Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 16 November 2018
Sec. Neuro-Otology
This article is part of the Research Topic Role of Inner Ear in Self and Environment Perception View all 18 articles

Effect of Rotating Auditory Scene on Postural Control in Normal Subjects, Patients With Bilateral Vestibulopathy, Unilateral, or Bilateral Cochlear Implants

\r\nCaroline Guigou,Caroline Guigou1,2Michel Toupet,Michel Toupet1,3Benoit Delemps,Benoit Delemps1,4Sylvie HeuschenSylvie Heuschen4Serge AhoSerge Aho5Alexis Bozorg Grayeli,*Alexis Bozorg Grayeli1,2*
  • 1Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Dijon University Hospital, Dijon, France
  • 2Le2i Research Laboratory, CNRS, UMR-6306, Dijon, France
  • 3Centre d'Explorations Fonctionnelles Otoneurologiques, Paris, France
  • 4Audika Auditory Rehabilitation Center, Dijon, France
  • 5Department of Epidemiology, Dijon University Hospital, Dijon, France

Objective: The aim of this study was to investigate the impact of a rotating sound stimulation on the postural performances in normal subjects, patients with bilateral vestibulopathy (BVP), unilateral (UCI), and bilateral (BCI) cochlear implantees.

Materials and Methods: Sixty-nine adults were included (32 women and 37 men) in a multicenter prospective study. The group included 37 healthy subjects, 10 BVP, 15 UCI, and 7 BCI patients. The average of age was 47 ± 2.0 (range: 23–82). In addition to a complete audiovestibular work up, a dynamic posturography (Multitest Framiral, Grasse) was conducted in silence and with a rotating cocktail party sound delivered by headphone. The center of pressure excursion surface (COPS), sensory preferences, as well as fractal, diffusion, and wavelet analysis of stabilometry were collected.

Results: The rotating sound seemed to influenced balance in all subgroups except in controls. COPS increased with sound in the BVP and BCI groups in closed eyes and sway-referenced condition indicating a destabilizing effect while it decreased in UCI in the same condition suggesting stabilization (p < 0.05, linear mixed model corrected for age, n = 69). BVP had higher proprioceptive preferences, BCI had higher vestibular and visual preferences, and UCI had only higher vestibular preferences than controls. Sensory preferences were not altered by rotating sound.

Conclusions: The rotating sound destabilized BVP and BCI patients with binaural hearing while it stabilized UCI patients with monaural hearing and no sound rotation effect. This difference suggests that binaural auditory cues are exploited in BCI patients for their balance.

Introduction

The interaction between binaural hearing and space representation has been discussed for several decades. In 1960, Hennebert studied the clinical effects of a rotating sound in healthy subjects and observed a provoked nystagmus which he called “audiokinetic.” This nystagmus had a slow phase parallel to the source movement. Other reported effects were a deviation of the upper limbs during the Romberg test, deviation of vertical writing toward the sound and also neurovegetative reactions (1).

Bats are probably the most performant animals in using their hearing for the representation of their environment (2). These animals can separate auditory cues related to echolocation from those used for communication during their flight (2) and echolocation seems to be quite efficient since it does not add large energetic costs to the aerodynamic power requirements of their flight (3). After several months of training, many blind humans can also develop echolocation using tongue clicks. They use this capacity in daily life to avoid obstacles and to obtain information on the form and the size of surrounding objects (4). Echolocation is a dynamic process and uses the head-related transfer function (46). This ability requires stereophony with two equivalently performant ears (7). In blind experts, a separate processing of auditory source-motion and echo-motion in temporal-occipital cortex is observed and fMRI data suggest central reorganization with a possible recruitment of visual cortex (8).

Other observations on postural behavior of normal subjects submitted to static or mobile sound sources support the idea that hearing afferences could have an impact on the gait when other afferences are destroyed or ineffective (911). Studies in elderly patients are in line with those in experimental conditions on the role of auditory input in the balance by showing that hearing aids enhance the gait during Romberg test, decrease the risk of falls (12) and improve many aspects of quality of life related to balance (13).

