Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Neurol., 21 September 2018
Sec. Movement Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Current State of Postural Research - Beyond Automatic Behavior View all 20 articles

New Insights on Emotional Contributions to Human Postural Control

  • 1Department of Kinesiology, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, Canada
  • 2School of Kinesiology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 3Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 4International Collaboration on Repair Discoveries, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

It has been just over 20 years since the effects of height-induced threat on human postural control were first investigated. Raising the height of the support surface on which individuals stood increased the perceived consequences of instability and generated postural control changes. Since this initial work, converging evidence has accumulated supporting the efficacy of using height-induced threat to study the effects of emotions on postural control and confirming a direct influence of threat-related changes in arousal, anxiety, and fear of falling on all aspects of postural control, including standing, anticipatory, and reactive balance. In general, threat-related postural changes promote a greater physical safety margin while maintaining upright stance. We use the static balance literature to critically examine the current state of knowledge regarding: (1) the extent to which threat-related changes in postural control are sensitive to threat-related changes in emotions; (2) the underlying neurophysiological and cognitive mechanisms that may contribute to explaining the relationship between emotions and postural control; and (3) the generalizability of threat-related changes across different populations and types of threat. These findings have important implications for understanding the neuromechanisms that control healthy balance, and highlight the need to recognize the potential contributions of psychological and physiological factors to balance deficits associated with age or pathology. We conclude with a discussion of the practical significance of this research, its impact on improving diagnosis and treatment of postural control deficits, and potential directions for future research.

Background

Fear of falling is frequently reported in older adults (1, 2) and patients with balance deficits (38) and is a significant predictor of future falls risk (9, 10). Maki et al. (11) were the first to report significant differences in balance control between fearful and non-fearful older adults, followed by evidence of balance control changes in individuals with anxiety disorders and phobias (12, 13). While these observational studies provided important evidence for a link between balance deficits and emotions, such as fear and anxiety, the direction of the relationship was not determined due to limitations of the cross-sectional design (i.e., individuals may be fearful because they have underlying balance deficits, or have balance deficits because they have an underlying fear of falling).

Brown and Frank (14) were the first to use an experimental design to examine the direct effect of postural threat on human balance control. These researchers employed a modified version of an elevated surface height paradigm used extensively to study fear and anxiety behaviors in animals [elevated plus maze, (15)]. When young healthy adults stood on an elevated (0.8 m) platform and responded to an unpredictable forward push to the trunk, they leaned back away from the platform edge and stiffened to constrain the forward movement of the body's center of mass (COM). A series of studies followed to examine the effects of postural threat on standing postural control in young healthy adults (1618). Collectively, these studies revealed threat-related postural changes that included leaning away from the platform edge (or away from the direction of the perceived threat), and decreased amplitude and increased frequency of center of pressure (COP) displacements during quiet standing. These threat-related responses were more pronounced with the eyes open and when forward stepping was restricted by the edge of the platform (16). Furthermore, these threat-related changes were scaled to the level of postural threat with progressive decreases in sway amplitude and increases in sway frequency observed with increasing surface heights up to 1.6 m (17). The combination of decreased amplitude and increased frequency of COP displacements suggested the adoption of an ankle stiffening strategy (19). With the body modeled as an inverted pendulum when standing quietly, increased muscle activity around the ankle joints would act to tighten control of the COM within the limits of the base-of-support (19, 20). This hypothesis was experimentally confirmed by observations of increased ankle muscle stiffness when standing at height, coupled with EMG changes consistent with increased co-contraction of lower leg muscles, and decreased COM displacements (18). Together, these early studies revealed that threat-related postural changes provided protection against a loss of balance by limiting body position and movement in the direction of the perceived risk associated with the threat. These changes in humans coincide with freezing and stiffening behavior observed in anxious animals when moving on elevated surfaces (21).

Since these initial studies, physically raising the height of the support surface on which individuals stand has been used extensively to: (1) confirm the effects on standing balance control in young and older healthy adults (2236), and patient populations such as individuals with unilateral vestibular loss (37) and Parkinson's disease (38, 39); (2) extend the effects of threat on different types of postural tasks including anticipatory postural control (34, 4042), reactive postural control (43, 44), functional balance tasks [e.g., one leg stance; (28)], and normal and adaptive gait (4553); and (3) explore the neural mechanisms underlying these threat effects (44, 5467). Studies have also provided converging evidence to confirm that the threat of standing on elevated surfaces (i.e., real or virtual) can evoke psychological (e.g., self-reported increases in perceived anxiety and fear) and physiological responses (e.g., increases in electrodermal activity, blood pressure) typically observed in fearful or anxious conditions [e.g., (25, 2830, 32, 34, 35, 40)]. Furthermore, significant relationships have been observed between threat-induced emotional as well as cognitive changes (e.g., conscious control of posture) and modifications in postural control [e.g., (28, 30, 64)].

