y, Wang T, Chen X, Zhang J, et al. Altered cerebellar white matter integrity in patients with mild traumatic brain injury in the acute stage. PLoS ONE (2016) 11:e0151489. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Maruta J, Heaton KJ, Maule AL, Ghajar J. Predictive visual tracking: specificity in mild traumatic brain injury and sleep deprivation. Mil Med. (2014) 179:619–25. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-13-00420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Warden DL, Bleiberg J, Cameron KL, Ecklund J, Walter J, Sparling MB, et al. Persistent prolongation of simple reaction time in sports concussion. Neurology (2001) 57:524–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.3.524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Collie A, Makdissi M, Maruff P, Bennell K, McCrory P. Cognition in the days following concussion: comparison of symptomatic versus asymptomatic athletes. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2006) 77:241–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.073155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Barthelemy S, Boulinguez P. Manual reaction time asymmetries in human subjects: the role of movement planning and attention. Neurosci Lett. (2001) 315:41–4. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02313-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Cole WR, Arrieux JP, Schwab K, Ivins BJ, Qashu FM, Lewis SC. Test-retest reliability of four computerized neurocognitive assessment tools in an active duty military population. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2013) 28:732–42. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Dretsch M, Parish R, Kelly M, Coldren R, Russell M. Eight-day temporal stability of the Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metric (ANAM) in a deployment environment. Appl Neuropsychol Adult (2015) 22:304–10. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2014.926454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Vincent AS, Roebuck-Spencer T, Gilliland K, Schlegel R. Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics (v4) Traumatic Brain Injury Battery: military normative data. Mil Med. (2012) 177:256–69. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-11-00289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Maruta J, Spielman LA, Rajashekar U, Ghajar J. Visual tracking in development and aging. Front Neurol. (2017) 8:640. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cicerone KD, Kalmar K. Persistent postconcussion syndrome: the structure of subjective complaints after mild traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. (1995) 10:1–17. doi: 10.1097/00001199-199510030-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Aubry M, Cantu R, Dvorak J, Graf-Baumann T, Johnston K, Kelly J, et al. Summary and agreement statement of the First International Conference on Concussion in Sport, Vienna 2001. Br J Sports Med. (2002) 36:6–10. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.36.1.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

32. Collins MW, Kontos AP, Okonkwo DO, Almquist J, Bailes J, Barisa M, et al. Statements of Agreement From the Targeted Evaluation and Active Management (TEAM) Approaches to Treating Concussion Meeting Held in Pittsburgh, October 15-16, 2015. Neurosurgery (2016) 79:912–29. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0000000000001447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Knudsen EI. Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2007) 30:57–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2012) 35:73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Broglio SP, Macciocchi SN, Ferrara MS. Neurocognitive performance of concussed athletes when symptom free. J Athl Train (2007) 42:504–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

36. Fazio VC, Lovell MR, Pardini JE, Collins MW. The relation between post concussion symptoms and neurocognitive performance in concussed athletes. NeuroRehabil. (2007) 22:207–16.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

37. McCrea M, Guskiewicz K, Randolph C, Barr WB, Hammeke TA, Marshall SW, et al. Incidence, clinical course, and predictors of prolonged recovery time following sport-related concussion in high school and college athletes. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2013) 19:22–33. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Reeves DL, Winter KP, Bleiberg J, Kane RL. ANAM genogram: historical perspectives, description, and current endeavors. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2007) 22(Suppl. 1):S15–37. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. King NS, Crawford S, Wenden FJ, Moss NE, Wade DT. The Rivermead Post Concussion Symptoms Questionnaire: a measure of symptoms commonly experienced after head injury and its reliability. J Neurol. (1995) 242:587–92. doi: 10.1007/BF00868811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Smith-Seemiller L, Fow NR, Kant R, Franzen MD. Presence of post-concussion syndrome symptoms in patients with chronic pain vs mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. (2003) 17:199–206. doi: 10.1080/0269905021000030823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Gordon WA, Haddad L, Brown M, Hibbard MR, Sliwinski M. The sensitivity and specificity of self-reported symptoms in individuals with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. (2000) 14:21–33. doi: 10.1080/026990500120907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gordon WA, Spielman LA, Hahn-Ketter AE, Sy KTL. The relationship between traumatic brain injury and criminality in juvenile offenders. J Head Trauma Rehabil. (2017) 32:393–403. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Heaton KJ, Maule AL, Maruta J, Kryskow EM, Ghajar J. Attention and visual tracking degradation during acute sleep deprivation in a military sample. Aviat Space Environ Med. (2014) 85:497–503. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.3882.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. McCrea M, Guskiewicz KM, Marshall SW, Barr W, Randolph C, Cantu RC, et al. Acute effects and recovery time following concussion in collegiate football players: the NCAA Concussion Study. JAMA (2003) 290:2556–63. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.19.2556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Eisenberg MA, Andrea J, Meehan W, Mannix R. Time interval between concussions and symptom duration. Pediatrics (2013) 132:8–17. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Guskiewicz KM. Postural stability assessment following concussion: one piece of the puzzle. Clin J Sport Med. (2001) 11:182–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

47. Mucha A, Collins MW, Elbin RJ, Furman JM, Troutman-Enseki C, DeWolf RM, et al. A Brief Vestibular/Ocular Motor Screening (VOMS) assessment to evaluate concussions: preliminary findings. Am J Sports Med. (2014) 42:2479–86. doi: 10.1177/0363546514543775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: closed head injury, mild traumatic brain injury, ocular pursuit, predictive timing, smooth pursuit

Citation: Maruta J, Spielman LA, Rajashekar U and Ghajar J (2018) Association of Visual Tracking Metrics With Post-concussion Symptomatology. Front. Neurol. 9:611. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00611

Received: 21 February 2018; Accepted: 09 July 2018;
Published: 26 July 2018.

Edited by:

Jack Tsao, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, United States

Reviewed by:

Joseph Bleiberg, National Intrepid Center of Excellence (NICoE), United States
John-Ross Rizzo, New York University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Maruta, Spielman, Rajashekar and Ghajar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jun Maruta, jun.maruta@mssm.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.