In patients with a cochlear implant (CI), the effect of sound on balance has been rarely reported (14). Many bilaterally deaf patients still receive CI on one side due to its cost (15). Today, based on proven benefits of binaural hearing on global auditory performances, bilateral CI (BCI) is proposed more frequently in developed countries (16). However, BCI does not provide stereophony in all patients due to asymmetries of auditory nerve function, and to the sound coding strategies which reduce or suppress binaural time cues (17). Although the effect of BCI on sound localization has been reported (17, 18), the influence of binaural cues provided by CI on balance performances has not been investigated to our knowledge. It should also be underlined that CI has a potential impact on vestibular integrity and function. Histological lesions in the saccule and semicircular canals as a consequence of CI have been described (19). However, the functional consequences in unilateral cochlear implantees (UCI) appear to be mild (20).

Recent studies on sound-gait interaction provide different and sometimes contradicting results, but they all suggest an effect of the sound on balance performances (2127). The contradictions are probably related to the experimental protocol, the characteristics of the subjects, the measured parameters and the fact that balance is a dynamic process which uses different sensory inputs changing in hierarchy depending on patients and on situations (28).

In order to better understand the interaction between hearing and vestibular functions, especially in patients with CI, we aimed at investigating the effect of moving sound sources on balance performances by dynamic posturography in healthy subjects, in patients with bilateral vestibulopathy (BVP), and also in UCI and BCI.

Materials and Methods

Population

Sixty-nine subjects were included in this prospective and multicenter study from September 2015 to February 2016. The population included 37 healthy volunteers, 10 BVP, 15 UCI, and 7 BCI. The group was composed of 32 women and 37 men. The mean age was 47 ± 2.0 years [range: 23–82] in the general population, 38 ± 2.1[23–66] for controls, 63 ± 2.4 [50–74] for BVP, 53 ± 3.7[23–71] for UCI, and 57 ± 9.2[25–82] for BCI. Controls were younger than other subgroups (p < 0.05, ANOVA followed by Dunett).

The study was conducted in two tertiary referral centers for balance disorders. The study received the approval of the local ethical research committee (CPP Est III) and from the French National Agency of Safety for Medicine and Health Products (number 2015-A00754-45). An informed consent was signed by all patients. All patients (but not controls) underwent a vestibular assessment with caloric and rotatory tests, videonystagmography analysis of gaze, pursuit, and saccade, and finally cervical vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMP). Eye movement analysis did not show signs of central involvement in this population.

BVP was defined according to the Barany Society criteria: the horizontal angular vestibulo ocular reflex (VOR) gain on both sides <0.6 (angular velocity 150–300°/s) and/or the sum of the maximal peak velocities of the slow phase caloric-induced nystagmus for stimulation with warm and cold water on each side <6°/s and/or the horizontal angular VOR gain <0.1 upon sinusoidal stimulation on a rotatory chair (0.1 Hz, Vmax = 50°/s) and/or a phase lead >68 degrees with a time constant of <5 s (29). All BVP subjects had a hearing test to confirm their normal hearing. Control subjects were asymptomatic and not tested for hearing.

In UCI group, nine patients (60%) had a normal caloric test, five (33%) had a deficit in the same side as the implanted ear, and one (7%) had a bilateral deficit. Eleven (73%) UCI had bilateral normal responses to cVEMPs, four (27%) had a deficit the same side as the implanted ear.

In BCI group, caloric stimulations were obtained in five cases. There was a bilateral deficit in three cases (60%), a unilateral deficit in one case (20%), and a normal caloric test for one patient. Four (57%) BCI had bilateral normal responses to cVEMPs, two (29%) patients had a unilateral right deficit, and one (14%) had a bilateral deficit. No UCI or BCI patient corresponded to the criteria of BVP.