Evaluating the Effects of Height-Induced Postural Threat On Standing Balance Control

Given the breadth of research on this topic over the past 20 years, we chose to focus on height-induced postural threat effects on standing balance control, as this represents the majority of studies conducted to date, and has the potential to influence anticipatory and reactive postural adjustments. In order to critically evaluate and allow for a direct comparison between the studies, we controlled for key factors known to influence standing balance control. A search of PubMed, PsychINFO, EMBASE, CINAHL (search terms: postural threat or anxiety, and height, and standing), and hand searches, identified 89 original research articles (non-duplicate). Manual screening removed 51 articles that did not include a manipulation of postural threat/anxiety or a standing task in the study design. The remaining 38 studies were examined, and a subset of studies was selected based on the following five criteria: (1) young or older healthy adults; (2) height threat; (3) quiet standing task; (4) sample duration (≥60 s); and (5) psychological or physiological measure to confirm the efficacy of the threat manipulation. Stance duration was considered a critical factor because it has been shown to significantly affect COP summary measures (6870) and varies widely across studies. At least one physiological (e.g., increased electrodermal activity) or psychological (e.g., increased perceived anxiety) measure was required to confirm that the height manipulation generated a significant emotional effect; this was important given the variability of heights and conditions used to manipulate threat across studies. Based on these criteria, eight studies were identified (Table 1), with six studies focusing specifically on young adults (2830, 32, 34, 35) and two involving older adults (25, 39).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Height-induced postural threat effects on quiet standing.

A consistent postural strategy emerged from the collective results of the eight studies that met our criteria. All studies revealed that young and older healthy adults leaned significantly away from the edge of the platform and significantly increased their COP sway frequency (25, 2830, 32, 34, 35, 39). The majority of the studies also showed that young and healthy older adults decreased their COP sway amplitude (25, 28, 29, 32, 34, 35); two exceptions to this observation reported no significant change in sway amplitude when threatened (30, 39). These observations reinforced the findings of earlier work on standing balance control in young healthy adults (1618) and extended the findings to older healthy adults. All selected studies were performed with eyes open and gaze fixed on near targets (<4-m) to control for effects of postural height vertigo that may occur with longer (>6-m) viewing distances (71, 72). Yet, similar height-induced postural changes have been observed with eyes closed, and also when peripheral vision was occluded (16, 18, 29). The selected studies focused predominantly on anterior-posterior COP changes (which align with the direction of the threat), with similar effects also reported in the medial-lateral direction, albeit to a lesser degree (17, 31), potentially due to the threat direction (42) or biomechanical constraints of controlling anterior-posterior versus medial-lateral sway (20).

Context-Dependent Threat-Related Changes In Standing Balance Control

Studies have utilized other methods to manipulate threat or emotions, to confirm if the effects of height are generalizable to other threat sources, and to avoid some of the context-specific limitations associated with standing on an elevated surface. One common alternative is to manipulate the threat of an impending perturbation, during which individuals are required to stand with or without the threat of experiencing a sudden, unpredictable balance disturbance, such as a push or pull to the upper trunk (73) or a support surface translation (74, 75) or rotation (57). Like height-induced threat, the threat of perturbation has been shown to significantly increase arousal, anxiety, and fear (57). Using the threat of multi-directional perturbations has the advantage of reducing the likelihood of individuals adopting any directionally specific strategies that are inherent to elevated surface paradigms (14). COP displacements during quiet standing when anticipating the threat of forward or backward perturbation are found to significantly increase in frequency and amplitude, with a significant shift of mean position forward instead of backward (75).

