Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 22 May 2019
Sec. Movement Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Movement Disorders Editor's Pick 2021 View all 10 articles

Pre-frontal Cortical Activity During Walking and Turning Is Reliable and Differentiates Across Young, Older Adults and People With Parkinson's Disease

  • 1Department of Neurology, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 2Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Università degli Studi di Roma Foro Italico, Rome, Italy

Introduction: Mobility declines with age and further with neurodegenerative disorders, such as Parkinson's disease (PD). Walking and turning ability, in particular, are vital aspects of mobility that deteriorate with age and are further impaired in PD. Such deficits have been linked with reduction in automatic control of movement and the need for compensatory cognitive cortical control via the pre-frontal cortex (PFC), however the underlying neural mechanisms remain unclear. Establishing and using a robust methodology to examine PFC activity during continuous walking and turning via mobile functional near infra-red spectroscopy (fNIRS) may aid in the understanding of mobility deficits and help with development of appropriate therapeutics.

This study aimed to: (1) examine test re-test reliability of PFC activity during continuous turning and walking via fNIRS measurement; and (2) compare PFC activity during continuous turning and walking in young, old and Parkinson's subjects.

Methods: Twenty-five young (32.3 ± 7.5 years), nineteen older (65.4 ± 7.0 years), and twenty-four PD (69.3 ± 4.1 years) participants performed continuous walking and 360° turning-in-place tasks, each lasting 2 min. Young participants repeated the tasks a second time to allow fNIRS measurement reliability assessment. The primary outcome was PFC activity, assessed via measuring changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) concentrations.

Results: PFC activity during continuous walking and turning was moderately reproducible (Intra-class correlation coefficient = 0.67). The PD group had higher PFC activation than young and older adults during walking and turning, with significant group differences for bilateral PFC activation (p = 0.025), left PFC activation (p = 0.012), and the early period (first 40 s) of walking (p = 0.007), with greater activation required in PD. Interestingly, older adults had similar PFC activation to young adults across conditions, however older adults required greater activation than young adults during continuous turning, specifically the early period of the turning task (Cohens d = 0.86).

Conclusions: PFC activity can be measured during continuous walking and turning tasks with acceptable reliability, and can differentiate young, older and PD groups. PFC activation was significantly greater in PD compared to young and older adults during walking, particularly when beginning to walk.

Introduction

Decline in mobility occurs with age (13), and gait and turning impairments, which are central to reduction in independence, with links to increased falls risk (46), occur early in Parkinson's disease (PD) (79). Previous studies have shown that gait and turning are slower in PD, with shorter steps during walking and a greater number of steps required during turns (1012). Turns during walking have been found to be impaired in PD (13, 14), and continuous 360° turning-in-place is particularly complex for people with PD and elicits intermittent mobility issues (15), such as freezing of gait (11, 16). While the motor contributions of mobility decline in older adults and people with PD have been well-studied through static imaging assessments (1), considerable evidence is building for non-motor contributions, such as the role of cognition (17, 18). Deficits in cognition can occur with age and are common in PD, with early impairment of executive function, visuo-spatial ability, working memory, and attention (19). Executive-attention may be an important contributor to gait and turning control in older adults and more so in PD, with associative and dual-task studies highlighting a strong association between them (2025). Executive-attentional projections stem from the pre-frontal cortex (PFC), and may become over-active during gait or turning in PD compared to healthy controls (26, 27) to compensate for the impaired basal ganglia output that affects the automaticity of movement (28).