In the UCI group, eight patients were implanted on the right side and seven UCI on left side. In this group, 13 patients were implanted with Digisonic SP implant and Zebra Processor (Oticon Medical Inc., Vallauris, France), one patient with Hi-Res 90K implant and Naida processor (Advanced Bionics, Valencia, CA), and one with Nucleus Freedom implant and processor (Cochlear Inc., Lane Cove, Australia). Three patients had a hearing aid on the contralateral ear. UCI were implanted 6 ± 1.2 years before the inclusion. The pure-tone average threshold of the implanted ear was 39 ± 2.0 dB HL (n = 15). The aided pure-tone average threshold of the contralateral ear was 95 ± 6.2 dB HL (n = 15).

BCI were implanted 4 ± 1.3 years before test tests for the right ears and 2.8 ± 1.06 years for the left ears. The pure-tone average threshold was 43 ± 5.2 dB HL on the right and 44 ± 3.2 dB HL on the left. In this group, six patients were bilaterally implanted with Digisonic SP implant and Zebra Processor (Oticon Medical Inc., Vallauris, France), and one with Advanced Bionics Hi-Res 90K implant and Naida processor (Advanced Bionics, Valencia, CA).

All patients were evaluated on a dynamic posturography platform. Each test was conducted in silence, with a clockwise and counterclockwise rotating sound.

Dynamic Posturography

The tests were performed on a conventional posturography plate bearing three pressure sensors (Balance Quest, Micromedical Technologies, Chatham, IL). The sampling rate was set at 50 Hz. The confidence ellipse surface containing 90% of all center of pressure positions was recorder and referred to as center of pressure excursion surface (COPS in mm2, Figure 1). A value of 300 was assigned to the test in case of fall. Sensory preferences and visual dependency indexes were calculated based on COPS measurements in different posturography conditions as follows (30): visual preference (eyes open, stable platform/eyes open, sway-referenced platform), vestibular preference (eyes open, stable/eyes closed, sway-referenced platform), proprioceptive preference (eyes open, stable/eyes closed, stable), visual dependency index (optokinetic stimulation, stable + optikenitic stimulation, sway-referenced)/(eyes closed, stable + eyes closed). Each condition was measured for 30 s and a 15-s break separated each condition.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Center of pressure excursion surface (COPS). Ellipse containing 90% of all center of pressure positions (mm2) in six conditions on dynamic posturography for a control subject and a bilateral cochlear implant patient. The line shows the movement of pressure positions during the 30 s. acquisition and “FALL” indicates patient falls during the test.

In the two most unstable conditions (eyes closed sway-referenced, EC-SR and optikenitic stimulation, sway-referenced, OK-SR), wavelet and diffusion analyses were conducted: the energy consumption was measured in two axes (mediolateral: X and anterior-posterior: Y) in three frequency bands 0.05–0.5, 0.5–1.5, and 1.5–10 Hz by wavelet analysis. Based on this measure, a postural stability index (PSI) representing the total time during which no energy consumption was measured, and a postural control index (PCI) representing the total time with postural activity were automatically calculated by the software in the three frequency bands (Posturopro ® Software, Inserm, Marseille, France). A postural instability index (PII) deduced from the two latter parameters was studied (PII = PSI/PCI). The diffusion analysis estimated the extent of oscillation around an equilibrium state and its break-point by two additional parameters: the critical time (CT, in s) and the critical amplitude (CA, in mm2). Moreover, a Fractal analysis was conducted and the result was presented as the proportion of Hausdorff points (n/N) representing the percentage of stochastic position points among all sampled positions.

Subjects were stimulated by a rotating sound on the dynamic posturography platform in four trials (clockwise rotating sound, silence, anti-clockwise rotating sound, silence). The stimulus consisted of a rotating Cocktail party sound at 189°/s horizontally around the subject. The sound was delivered at 75 dB by a headphone (HD 205, Sennheiser, Wedemark, Germany) to mask the noise produced by the posturography platform. The rotating sound effect was created by CSoundQT® 3.1 Software (Pelican, Gumby Framework, New Haven, CT) based on head-relative transfer function.

In silence, control and BVP subjects were tested in a quiet room with the headphone off placed on ears. For UCI and BCI patients, the external processors and the hearing aids were removed.