Initial comparisons between reported effects of height and perturbation-related threat reveal a common effect of increased frequency of COP displacements during quiet stance (Figure 1A). In contrast, the amplitude of COP displacements and leaning seems context dependent, with smaller amplitude displacements and backwards leaning specific to height-induced threat, and larger amplitude displacements more commonly observed with the threat of a perturbation. While direct comparisons are made difficult by the shorter sample durations typically used in threat of perturbation studies, more recent studies using 60-s durations confirmed the increased amplitude and frequency of COP displacements with this type of threat (76), which are also dependent on the orientation of stance relative to the perceived direction of the threat (Figure 1A). An increase in COP frequency has also been consistently reported in other contexts, including “white coat” effects observed in older women standing under the perceived threat of negative evaluation (78), and young adults standing while viewing affective pictures that elicited increases in arousal, independent of valence (79). In contrast, the increased arousal elicited by mental arithmetic, appears to influence mean position (80), but not COP frequency or amplitude (80, 81), unless coupled with a social evaluative threat (81). Thus, standing balance changes appear to be highly specific to the context, direction, and nature of the perceived threat, which coincides with other threat-avoidance behaviors (82).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Threat context (A) and adaptation (B) effects. (A) Change in center of pressure (COP) mean position (MP), mean power frequency (MPF), and root mean square (RMS) between threat and no threat conditions for height, [0.8 and 3.2 m surface; 60-s stance duration; modified from (34)] and support surface translations (Trans) in the anterior-posterior (AP) direction [30 s stance duration; modified from (75)] and medial-lateral (ML) direction [60-s stance duration; (76)]. Closed circles reflect AP COP measures while open circles reflect ML COP measures. (B) Adaptation of emotional, cognitive (attention focus to movement processes; Att. MP) and postural responses for low (gray circles) and high (black circles) surface heights over 5 repeated 120-s standing trials (77).

Potential Mechanisms Underlying Threat-Related Postural Changes

The mechanisms that may contribute to, or explain threat-related changes in postural control remain poorly understood. In general, theories can be divided along the lines of emotionally-evoked neurophysiological changes, and/or changes in attention.

Neurophysiological theories are based on the existence of highly-integrated neural networks responsible for processing emotional information, such as fear and anxiety, and sensori-motor control of upright stance (83) and gait (84). Neuro-anatomical evidence for direct influences of emotion onto balance control systems has been well-established in animal models (83, 85, 86). Supporting evidence has been established in standing humans, with threat-induced increases observed in: (1) muscle-spindle sensitivity (56, 57, 87); (2) 1b reflex gain (65); and (3) vestibular gain of balance, head and eye-reflexes (6063, 66). While early cortical potentials seem unaffected by threat (55, 56), later cortical changes thought to be responsible for processing sensory information are significantly influenced with threat (44, 55, 88).

Alternatively, changes in attention may mediate threat-related postural changes (82, 84). It is possible that threat influences how attention resources are allocated (e.g., individuals choosing to direct attention to their posture) contributing to the postural changes. Huffman et al. (30) demonstrated that with height-induced threat: (1) individuals had a greater tendency to consciously control and monitor their posture; and (2) an increase in conscious control of posture was related to leaning further back away from the platform edge, independent of any changes in amplitude or frequency of COP displacements. Zaback et al. (64) used open-ended questions to categorize how individuals directed their attention under non-threatening and threatening conditions, with five attention focus categories emerging. When standing at a high compared to low height, individuals directed more attention to movement processes, threat-related stimuli, and self-regulatory or coping strategies, and less attention to task objectives and task-irrelevant information. Again, these threat-related attention focus changes were associated with changes in postural control. For example, individuals who directed more attention to movement processes were more likely to demonstrate increases in frequency of COP displacements, and decreases in amplitude of COP displacements when directing less attention to movement processes. In addition, participants that reported increased attention focus to self-regulatory strategies were more likely to show greater decreases in amplitude of postural displacements. Differences in the approach used to assess attention focus in Zaback et al. (64) and Huffman et al. (30) likely contributed to the differences in reported relationships between attention focus and COP measures across studies. This work linking changes in attention focus and postural control provides preliminary evidence that threat-related changes in attention focus may be a mechanism underlying the postural changes (82).

It is most likely that the effects of threat on balance control rely on a complex interaction between neurophysiological changes and changes in attentional processes. With repeated exposure to height, emotional and attentional changes are attenuated, and correspond to reduced changes in high frequency COP displacements and co-contraction of lower leg muscles. In contrast, initial height-induced posterior leaning and decreases in COP amplitude do not appear to attenuate with repeated exposure and thus may be influenced by other mechanisms (e.g., sensory changes, vigilance) not accounted for in the study [Figure 1B; (77)]. Likewise, changes in perception of balance, which relies on a combination of neurophysiological and cognitive-attentional processes, could also contribute to threat-related changes in postural control. Cleworth et al. (35) demonstrated no change in perceived sway (both self-reported, and tracked in real-time using a hand-held device), in contrast to significant reductions in COP and COM amplitude when standing at a high compared to low height. The incongruency of perceived and actual sway with threat (35) mirrors the reported increase in perceived instability of individuals standing on elevated surfaces, despite no change or an actual decrease in sway amplitude (28, 32, 35).