Technological advancement has recently allowed monitoring of PFC activity during mobility tasks, using methods such as mobile functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) (29) or electroencephalography (EEG) head caps. These devices are a valid means to measure cortical activity in humans (30, 31) and can be used in a variety of different motor tasks, from static seated tasks to more dynamic mobility tasks, such as walking in different conditions (3241). The majority of previous studies that have used mobile fNIRS or EEG during walking have examined healthy young or older adults (33), with few investigating changes in PD (32, 3640). Measurement of PFC activity via fNIRS in individuals with PD may be particularly useful, as it is relatively quick and easy to set-up and use compared to EEG (42). The previous studies that have examined PFC response using fNIRS in PD during dynamic mobility tasks have generally shown increased PFC activity in people with PD, generally tested ON dopaminergic medication, compared to older adults (32, 33, 3741, 43). However, the majority of these previous studies have used different protocols and two recent reviews of this research area have highlighted methodological issues with previous studies that may prevent the generalization of results (32, 33). In fact, the majority of the studies have examined relatively small cohorts (N < 15), and have lacked short-separation reference channels (emitter-detector distances <1.5 cm apart) that are used remove peripheral hemodynamic response (i.e., increased skin or superficial blood flow due to physical activity rather than cortical activity) from regular channels (3 cm apart) (44), which limits or possibly inflates interpretation of results. Similarly, previous studies have provided no information on the reliability of findings, with little information on how reliable findings are in different populations, such as differences with age or neurological disease. Therefore, using a mobile fNIRS system with short-separation reference channels, this study aimed to: (1) examine test re-test reliability of PFC activity during turning and walking tasks in young adults; and (2) compare PFC activity during turning and walking tasks in young, old and people with PD.

Methods

Participants

A total of 68 participants participated in this study; twenty-five young adults (YA), nineteen older adults (OA) and twenty-four people with PD. Young and older adult participants were recruited via local advertisement (i.e., posters within campus notice boards and e-mails circulated to students and staff members). People with PD were recruited from local Neurology clinics via referrals from movement disorder specialist neurologists. All study procedures were approved by the Oregon Health and Science University Institutional Review Board, with written informed consent obtained before participation.

Participants were included if they were aged 20–40 (young adults) or 50–90 (older adults or PD) years, able to stand or walk for 2 min without assistance. People with PD were included if they had a diagnosis of PD as defined by the UK Brain Bank criteria, Hoehn and Yahr score II-IV and were taking anti-Parkinsonian medication. Exclusion criteria were; musculoskeletal, vestibular, visual, or other medical condition that affected gait or balance.

Experimental Design and Equipment

Participant characteristics of age, sex, height, and weight were recorded. Global cognition was measured with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (45). All testing in people with PD was performed in the OFF medication state, ~12 h after taking last medication dosage. Disease severity was measured using the Movement Disorders Society (MDS-revised) unified Parkinson's disease rating scale (MDS-UPDRS) (46), freezing-status was measured using the new Freezing of Gait Questionnaire (nFOGQ) (47), and levodopa equivalent daily dosage (LEDD) was calculated (48). Specifically, out of twenty-four participants with PD, twelve had freezing of gait, as reported by the nFOGQ (nFOGQ>1).

The participants completed two different motor tasks at self-selected pace; (1) a 2-min 360° turning-in-place task, alternating 360° turning to the left and 360° turning to the right; and (2) a 2-min walking task. Each condition included a baseline of 20 s of standing at the start and end of the task (with 80 s of performing the turning or walking task in between). The walking condition was conducted over-ground with participants walking back and forth over a 9 m straight path, with a 180° degree turn at each end. Condition order was randomized for the participants, with breaks between tasks if needed. A research assistant walked with the participants and stood by the participants during turning to ensure their safety.

Test re-test reliability of the fNIRS measurement of PFC activity during the continuous turning and walking trials was conducted within the young adult subjects. The young adult group performed the same walking and turning tasks for a second time after having the fNIRS device removed and replaced following a short break (~5–10 min).

A mobile fNIRS system (Oxymon, Artinis, Netherlands) was used to record changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) within the PFC at a sampling rate of 50 Hz. Distance from transmitter to detector was 3.5 cm (38) and data was collected and processed in line with previous studies (32, 33). Additionally, two short-separation reference channels at a distance of 1.5 cm (one left and one right hemisphere) were used to allow for removal of peripheral interference (i.e., from blood flow changes in the extra-cerebral layers of the head) in the long source-detector separation channels (44).

Data Processing and Analysis

All processing of fNIRS signals followed current recommendations where possible. A digitizer (PATRIOT, Polhemus, VT, USA) was used to provide 3-dimensional morphological locations for cortical regions of interest relative to scalp position and the fNIRS optode measure. Data from the digitizer was entered into the software package NIRS-statistical package metric mapping (NIRS-SPM, http://www.nitrc.org/projects/nirs_spm) (49), which was implemented within MATLAB 2017a (Mathworks, MA, USA). NIRS-SPM allows registration of fNIRS channel data onto the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard brain space (50). NIRS-SPM used probabilistic registration of the fNIRS co-ordinate data to determine channels that related to ROIs at the group level [described in detail elsewhere (51)]. HbO2 changes were recorded bilaterally (left and right) within the pre-frontal cortex (PFC). The Brodmann areas (BA) that corresponded to the PFC consisted of BA9 and BA10 for all of the participants.