Statistics

Values were expressed as mean ± SD. Linear mixed models were used to access relationship between sound and gait of different subgroups. As the age could be a significant factor for the performances on the dynamic posturography, the model was corrected for age. A robust estimator of variance was used (31). Statistical tests were conducted on Stata (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX).

Results

Posturography

Center of Pressure Excursion Surfaces and Sensory Organization Test

As expected, patients with bilateral vestibular loss had larger COPS than controls in eyes closed or optokinetic and sway-referenced conditions (Table 1, p < 0.05, Linear mixed models, n = 69). Interestingly, UCI and BCI had greater excursion surfaces than control subjects not only in sway-referenced conditions but also in eyes closed and stable platform condition (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Center of Pressure (COP) excursion surfaces as a function of posturography conditions in silence and rotating sound.

Rotating sound seemed to influence COPS differently in patients with bilateral vestibular loss and in those with UCI:COPS increased with sound in the bilateral vestibular loss group (n = 10) in EC-SR condition, indicating a destabilizing effect, while it decreased in UCI in the same environment suggesting stabilization (n = 15, p < 0.05, linear mixed model corrected for age, n = 69, Table 1).

In the optokinetic and sway-referenced condition, BCI had also larger COPS with rotating sounds (CW and CCW) than in silence suggesting a destabilizing effect (p < 0.05, linear mixed model corrected for age, n = 69, Table 1) while other groups did not seem to be influenced in this condition.

SOT in silence revealed a lower proprioceptive preference index in subjects with bilateral vestibular loss than in controls (Table 2). In contrast, UCI patients had a lower vestibular preference index than controls. In BCI subjects, both vestibular and visual preference indexes were lower than controls (Table 2, linear mixed models corrected for age, n = 69). Sound did not seem to influence sensory preferences (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sensory preference index based on center of pressure excursion surfaces in silence and rotating sound.

Although CIs had a COPS similar to controls in the easy condition (eyes open, stable platform), they showed greater COPS in difficult conditions (closed eyes, sway referenced), together with a reduced vestibular preference. This suggested a compensated vestibular deficit in accordance with caloric and otolithic tests.

Wavelet, Diffusion, and Fractal Analysis of Stabilometry

In silence and in EC-SR condition, PII was higher in UCI than in control suggesting more instability (Table 3, p < 0.05, Linear mixed models, n = 69). This difference was not observed in OK-SR condition. Other groups of patients had similar PII to the control in EC-SR and OK-SR conditions (Table 3, p < 0.05, linear mixed models corrected for age, n = 69). Sound did not seem to influence PII (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Postural instability index.

Diffusion analysis revealed higher CAs in BVP, UCI, and BCI patients than control in EC-SR and OK-SR conditions regardless of sound conditions (p < 0.05, linear mixed models corrected for age, N = 69, Table 4). Critical time did not differ between groups (linear mixed models corrected for age, n = 69, Table 5).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Critical time.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Critical amplitude.

Fractal analysis showed lower proportion of Hausdorff points in UCI and BCI subjects than in controls in Y (roll) axis in EC-SR and OK-SR conditions (p < 0.05, linear mixed models corrected for age, n = 69, Table 6). Sound did not seem to influence the proportion of Hausdorff points in the Y axis in any subgroup (Table 6). No difference between subgroups or effect of sound could be observed in the X axis (pitch, data not shown).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Proportion of Hausdorff points in Y (Roll) axis.

Discussion

In this study, we showed that the hearing afferences could have an impact on the gait especially when other sensory inputs are impaired. As expected, patients with BVP, UCI, and BCI had poorer posturography performances than the control and their sensory organization was altered. Interestingly, the rotating sound reduced the COPS in patients with UCI and no stereophony but increased COPS in patients with BVP enjoying stereophony and BCI with binaural hearing in sway-referenced conditions and disturbed visual input. In contrast, control subjects did not modify their COPS under the sound effect suggesting a different hierarchy of sensory inputs in these individuals. The destabilizing effect of the rotating sound in BVL and BCI patients could be enhanced by the vestibular deficit in these subjects. This deficit might have modified the hierarchy of sensory inputs for balance.