Clinical Relevance

It is crucial to understand how emotional factors can directly and indirectly influence balance control, as these changes have the potential to mask or modify underlying balance deficits. This is particularly important given the high prevalence of fear and anxiety in populations with balance deficits due to age or pathology such as Parkinson's disease, vestibular disorders, stroke, and multiple sclerosis (18), and links with postural instability and gait deficits (4, 84). Studies have shown that older adults, individuals with vestibular loss and Parkinson's disease have a similar postural response as young healthy adults to a height-induced threat (2426, 36, 37, 39). However, the extent to which balance control deficits in these individuals may be attributed to high levels of state and trait fear or anxiety are still unknown (11, 29, 34, 36).

The capacity for fear and anxiety to directly influence balance in healthy adults provides important insight into potential mechanisms through which clinical balance deficits may present without any clear physiological dysfunction. For example, Chronic Dizziness Disorder and Phobic Postural Vertigo (now unified under the diagnosis of Persistent Postural-Perceptual Dizziness; PPPD) are functional dizziness disorders characterized by non-spinning vertigo and subjective balance instability in the absence of any neurological or structural findings, and often have secondary psychological co-morbidities including fear of falling, anxiety or depressive disorders (89). Postural changes in patients with PPPD include increased high frequency (>1 Hz) sway and increased co-contraction of lower-leg muscles under normal standing conditions (90, 91). These changes become less distinct from healthy controls under conditions of threat (92) or attentional distraction tasks (90). These changes correspond to threat-related changes in healthy adults that adapt to repeated exposure and correlate with changes in conscious attention to movement [(77); Figure 1B]. Overall, these observations support the hypothesis that postural changes with PPPD reflect a maladaptation of high-risk postural control strategies triggered by an initial stimulus that persists due, in part to, excessive self-observation and anxiety (89). Likewise, individuals with visual height intolerance (VHI) have been identified in ~30% of the population, defined as those with “an unpleasant feeling caused by visual exposure to heights” (93). When standing on elevated surfaces (15 m), individuals with VHI have increased tibialis anterior activity, greater co-contraction of lower leg muscles, increased ranges of COP sway, and no change in COP RMS (33). However, in the absence of a control group without VHI, it is unclear to what extent the postural changes reported in VHI differ from an otherwise normal manifestation of balance changes observed in healthy individuals standing under conditions of increased postural threat (Table 1).

The potential for fear and anxiety to influence balance is also important to account for when designing intervention studies that require longitudinal measures of balance-related performance in comparison to a baseline measure. Given known white-coat effects (78) and potential first trial effects (17) on balance, it is important that multiple baseline measures be recorded, and/or a control group incorporated to address potential order effects that may be mediated by adaptation of fear/anxiety with repeated exposure (77). Finally, it is important to recognize, and understand, how clinical balance treatments and interventions may be impacted by emotional-balance interactions (83).

Future Research Directions

Most studies that used a height manipulation to understand how threat affects normal balance excluded participants with height phobias for safety/ethical concerns; however, there is some reason to believe that a true fear response may have distinct balance changes compared to an anxious response. For example, individuals standing at extreme surface heights [over 9-m high, (22, 23)] are shown to have significant increases in amplitude of postural sway, in contrast to the reduced sway seen in most individuals at lower surface heights (up to 3.2-m high, see Table 1). However, Davis et al. (29) showed that a sub-group of their subjects, who reported a robust fear response to the moderate (3.2 m) surface height threat (>50% change from ground) had a significantly larger amplitude and frequency of COP displacements compared to anxious but less fearful (<50% change) subjects. Thus, more work needs to be done to determine how fear and anxiety may differentially impact unique aspects of balance control, and distinguish those from the context-specific changes which may or may not translate from a lab setting to daily-life situations experienced by those with a fear of falling. Future work also needs to continue to investigate the potential neurophysiological and attentional mechanisms that contribute to postural changes with threat. This includes probing how different sensori-motor systems respond to different threat conditions, and investigating whether specific instructions or tasks designed to shift attention from posture can modify the postural response to threat. Finally, there is a need for exploration of novel techniques such as virtual reality/augmented reality as a means to test and treat individuals with fear of falling, and develop more effective types of balance interventions that are designed to influence both the psychological and physiological aspects of a balance deficit.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to AA (#288164) and MC (#326910). The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Martin Zaback, Taylor Cleworth, and Kyle Johnson for their valuable assistance in preparing the manuscript and figures.