The fNIRS data were processed within custom-made MATLAB algorithms, which consisted of several steps:

1. Data filtering: After zeroing data to the initial time-point, a low-pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 0.14 Hz, based on canonical hemodynamic response function, removed high-frequency noise (52).

2. Baseline correction: Removing the median of the initial 20 s of baseline standing fNIRS signal from the entire trial (i.e., subtracting the baseline period from the rest of the signal).

3. Reference channel correction: This step corrected signal distortions due to artifact caused by breathing, cardiac cycle, vasomotor or other error related to movement (53, 54). First, a scaling factor was determined by detecting the peaks (positive and negative) of the heart rate within the long and short-separation channel signals, then dividing them to produce the scaling factor. This was then used to remove the noise detected within the short-separation reference channels within the long-separation channels. The following formula describe the reference channel correction:

Scaling factor=Peak to peak difference in heart rate in long seperation channel Peak to peak difference in heart rate in short separation channelfNIRS signal   =long separation channel signal                            -(short separation channel signal × Scaling factor)

4. Visual signal inspection: All of the fNIRS signals were visually examined to ensure divergence between the HbO2 and HHb traces. This step allowed exclusion of trials that had poor fNIRS signal collection from participants, as a lack of divergence in HbO2 and HHb indicates noise interference.

5. Averaging across fNIRS channels: In line with previous research (32), bilateral signals from fNIRS optodes over the PFC were median averaged for further analysis. We also median averaged the left and right sided fNIRS optodes separately for further analysis.

Primary Outcome

The primary outcome for this study was change in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) from baseline standing to walking or turning, which is a proxy for cortical activation. The fNIRS system used emitter-detector pairs to emit light into the skull that diffused through brain tissues resulting in scattering of multiple photons (55). These photons were then detected by the fNIRS detector channels when exiting the skull after passing through the cortical brain layers (typically ~1–2 cm deep, with an emitter-detector optode distance of 3.5 cm). Importantly, HbO2, and HHb have different absorption rates for different wavelengths of near-infrared light, which can be analyzed with Beer-Lamberts law equations (56) within the fNIRS software to calculate the relationship between an exciting photon intensity and incident photon intensity to derive changes in HbO2 and HHb (57). Therefore, the fNIRS system measured optical density of the raw signal and converted this to HbO2 and HHb using Beer-Lamberts law (57). HbO2 rather than HHb was reported as our primary outcome due to its sensitivity to walking and cognitive tasks (58, 59). Additionally, changes in HbO2 concentration within local brain capillary networks are caused by neuron firings with brain activity, which is commonly referred to as neurovascular coupling (60). Relative changes from baseline standing (initial 20 s) in HbO2 concentration was reported in an attempt to account for between individual physiological variations (33); see below calculations.

Walking and turning periods;

1. Early = Median HbO2 first 40 s of task—Median HbO2 initial standing period (20 s)

2. Late = Median HbO2 second 40 s of task—Median HbO2 initial standing period (20 s)

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted in SPSS (v.24, IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) and Shapiro-Wilks tests determined data normality with parametric analysis used throughout. Mean differences with paired-sample t-tests, intra-class correlation coefficients (Absolute Agreement: ICC2, 1) and Bland-Altman plots with 95% limits of agreement (LoA 95%) were used to determine the test re-test reliability of PFC activity measurement via fNIRS between the first and second data captures in young adults. Acceptance ratings for ICCs were set at excellent (>0.75), moderate (0.40–0.75), and poor (<0.40) agreement (61).