Two limitations of our study were that the mean age of the control population was lower than other subgroups, and that although control subjects were totally asymptomatic, they were not explored by audiometry. We corrected the effect of age in our multivariate analysis, but these two aspects might limit the comparison between subgroups.

Several studies have already reported the effect of the sound on the gait or vestibular function with contradicting results: Some studies reported higher stability (9, 1113, 32), while others described poorer balance performances (10, 21, 22). The apparent contradiction could lie in the nature of the stimulus: stable vs. moving source.

Stevens et al. (32) tested the impact of the sound on the gait by dynamic posturography in 12 control and six patients with neurological diseases. They delivered a stable white noise via four earth-referenced speakers placed around the subject during the six conditions of posturography and showed that this type of stimuli decreased the COPS in both patients and controls. By comparing head-fixed stationary sound to silence, the authors did not find an improvement of the gait and concluded that the effect of an earth-fixed sound is probably based on localization cues more than on alertness. This observation was in accordance with a previous study in which stationary music delivered by a headphone did not influence the gait during posturography (26). Moreover, the stabilizing effect of earth-fixed stationary sounds were in accordance with other studies in healthy subjects evaluating gait by the Fukuda stepping test (9) and Romberg test (12).

In addition to providing cues on the distance and orientation of the body relative to earth-referenced sound sources, recent reports suggest that stable sound sources may interact with the visual input (33). Indeed, directing the gaze toward the source without moving the head significantly enhances the detection of interaural time and intensity differences (33). This observation indicates a visual and auditory interaction possibly at the brainstem level which could also benefit to the gait.

The effect of moving sound fields has also been investigated by stabilometry in healthy subjects but with contradictory results. While some authors report that moving sounds appear to increase sway (1, 21, 22), others observe a stabilizing effect of rotatory sounds and argue that contrary to moving visual cues, mobile sound sources are easier to identify and consequently more valuable in a multisensory processing of the balance (27).

The type of sound used in the experimental protocol might also explain the discrepancies between the reports on this subject (1, 10, 34). In 1960, Hennebert stated that rotating continuous pure tone elicited little vestibular response (nystagmus, Romberg test) while discontinuous pure tones with regular interruptions at a 3–5 Hz frequency have a higher impact. Similarly, white noise or complex sounds such as music yielded better responses (1). In this report, no quantification of the effects and quantitative comparison was provided. To our knowledge, no other study has compared the effect different sounds on the gait. For our subjects, we chose a cocktail party noise in order to be realistic and close to daily-life situations.

In our study, the rotating sound algorithm provided an impression of source displacement to patients with binaural hearing (BVP and BCI) but appeared as a sound oscillating in amplitude in the implanted ear of UCI patients. Based on previous reports, this could explain the destabilizing effect of the rotating sound in BVP and BCI subjects in contrast to the stabilizing impact on the UCI patients. Our sound stimuli did not disturb the healthy subjects even in conditions where the visual and proprioceptive inputs were hampered. This observation suggests that hearing afferences have a more prominent effect on the posture when other afferences (especially vestibular) are damaged.

The impact of CI on the vestibular system is a concern among otologists, mainly because the surgical trauma of the implantation in an already fragile inner ear may partly destroy the vestibule. This possibility influences the rehabilitation strategy especially in case of bilateral implantation (35). However, few studies have focused on the potential advantage of auditory input on the gait as a result of a richer multisensory input (14, 36, 37). While the effect of an active CI is undetectable by dynamic posturography if the patient is not stimulated by sound (37), patients seem to performed better with their CI on and an earth-referenced white noise in dark in comparison to the same situation with deactivated CI (14). Similarly, patients with hearing aids appear to benefit from the enhanced auditory input for their balance performance in the presence of a stationary sound (36).