References

1. Legters K. Fear of falling. Phys Ther. (2002) 82:264–72. doi: 10.1093/ptj/82.3.264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Scheffer AC, Schuurmans MJ, Van Dijk N, Van Der Hooft T, De Rooij SE. Fear of falling: measurement strategy, prevalence, risk factors and consequences among older persons. Age Ageing (2008) 37:19–24. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afm169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bloem BR, Grimbergen YA, Cramer M, Willemsen M, Zwinderman AH. Prospective assessment of falls in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. (2001) 248:950–8. doi: 10.1007/s004150170047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Adkin AL, Frank JS, Jog MS. Fear of falling and postural control in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2003) 18:496–502. doi: 10.1002/mds.10396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Watanabe Y. Fear of falling among stroke survivors after discharge from inpatient rehabilitation. Int J Rehabil Res. (2005) 28:149–52. doi: 10.1097/00004356-200506000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Finlayson ML, Peterson EW, Cho CC. Risk factors for falling among people aged 45 to 90 years with multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehab. (2006) 87:1274–9. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Schmid AA, Van Puymbroeck M, Knies K, Spangler-Morris C, Watts K, Damush T, et al. Fear of falling among people who have sustained a stroke: a 6-month longitudinal pilot study. Am J Occup Ther. (2011) 65:125–32. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2011.000737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Schlick C, Schniepp R, Loidl V, Wuehr M, Hesselbarth K, Jahn K. Falls and fear of falling in vertigo and balance disorders: a controlled cross-sectional study. J Vestib Res. (2015) 25, 241–51. doi: 10.3233/VES-150564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Cumming RG, Salkeld G, Thomas M, Szonyi G. Prospective study of the impact of fear of falling on activities of daily living, SF-36 scores, and nursing home admission. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2000) 55:M299–305. doi: 10.1093/gerona/55.5.M299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Friedman SM, Munoz B, West SK, Rubin GS, Fried LP. Falls and fear of falling: which comes first? A longitudinal prediction model suggests strategies for primary and secondary prevention. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2002) 50:1329–35. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50352.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Maki BE, Holliday PJ, Topper AK. Fear of falling and postural performance in the elderly. J Gerontol. (1991) 46:M123–31. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.4.M123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Jacob RG, Furman JM, Durrant JD, Turner SM. Surface dependence: a balance control strategy in panic disorder with agoraphobia. Psychosom Med. (1997) 59:323–30. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199705000-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Redfern MS, Furman JM, Jacob RG. Visually induced postural sway in anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. (2007) 21:704–16. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Brown LA, Frank JS. Postural compensations to the potential consequences of instability: kinematics. Gait Posture (1997) 6:89–97. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(96)01106-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hogg S. A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1996) 54:21–30. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02126-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Carpenter MG, Frank JS, Silcher CP. Surface height effects on postural control: a hypothesis for a stiffness strategy for stance. J Vestib Res. (1999) 9:277–86.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