PFC HbO2 activation during turning and walking in young, older and PD groups was reported via mean and standard deviation. Separate linear mixed-effect models compared groups (YA, OA, PD), Periods (Early vs. Late) and PFC regions (Left vs. Right), with a random intercept for each subject within the models. Post-hoc independent t-tests explored significant differences between specific groups (YA vs. OA, YA vs. PD, OA vs. PD). We also compared between groups differences using Cohen's d effect sizes based on mean (SD) scores. Effect sizes were interpreted as small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8) (61). Due to the exploratory nature of the analysis, statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Participants

Demographic characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1. Groups were significantly different for age (p < 0.001), but age was not significantly different between older adults and PD groups (p = 0.166). Similarly, young adults tended to be taller than the older adults and PD subjects, but the older adults and PD groups did not significantly differ for height (p = 0.146). The groups were well-matched for gender (p = 0.160), weight (p = 0.287), and cognitive ability (p = 0.227).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographic characteristics.

Test Re-test Reliability of fNIRS Measurement During Turning and Walking

Following removal of fNIRS data that had poor signal quality, data from 19 young adult subjects were analyzed for walking (n = 6 young adult walking trials were excluded following visual signal inspection: 4 first trial and 2 second trial errors) and 25 subjects were analyzed for turning to determine test re-test reliability of fNIRS measurement.

On average, there was moderate (ICC2, 1 = 0.67) reliability of PFC activity measured via mobile fNIRS during turning and walking in young adults (Table 2). There were no significant differences between any of the outcomes over the two sessions, with very low average difference between testing sessions (Mean Difference = 0.03 μm, Figures 1, 2). Reliability across the different time periods (early vs. late) and regions of interest (Left, Right or combined) within the PFC ranged from moderate (ICC2, 1 = 0.52) to excellent (ICC2, 1 = 0.79). Importantly, the primary outcome over the duration of the motor tasks (combined left and right PFC regions) had moderate reliability (turning; ICC2, 1 = 0.67, walking; ICC2, 1 0.71) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reliability of fNIRS recording of PFC activity in young adults.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Bland Altman plots demonstrating agreement between the two 2-min trials of walking for overall PFC activity, Left and Right PFC activity, and Early and Late PFC activity. Dashed lines represent LoA.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Bland Altman plots demonstrating agreement between the two 2-min trials of turning for overall PFC activity, Left and Right PFC activity, and Early and Late PFC activity. Dashed lines represent LoA.

PFC Activity During Continuous Turning and Walking in Young, Old, and Parkinson's

Table 3 shows the relative change in HbO2 during continuous turning and walking in the groups. Overall, the PD group had higher average levels of PFC activation during turning and walking than young or older adults across the majority of the time-points and PFC regions. Older adults also had higher average PFC activation during turning and walking compared to young adults.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Relative change in HbO2 during turning and walking in young, old and Parkinson's participants.

During walking the groups were significantly different for overall PFC activation (p = 0.025), left PFC activation (p = 0.012) and for the early period (first 40 s) of walking (p = 0.007) (Table 3). There was also a significant difference in PFC activation between early and late periods of walking across the groups (p = 0.012), with higher PFC activity in the early period. Post-hoc testing indicated that the PD group had significantly higher PFC activation during the early period of walking compared to the young (p = 0.008) and older adults (p = 0.037). The PD group also had significantly higher overall (p = 0.025) and left side (p = 0.017) PFC activation than the young adults. However, the older adult group did not have significantly different PFC activation compared to the young adults, with small effect size differences between groups (Table 4). Overall, the PD group had moderate to large differences (Cohen's d 0.52 to 0.86) in PFC activation during walking compared to young and older adults (Table 4), particularly for the early period and left PFC region.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effect sizes (Cohens d) for differences in HbO2 during turning and walking between groups.

Tables 3, 4 demonstrate that the effect of turning on PFC activation was similar across groups, as during continuous 360° turning there were no significant group differences for any outcome. However, there was a trending difference between PFC regions during turning (left vs. right, p = 0.059), with higher right PFC activity compared to left (Table 3). Despite the lack of a significant difference, effect sizes showed that the early period of turning differentiated the young group from the older adult and PD groups (Table 4; Cohens d of 0.86 and 1.21, respectively), with young adults having the lowest activation of the three groups (Table 3).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the test re-test reliability of PFC activity measurement via fNIRS during continuous walking and turning tasks. Findings demonstrate that the measurement of PFC activity during continuous (2 min) turning or walking has acceptable reliability, as there was little difference in HbO2 measurement between separate re-test trials. With a growing interest in understanding brain activity during motor tasks, our results contribute to the development of robust protocols to examine PFC activity using fNIRS during continuous walking or turning tasks.