Observations on dynamic posturography are also in accordance with several publications reporting an increase in risk of falls beginning in mild hearing losses and proportional to its severity: 1.4 X for every 10 dB loss in senior population (12, 13, 3840). Additionally, the idea that auditory input can be readily integrated in the multisensory gait control is also supported by the observation that translating hip and trunk movement into sound at delivering it to the subjects through headphone (auditory biofeedback) enhanced postural control in both BVP and healthy subjects (11).

Wavelet, diffusion, and fractal analysis of stabilometry in dynamic posturography have already been reported as meaningful indicators of balance performance and efficiency (4143). More than instability, they are indicators of energy consumption and balance control strategy: strict correction of COP displacements requiring more energy vs. a more tolerant strategy (44). Wavelet and diffusion analysis, confirmed deductions from COPS showing lower balance performances in patients with CI and in BVP compared to controls. However, they did not indicate a significant modification of energy consumption or balance strategy under the rotating sound effect. This could be explained by large interindividual variations (especially for the CA) and the fact that dynamic posturography evaluates the balance in a standing position and not during movements (walking). Indeed, walking is probably a more ecological manner to evaluate the multisensory integration in balance (45).

Interaction between auditory and vestibular information seems to take place at the cortical level. The temporo–parietal junction, connecting the auditory, somatosensory, and visual cortices is involved in a multimodal representation of space. It occupies the posterior portion of the superior temporal plane and the superior temporal gyrus. It contains trimodal neurons with receptive fields over the head–neck–shoulder region potentially involved in head orientation (46). A recent fMRI investigation suggests that superior temporal gyrus (planum temporale) and the posterior insula are particularly involved in the processing of both auditory and vestibular information (47). Sound–movement interaction may also be processed at a subcortical level. Recent studies on the influence of gaze direction on the auditory spatial resolution suggest a multisensory integration in the brainstem, presumably in the superior colliculus (32).

In conclusion, rotating sound influences the gait and alters the balance strategy in patients with CI and in BVP. While it destabilizes patients with binaural hearing (BCI and BVP), it seems to stabilize those with monaural hearing (UCI). These observations indicate the integration of binaural auditory cues for the balance control in patients with BCI.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Author Contributions

AB, MT, and CG designed the study. BD, CG, MT, and AB developed the experimental setup. CG, BD, and SH evaluated the subjects. CG, SA, and AB analyzed the data and prepared the manuscript.