17. Adkin AL, Frank JS, Carpenter MG, Peysar GW. Postural control is scaled to level of postural threat. Gait Posture (2000) 12:87–93. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00057-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Carpenter MG, Frank JS, Silcher CP, Peysar GW. The influence of postural threat on the control of upright stance. Exp Brain Res. (2001) 138:210–8. doi: 10.1007/s002210100681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Winter DA, Patla AE, Prince F, Ishac M, Gielo-Perczak K. Stiffness control of balance in quiet standing. J Neurophysiol. (1998) 80:1211–21. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Winter DA. Human balance and posture control during standing and walking. Gait Posture (1995) 3:193–214. doi: 10.1016/0966-6362(96)82849-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lepicard EM, Venault P, Perez-Diaz F, Joubert C, Berthoz A, Chapouthier G. Balance control and posture differences in the anxious BALB/cByJ mice compared to the non- anxious C57BL/6J mice. Behav Brain Res. (2000) 117:185–95. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00304-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nakahara H, Takemori S, Tsuruoka H. Influence of height on the spatial orientation and equilibrium of the body. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2000) 123:501–4. doi: 10.1067/mhn.2000.107316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Simeonov P, Hsiao H. Height, surface firmness, and visual reference effects on balance control. Inj Prev. (2001) 7:i50–3. doi: 10.1136/ip.7.suppl_1.i50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Brown LA, Polych MA, Doan JB. The effect of anxiety on the regulation of upright standing among younger and older adults. Gait Posture (2006) 24:397–405. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.04.013.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Carpenter MG, Adkin AL, Brawley LR, Frank JS. Postural, physiological and psychological reactions to challenging balance: does age make a difference? Age Ageing (2006) 35:298–303. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Laufer Y, Barak Y, Chemel I. Age-related differences in the effect of a perceived threat to stability on postural control. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2006) 61:500–4. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.5.500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Alpers GW, Adolph D. Exposure to heights in a theme park: fear, dizziness, and body sway. J Anxiety Disord. (2008) 22:591–601. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hauck LJ, Carpenter MG, Frank JS. Task-specific measures of balance efficacy, anxiety, and stability and their relationship to clinical balance performance. Gait Posture (2008) 27:676–82. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Davis JR, Campbell AD, Adkin AL, Carpenter MG. The relationship between fear of falling and human postural control. Gait Posture (2009) 29:275–9. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Huffman JL, Horslen BC, Carpenter MG, Adkin AL. Does increased postural threat lead to more conscious control of posture? Gait Posture (2009) 30:528–32. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stins JF, Roerdink M, Beek PJ. To freeze or not to freeze? Affective and cognitive perturbations have markedly different effects on postural control. Hum Mov Sci. (2011) 30:190–202. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2010.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Cleworth TW, Horslen BC, Carpenter MG. Influence of real and virtual heights on standing balance. Gait Posture (2012) 36:172–6. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wuehr M, Kugler G, Schniepp R, Eckl M, Pradhan C, Jahn K, et al. (2014). Balance control and anti-gravity muscle activity during the experience of fear at heights. Physiol Rep. 2:e00232. doi: 10.1002/phy2.232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Zaback M, Cleworth TW, Carpenter MG, Adkin AL. Personality traits and individual differences predict threat-induced changes in postural control. Hum Mov Sci. (2015) 40:393–409. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cleworth TW, Carpenter MG. Postural threat influences conscious perception of postural sway. Neurosci Lett. (2016) 620:127–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sturnieks DL, Delbaere K, Brodie MA, Lord SR. The influence of age, anxiety and concern about falling on postural sway when standing at an elevated level. Hum Mov Sci. (2016) 49:206–15. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2016.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Young LR, Bernard-Demanze L, Dumitrescu M, Magnan J, Borel L, Lacour M. Postural performance of vestibular loss patients under increased postural threat. J Vestib Res. (2012) 22:129–38. doi: 10.3233/VES-2012-0449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Brown LA, Doan JB, Whishaw IQ, Suchowersky O. Parkinsonian deficits in context-dependent regulation of standing postural control. Neurosci Lett. (2007) 418:292–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.03.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Pasman EP, Murnaghan CD, Bloem BR, Carpenter MG. Balance problems with Parkinson's disease: are they anxiety-dependent? Neuroscience (2011) 177:283–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.12.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Adkin AL, Frank JS, Carpenter MG, Peysar GW. Fear of falling modifies anticipatory postural control. Exp Brain Res. (2002) 143:160–70. doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0974-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Yiou E, Deroche T, Do MC, Woodman T. Influence of fear of falling on anticipatory postural control of medio-lateral stability during rapid leg flexion. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2011) 111:611–20. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1680-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gendre M, Yiou E, Gélat T, Honeine J, Deroche T. Directional specificity of postural threat on anticipatory postural adjustments during lateral leg raising. Exp Brain Res. (2016) 234:659–71. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4471-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Carpenter MG, Frank JS, Adkin AL, Paton A, Allum JHJ. Influence of postural anxiety on postural reactions to multi-directional surface rotations. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 92:3255–65. doi: 10.1152/jn.00724.