This study also reports differences in PFC activation during continuous (2 min) 360° turning and walking in young adults, older adults and people with PD. Specifically, people with PD had significantly higher PFC activation during walking compared to young and older adults; however, older adults were not significantly different compared to young adults. Increased PFC activation may indicate greater cognitive demand during walking in PD due to impaired movement automaticity.

Reliability of fNIRS Monitoring of PFC Activation During Turning and Walking

The mobile fNIRS device used in this study is a commercial device that allows access to the raw data that registers HbO2 concentration and subsequent implementation of our custom-made algorithms for data analysis. Re-test reliability of HbO2 signal recorded with the mobile fNIRS device was conducted within our young adult group with some variations across the conditions (walking or turning), as well as when the HbO2 signal was broken into different PFC hemispheres (left or right) or trial times (early or late). When using our fNIRS system with short-separation reference channels and fixed data analysis pipeline we found moderate reliability (ICC2, 1 0.67) of the PFC HbO2 signal during the continuous walking and turning tasks. These results, together with small LoA between trials, indicated that we could be confident that this signal is reliable, as reported in Figures 1, 2. However, there were changes in reliability when breaking the fNIRS signal down into individual hemispheres or times of the signal (early vs. late), which is similar to previous static fNIRS findings (62). On the basis of our results, it can be stated that HbO2 outcomes measured using a mobile fNIRS device during continuous (2 min) walking or turning are relatively stable. However, when breaking the signal into specific features, such as hemispheres or time-periods, the stability of the HbO2 signal can be altered.

Impact of Aging and PD on PFC Activation During Turning and Walking

This study confirms that PFC activity, measured through mobile fNIRS, can show differences in PFC activation during continuous walking and turning between young, older and subjects with PD. Specifically, we found that people with PD, OFF their medication, had significantly higher PFC activation during walking than young and older adults, in line with previous studies where PD subjects were ON medication (32, 33, 3740). Higher levels of PFC activation during continuous walking, particularly the early period of walking, in PD compared to the other groups likely reflects the need to use executive-attentional resources even during relatively simple tasks (i.e., usual walking) (21, 26). The use of cognitive resources to compensate for PD related deficits is similar to previously reported theories of PD walking, which hypothesized that to compensate for movement automaticity deficits people with PD increased cognitive control (63), particularly executive control (26). However, executive-attentional deficits are common in PD (64) and these impact the ability to effectively compensate for underlying deficits. Therefore, when tasks become more challenging (such as dual-tasks, obstacles etc.) people with PD may not be able to effectively respond (24), which impacts gait and mobility, with implications for falls risk. Future studies may uncover further age or PD-related deficits with the use of more complex tasks that provide additional cognitive burden to participants.

Interestingly, although older adults had slightly greater PFC activation levels than young adults during walking, our findings were not significantly different which is in agreement to another previous fNIRS study (32). This demonstrated that healthy young and older adults may use their executive-attentional resources in a similar manner when walking, with little need to activate the PFC due to intact lower-level neural structures and activity compared to people with PD. Similarly, there were limited group differences found during the continuous 360° turning-in-place task, which may indicate that this complex task requires cognitive resources regardless of age or disease. Indeed, average PFC activation was higher in young adults, older adults and PD during continuous turning compared to walking, with PD participants having the highest HbO2 concentrations across groups. Interestingly, although there were no significant group differences, there were large between group effects for PFC activation during the early period of the turning task. This highlighted that young adults had much lower PFC activation during the early period of continuous turning than older adults or people with PD. Aging and PD may therefore lead to greater cognitive control being required to begin complex continuous motor tasks, however less cognitive control is required once the task underway.