Funding

This work was financially supported by Oticon Medical (Vallauris, France).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Hennebert PE. Nystagmus audiocinétique. Acta Oto Laryngol. (1960) 51:412–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. Liu Y, Feng J, Metzner W. Different auditory feedback control for echolocation and communication in horseshoe bats. PLoS ONE (2013) 8:e62710. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Voigt CC, Lewanzik D. 'No cost of echolocation for flying bats' revisited. J Comp Physiol B (2012) 182:831–40. doi: 10.1007/s00360-012-0663-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Wallmeier L, Wiegrebe L. Self-motion facilitates echo-acoustic orientation in humans. R Soc Open Sci. (2014) 1:140185. doi: 10.1098/rsos.140185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kondo HM, Pressnitzer D, Toshima I, Kashino M. Effects of self-motion on auditory scene analysis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2012) 109:6775–80. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112852109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Guarato F, Windmill J, Gachagan A. A beam-based method for target localization: inspiration from bats' directivity and binaural reception for ultrasonic sonar. J Acoust Soc Am. (2013) 133:4077–86. doi: 10.1121/1.4802829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Nilsson ME, Schenkman BN. Blind people are more sensitive than sighted people to binaural sound-location cues, particularly inter-aural level differences. Hear Res. (2016) 332:223–32. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Thaler L, Milne JL, Arnott SR, Kish D, Goodale MA. Neural correlates of motion processing through echolocation, source hearing, and vision in blind echolocation experts and sighted echolocation novices. J Neurophysiol. (2014) 111:112–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.00501.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Munnings A, Chisnall B, Oji S, Whittaker M, Kanegaonkar R. Environmental factors that affect the Fukuda stepping test in normal participants. J Laryngol Otol. (2015) 129:450–3. doi: 10.1017/S0022215115000560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Raper SA, Soames RW. The influence of stationary auditory fields on postural sway behaviour in man. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. (1991) 63:363–7. doi: 10.1007/BF00364463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dozza M, Horak FB, Chiari L. Auditory biofeedback substitutes for loss of sensory information in maintaining stance. Exp Brain Res. (2007) 178:37–48. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0709-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rumalla K, Karim AM, Hullar TE. The effect of hearing aids on postural stability. Laryngoscope (2015) 125:720–3. doi: 10.1002/lary.24974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Lacerda CF, Silva LO, de Tavares Canto RS, Cheik NC. Effects of hearing aids in the balance, quality of life and fear to fall in elderly people with sensorineural hearing loss. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol. (2012) 16:156–62. doi: 10.7162/2FS18097772012000200002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Shayman CS, Mancini M, Weaver TS, King LA, Hullar TE. The contribution of cochlear implants to postural stability. Laryngoscope (2018) 128:1676–80. doi: 10.1002/lary.26994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lammers MJ, Grolman W, Smulders YE, Rovers MM. The cost-utility of bilateral cochlear implantation: a systematic review. Laryngoscope (2011) 121:2604–9. doi: 10.1002/lary.22387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Cullington H, Bele D, Brinton J, Lutman M. United Kingdom national paediatric bilateral cochlear implant audit: preliminary results. Cochlear Implants Int. (2013)14:S22–6. doi: 10.1179/1467010013Z.000000000128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Francart T, Lenssen A, Büchner A, Lenarz T, Wouters J. Effect of channel envelope synchrony on interaural time difference sensitivity in bilateral cochlear implant listeners. Ear Hear. (2015) 36:e199–206. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Churchill TH, Kan A, Goupell MJ, Litovsky RY. Spatial hearing benefits demonstrated with presentation of acoustic temporal fine structure cues in bilateral cochlear implant listeners. J Acoust Soc Am. (2014) 136:1246. doi: 10.1121/1.4892764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Tien HC, Linthicum FH. Histopathologic changes in the vestibule after cochlear implantation. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2002) 127:260–64. doi: 10.1067/mhn.2002.128555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Buchman CA, Joy J, Hodges A, Telischi FF, Balkany TJ. Vestibular effects of cochlear implantation. Laryngoscope (2004) 114:1–22. doi: 10.1097/00005537-200410001-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Soames RW, Raper SA. The influence of moving auditory fields on postural sway behaviour in man. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. (1992) 65:241–5.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Tanaka T, Kojima S, Takeda H, Ino S, Ifukube T. The influence of moving auditory stimuli on standing balance in healthy young adults and the elderly. Ergonomics (2001) 44:1403–12. doi: 10.1080/00140130110110601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Harris CS. Effects of increasing intensity levels of intermittent and continuous 1000 –Hz tones on human equilibrium. Perceptual Motor Skills (1972) 35:395–405.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. Njiokiktjien C. The influence of an auditory task on romberg's test in healthy people and neurological patients. Agressologie (1973) 14:11–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

25. Bensel CK, Dzendolet E, Meiselman HL. Body sway during long-term standing and as affected by pure tones. Percept Psychophys. (1968) 4:171–4.

Google Scholar

26. Palm HG, Strobel J, Achatz G, von Luebken F, Friemert B. The role and interaction of visual and auditory afferents in postural stability. Gait Posture (2009) 30:328–33. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gandemer L, Parseihian G, Kronland-Martinet R, Bourdin C. The influence of horizontally rotating sound on standing balance. Exp Brain Res. (2014) 232:3813–20. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4066-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mergner T. Modeling sensorimotor control of human upright stance. Prog Brain Res. (2007)165:283–97. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)65018-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Strupp M, Kim JS, Murofushi T, Straumann D, Jen JC, Rosengren SM, Della Santina CC, et al. Bilateral vestibulopathy: diagnostic criteria consensus document of the classification committee of the Bárány society. J Vestib Res. (2017) 27:177–89. doi: 10.3233/VES-170619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Nashner LM, Peters JF. Dynamic posturography in the diagnosis and management of dizziness and balance disorders. Neurol Clin. (1990) 8:331–49.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

31. Vach W. Regression Models as a Tool in Medical Research. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (2013). p. 496.