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Adkin AL, Campbell AD, Chua R, Carpenter MG. The influence of postural threat on the cortical response to unpredictable and predictable postural perturbations. Neurosci Lett. (2008) 435:120–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Brown LA, Gage WH, Polych MA, Sleik RJ, Winder TR. Central set influences on gait: Age-dependent effects of postural threat. Exp Brain Res. (2002) 145:286–96. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1082-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gage WH, Sleik RJ, Polych MA, McKenzie NC, Brown LA. The allocation of attention during locomotion is altered by anxiety. Exp Brain Res. (2003) 150:385–94. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1468-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. McKenzie NC, Brown LA. Obstacle negotiation kinematics: age-dependent effects of postural threat. Gait Posture (2004) 19:226–34. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(03)00060-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Brown LA, Doan JB, McKenzie NC, Cooper SA. Anxiety-mediated gait adaptations reduce errors of obstacle negotiation among younger and older adults: implications for fall risk. Gait Posture (2006) 24:418–23. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Delbaere K, Sturnieks DL, Crombez G, Lord SR. Concern about falls elicits changes in gait parameters in conditions of postural threat in older people. J Gerontol A Biomed Sci Med Sci. (2009) 64:237–42. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gln014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Caetano MJD, Gobbi LTB, del Rosario Sánchez-Arias M, Stella F, Gobbi S. Effects of postural threat on walking features of Parkinson's disease patients. Neurosci Lett. (2009) 452:136–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.01.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tersteeg MCA, Marple-Horvat DE, Loram ID. Cautious gait in relation to knowledge and vision of height: is altered visual information the dominant influence? J Neurophysiol. (2012) 107:2686–91. doi: 10.1152/jn.00875.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Schniepp R, Kugler G, Wuehr M, Eckl M, Huppert D, Huth S, et al. Quantification of gait changes in subjects with visual height intolerance when exposed to heights. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:963. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Hackney AL, Cinelli ME, Denomme LT, Frank JS. The effects of narrow and elevated path walking on aperture crossing. Hum Mov Sci. (2015) 41:295–306. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Sibley KM, Carpenter MG, Perry JC, Frank JS. Effects of postural anxiety on the soleus H-reflex. Hum Mov Sci. (2007) 26:103–12. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sibley KM, Mochizuki G, Frank JS, McIlroy WE. The relationship between physiological arousal and cortical and autonomic responses to postural instability. Exp Brain Res. (2010) 203:533–40. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2257-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Davis JR, Horslen BC, Nishikawa K, Fukushima K, Chua R, Inglis JT, et al. Human proprioceptive adaptations during states of height-induced fear and anxiety. J Neurophysiol. (2011) 106:3082–90. doi: 10.1152/jn.01030.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Horslen BC, Murnaghan CD, Inglis JT, Chua R, Carpenter MG. Effects of postural threat on spinal stretch reflexes: evidence for increased muscle spindle sensitivity? J Neurophysiol. (2013) 110:899–906. doi: 10.1152/jn.00065.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Osler CJ, Tersteeg MCA, Reynolds RF, Loram ID. Postural threat differentially affects the feedforward and feedback components of the vestibular-evoked balance response. Eur J Neurosci. (2013) 38:3239–47. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tanaka T, Matsugi A, Kamata N, Hiraoka K. Postural threat increases corticospinal excitability in the trunk flexor muscles in the upright stance. J Psychophysiol. (2013) 27:165. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Horslen BC, Dakin CJ, Inglis JT, Blouin JS, Carpenter MG. Modulation of human vestibular reflexes with increased postural threat. J Physiol. (2014) 592:3671–85. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.270744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Naranjo EN, Allum JHJ, Inglis JT, Carpenter MG. Increased gain of vestibulospinal potentials evoked in neck and leg muscles when standing under height-induced postural threat. Neuroscience (2015) 293:45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Naranjo EN, Cleworth TW, Allum JHJ, Inglis JT, Lea J, Westerberg BD, et al. Vestibulo-spinal and vestibulo-ocular reflexes are modulated when standing with increased postural threat. J Neurophysiol. (2015) 115:833–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.00626.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lim SB, Cleworth TW, Horslen BC, Blouin JS, Inglis JT, Carpenter MG. Postural threat influences vestibular-evoked muscular responses. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 117:604–11. doi: 10.1152/jn.00712.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Zaback M, Carpenter MG, Adkin AL. Threat-induced changes in attention during tests of static and anticipatory postural control. Gait Posture (2016) 45:19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Horslen BC, Inglis JT, Blouin JS, Carpenter MG. Both standing and postural threat decrease Achilles' tendon reflex inhibition from tendon electrical stimulation. J Physiol. (2017) 595:4493–506. doi: 10.1113/JP273935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Naranjo EN, Cleworth TW, Allum JHJ, Inglis JT, Lea J, Westerberg BD, et al. Threat effects on human oculo-motor function. Neuroscience (2017) 359:289–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Cleworth TW, Chua R, Inglis JT, Carpenter MG. Influence of virtual height exposure on postural reactions to support surface translations. Gait Posture (2016) 47:96–102. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Le Clair K, Riach C. Postural stability measures: what to measure and for how long. Clin Biomech. (1996) 11:176–8. doi: 10.1016/0268-0033(95)00027-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Carpenter MG, Frank JS, Winter DA, Peysar GW. Sampling duration effects on centre of pressure summary measures. Gait Posture (2001) 13:35–40. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00093-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. van der Kooij H, Campbell AD, Carpenter MG. Sampling duration effects on centre of pressure descriptive measures. Gait Posture (2011) 34:19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Brandt T, Bles W, Arnold F, Kapteyn TS. Height vertigo and human posture. Adv Otorhinolaryngol. (1979) 25:88–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