Clinical Interpretation

PFC activation appears to increase with walking in PD, but not with age. This may represent cortical compensation for sub-cortical dysfunction with PD. Findings suggest that targeting cortical activation, particularly executive-attentional activity within the PFC, with interventions such as pharmaceuticals, cueing strategies (visual, auditory or proprioceptive) or transcranial magnetic or direct current stimulation may help to alleviate the cortical burden of walking in PD. However, future studies are needed to establish findings and examine response to such interventions. Specifically, future studies should assess whether the observed increase in PFC activation is related to PD itself or to freezing of gait. In fact, as freezing of gait could represent a disruption of gait automaticity, people experiencing freezing of gait, even without PD, may show an increased cortical control of gait.

Study Strengths and Limitations

A key strength of this study was the use of short-separation reference channels (1.5 cm apart) to account for the peripheral hemodynamic response that is associated with physical activity (44). Short-separation channels have only been used in one other mobile fNIRS study (65), but they should be used within future work to reduce noise and ensure repeatable findings. Another strength is that we have provided detailed data analysis steps from our fNIRS signal processing, whereas other previous studies often use ‘black-box' data analysis tools such as NIRS-SPM (66) or Homer2 (67).

A limitation of this study was that, despite being the first to asses test re-test reliability of fNIRS HbO2 measurement during walking and turning, test re-test analysis was only conducted in young adults. Reliability could possibly change in older adults or PD, as previous studies have shown this in patients with TBI (68). However, we may expect that reliability would even be higher in older adults and further in PD as, unlike young adults, they require more cognitive control of movement due to a loss of motor automaticity (1, 28), therefore findings in these groups may be more consistent. However, this is mainly speculative at this point, future studies could examine re-test reliability in older adult and PD using the same protocol developed here. Additionally, we did not examine the influence of disease stage, duration or other clinical factors (e.g., freezing of gait) in PD, which future studies should examine to provide greater understanding of cortical activation during walking and turning in PD.

Conclusions

This study has demonstrated that the measurement of PFC activation during continuous turning and walking using a mobile fNIRS system with short-separation reference channels is repeatable. PFC activation during continuous turning and walking differs between young adults, older adults and PD, with greater activation required in PD compared to control groups for these motor tasks. Using the robust method developed in this study, future work could establish these findings within larger cohorts and examine the impact of more complex tasks on PFC activation.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Oregon Health & Science University IRB Committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the OHSU IRB.

Author Contributions

SS and MM conceptualized the question and hypothesis. MM designed the study from which the data originates. SS, VB, and MM contributed to data collection and analysis. SS, VB, JQ, and MM contributed to the interpretation, writing and editing of the manuscript. SS wrote the first draft.