Google Scholar

32. Stevens MN, Barbour DL, Gronski MP, Hullar TE. Auditory contributions to maintaining balance. J Vestib Res. (2016) 26:433–38. doi: 10.3233/VES-160599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Maddox RK, Pospisil DA, Stecker GC, Lee AK. Directing eye gaze enhances auditory spatial cue discrimination. Curr Biol. (2014) 24:748–52. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Süss KJ, Schaefer KP, Neetz A. Moving sound sources: techniques and applications. Acta Otolaryngol. (1982) 94:29–35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

35. Janky KL, Givens D. Vestibular, visual acuity, and balance outcomes in children with cochlear implants: a preliminary report. Ear Hear. (2015) 36:e364–72. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Weaver TS, Shayman CS, Hullar TE. The effect of hearing aids and cochlear implants on balance during gait. Otol Neurotol. (2017) 38:1327–32. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Shayman CS, Earhart GM, Hullar TE. Improvements in gait with hearing aids and cochlear implants. Otol Neurotol. (2017) 38:484–86. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Lin FR, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and falls among older adults in the United States. Arch Intern Med. (2012) 172:369–71. doi: 10.1001/2Farchinternmed.2011.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Agmon M, Lavie L, Doumas M. The Association between hearing loss, postural control, and mobility in older adults: a systematic review. J Am Acad Audiol. (2017) 28:575–88. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.16044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Campos J, Ramkhalawansingh R, Pichora-Fuller MK. Hearing, self-motion perception, mobility, and aging. Hear Res. (2018) 369:42–55. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2018.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lorin P, Manceau C, Foubert F. Effect of vestibular neuritis on postural control using wavelets and fractal analysis. Rev Laryngol Otol Rhinol. (2010) 131:235–41.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

42. Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Lê TT, Vernet M, Berbey N, Orssaud C, et al. Medio-lateral postural instability in subjects with tinnitus. Front Neurol. (2011) 27:35. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2011.00035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Cimolin V, Galli M, Rigoldi C, Grugni G, Vismara L, Mainardi L, et al. Fractal dimension approach in postural control of subjects with Prader-Willi Syndrome. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2011) 8:45. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-8-45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Paillard T, Costes-Salon C, Lafont C, Dupui P. Are there differences in postural regulation according to the level of competition in judoists? Br J Sports Med. (2002) 36:304–5. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.36.4.304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Tournier I, Dommes A, Cavallo V. Review of safety and mobility issues among older pedestrians. Accid Anal Prev. (2016) 91:24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.02.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Leinonen L, Hyvärinen J, Sovijärvi ARA. Functional properties of neurons in the temporo-parietal association cortex of awake monkey. Exp Brain Res. (1980) 39:203–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

47. Oh SY, Boegle R, Ertl M, Stephan T, Dieterich M. Multisensory vestibular, vestibular-auditory, and auditory network effects revealed by parametric sound pressure stimulation. Neuroimage (2018) 176:354–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: binaural hearing, stereophony, balance, bilateral vestibulopathy, posturography, multisensory integration

Citation: Guigou C, Toupet M, Delemps B, Heuschen S, Aho S and Bozorg Grayeli A (2018) Effect of Rotating Auditory Scene on Postural Control in Normal Subjects, Patients With Bilateral Vestibulopathy, Unilateral, or Bilateral Cochlear Implants. Front. Neurol. 9:972. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00972

Received: 10 September 2018; Accepted: 29 October 2018;
Published: 16 November 2018.

Edited by:

Yuri Agrawal, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Reviewed by:

Eduardo Martin-Sanz, Hospital de Getafe, Spain
Juan Carlos Amor-Dorado, Hospital Can Misses, Spain

Copyright © 2018 Guigou, Toupet, Delemps, Heuschen, Aho and Bozorg Grayeli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexis Bozorg Grayeli, alexis.bozorggrayeli@chu-dijon.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.