72. Bles W, Kapteyn TS, Brandt T, Arnold F. The mechanism of physiological height vertigo: II. Posturography Acta Otolaryngol. (1980) 89:534–40.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

73. Shaw JA, Stefanyk LE, Frank JS, Jog MS, Adkin AL. Effects of age and pathology on stance modifications in response to increased postural threat. Gait Posture (2012) 35:658–61. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Phanthanourak AL, Cleworth TW, Adkin AL, Carpenter MG, Tokuno CD. The threat of a support surface translation affects anticipatory postural control. Gait Posture (2016) 50:145–50. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Johnson KJ, Zaback M, Tokuno CD, Carpenter MG, Adkin AL. Exploring the relationship between threat-related changes in anxiety, attention focus, and postural control. Psychol Res. (2017). doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0940-0. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Johnson KJ, Zaback M, Tokuno CD, Carpenter MG, Adkin AL. Cognitive, emotional, and postural adaptations to repeated postural threat exposure. In: 12th Annual Canadian Neuroscience Meeting (2018). Available online at: https://can-acn.org/documents/2018/CAN2018-final-program.pdf

Google Scholar

77. Zaback M, Adkin AL, Carpenter MG. Repeated exposure to height-related postural threat: how do humans adapt? In: International Society of Posture and Gait Research World Congress (2017). Available online at: https://silkstart.s3.amazonaws.com/59415e0f68621305b714ddfc.pdf

Google Scholar

78. Geh CL, Beauchamp MR, Crocker PR, Carpenter MG. Assessed and distressed: white-coat effects on clinical balance performance. J Psychosom Res. (2011) 70:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Horslen BC, Carpenter MG. Arousal, valence and their relative effects on postural control. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 215:27. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2867-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Maki BE, Mcllroy WE. Influence of arousal and attention on the control of postural sway. J Vestib Res. (1996) 6:53–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

81. Doumas M, Morsanyi K, Young WR. Cognitively and socially induced stress affects postural control. Exp Brain Res. (2018) 236:305–14. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5128-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Young WR, Williams AM. How fear of falling can increase fall-risk in older adults: applying psychological theory to practical observations. Gait Posture (2015) 41:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Staab JP, Balaban CD, Furman JM. Threat assessment and locomotion: clinical applications of an integrated model of anxiety and postural control. Semin Neurol. (2013) 33:297–306. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1356462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Avanzino L, Lagravinese G, Abbruzzese G, Pelosin E. Relationships between gait and emotion in Parkinson's disease: A narrative review. Gait Posture (2018) 65:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.06.171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Balaban CD, Thayer JF. Neurological bases for balance-anxiety links. J Anxiety Disord. (2001) 15:53–79. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(00)00042-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Balaban CD. Neural substrates linking balance control and anxiety. Physiol Behav. (2002) 77:469–75. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00935-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Horslen BC, Zaback M, Inglis JT, Blouin JS, Carpenter MG. Static and Dynamic Soleus Stretch Reflex Responses in Standing Humans are Independently Modulated by Postural Threat. San Diego, CA: Society for Neuroscience (2016).

Google Scholar

88. Horslen B. C. Vestibular, Proprioceptive, and Cutaneous Reflex Modulation Explored Through a Height-Induced Postural Threat. Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia (2016). doi: 10.14288/1.0319265

CrossRef Full Text

89. Popkirov S, Staab JP, Stone J. Persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD): a common, characteristic and treatable cause of chronic dizziness. Pract Neurol. (2018) 18:5–13. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2017-001809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Wuehr M, Brandt T, Schniepp R. Distracting attention in phobic postural vertigo normalizes leg muscle activity and balance. Neurology (2016). 88:284–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Dieterich M, Staab JP. Functional dizziness: from phobic postural vertigo and chronic subjective dizziness to persistent postural-perceptual dizziness. Curr Opin Neurol. (2017) 30:107–13. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Holmberg J, Tjernstrom F, Karlberg M, Fransson PA, Magnusson M. Reduced postural differences between phobic postural vertigo patients and healthy subjects during a postural threat. J Neurol. (2009) 256:1258–62. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5110-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Huppert D, Grill E, Brandt T. Down on heights? One in three has visual height intolerance. J Neurol. (2013) 260:597–604. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6685-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: postural control, balance, emotions, fear, anxiety, threat, surface height

Citation: Adkin AL and Carpenter MG (2018) New Insights on Emotional Contributions to Human Postural Control. Front. Neurol. 9:789. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00789

Received: 29 June 2018; Accepted: 31 August 2018;
Published: 21 September 2018.

Edited by:

Emily Keshner, Temple University, United States

Reviewed by:

William Richard Young, Brunel University London, United Kingdom
Laura Avanzino, Università di Genova, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Adkin and Carpenter. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Allan L. Adkin, aadkin@brocku.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.