Funding

This study was funded by a Career Development Award 5R00HD078492-04 (PI MM) and PDF Fellowship PF-FBS-1898 (PI SS).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Scherder E, Eggermont L, Swaab D, Van heuvelen M, Kamsma Y, de GreefM, et al. Gait in ageing and associated dementias; its relationship with cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2007) 31:485–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Dite W, Temple VA. Development of a clinical measure of turning for older adults. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. (2002) 81:857–66. doi: 10.1097/00002060-200211000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R, Xue X. Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2007) 78:929–35. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.106914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ambrose AF, Paul G, Hausdorff JM. Risk factors for falls among older adults: a review of the literature. Maturitas. (2013) 75:51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Springer S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Yogev G, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: the role of aging, falls, and executive function. Mov Disord. (2006) 21:950–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.20848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Montero-Odasso M, Muir SW, Speechley M. Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking, and risk of falls. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2012) 93:293–9 doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Galna B, Lord S, Burn DJ, Rochester L. Progression of gait dysfunction in incident Parkinson's disease: impact of medication and phenotype. Mov Disord. (2015) 30:359–67. doi: 10.1002/mds.26110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Stack E, Ashburn A. Dysfunctional turning in Parkinson's disease. Disabil Rehabil. (2008) 30:1222–9. doi: 10.1080/09638280701829938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Zampieri C, Salarian A, Carlson-Kuhta P, Nutt JG, Horak FB. Assessing mobility at home in people with early Parkinson's disease using an instrumented Timed Up and Go test. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2011) 17:277–80. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Mellone S, Mancini M, King LA, Horak FB, Chiari L. The quality of turning in Parkinson's disease: a compensatory strategy to prevent postural instability? J NeuroEng Rehabil. (2016) 13:39. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0147-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mancini M, Smulders K, Cohen RG, Horak FB, Giladi N, Nutt JG. The clinical significance of freezing while turning in Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience. (2017) 343:222–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.11.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Chen P-H, Liou D-J, Liou K-C, Liang J-L, Cheng S-J, Shaw J-S. Walking turns in Parkinson's disease patients with freezing of gait: the short-term effects of different cueing strategies. Int J Gerontol. (2016) 10:71–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ijge.2014.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mak MK, Patla A, Hui-Chan C. Sudden turn during walking is impaired in people with Parkinson's disease. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 190:43–51. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1446-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Huxham F, Baker R, Morris ME, Iansek R. Footstep adjustments used to turn during walking in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:817–23. doi: 10.1002/mds.21932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Giladi N. Freezing of gait. Clin Overview Adv Neurol. (2001) 87:191–7. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-7007-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs E, Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease: the impact of dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2563–70. doi: 10.1002/mds.23327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Morris R, Lord S, Bunce J, Burn D, Rochester LJN, Reviews B. Gait and cognition: mapping the global and discrete relationships in ageing and neurodegenerative disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 64:326–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Morris R, Lord S, Lawson RA, Coleman S, Galna B, Duncan GW, et al. Gait rather than cognition predicts decline in specific cognitive domains in early Parkinson's disease. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2017) 72:1656–62. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Burn DJ, Rowan EN, Allan LM, Molloy S, O'brien JT, Mckeith IG. Motor subtype and cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease, Parkinson's disease with dementia, and dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2006) 77:585–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson's disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci. (2005) 22:1248–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:329–42. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hausdorff JM, Balash J, Giladi N. Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2003) 16:53–8. doi: 10.1177/0891988702250580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hollman JH, Kovash FM, Kubik JJ, Linbo RA. Age-related differences in spatiotemporal markers of gait stability during dual task walking. Gait Posture. (2007) 26:113–9. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kelly VE, Eusterbrock AJ, Shumway-Cook A. A review of dual-task walking deficits in people with Parkinson's disease: motor and cognitive contributions, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Parkinsons Dis. (2012) 2012:918719. doi: 10.1155/2012/918719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Bilateral coordination of gait and Parkinson's disease: the effects of dual tasking. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2009) 80:347–50. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.157362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lord S, Rochester L, Hetherington V, Allcock LM, Burn D. Executive dysfunction and attention contribute to gait interference in 'off' state Parkinson's Disease. Gait Posture. (2010) 31:169–74. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Amboni M, Barone P, Hausdorff JM. Cognitive contributions to gait and falls: evidence and implications. Mov Disord. (2013) 28:1520–33. doi: 10.1002/mds.25674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wu T, Hallett M, Chan P. Motor automaticity in Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Dis. (2015) 82:226–34. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Herold F, Wiegel P, Scholkmann F, Thiers A, Hamacher D, Schega L. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy in movement science: a systematic review on cortical activity in postural and walking tasks. Neurophotonics. (2017) 4:041403. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.4.4.041403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Noah JA, Ono Y, Nomoto Y, Shimada S, Tachibana A, Zhang X, et al. fMRI validation of fNIRS measurements during a naturalistic task. J Vis Exp. (2015) 100:e52116. doi: 10.3791/52116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Strangman G, Culver JP, Thompson JH, Boas DA. A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation. Neuroimage. (2002) 17:719–31. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Stuart S, Vitorio R, Morris R, Martini DN, Fino PC, Mancini M. Cortical activity during walking and balance tasks in older adults and in people with Parkinson's disease: a structured review. Maturitas. (2018) 113:53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2018.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Vitorio R, Stuart S, Rochester L, Alcock L, Pantall A. fNIRS response during walking - artefact or cortical activity? A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 83:160–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Holtzer R, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, Izzetoglu K, Onaral B, Verghese J. fNIRS study of walking and walking while talking in young and old individuals. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2011) 66:879–87. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Holtzer R, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, Wang C, England S, Verghese J. Online fronto-cortical control of simple and attention-demanding locomotion in humans. Neuroimage. (2015) 112:152–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Holtzer R, Verghese J, Allali G, Izzetoglu M, Wang C, Mahoney JR. Neurological gait abnormalities moderate the functional brain signature of the posture first hypothesis. Brain Topogr. (2016) 29:334–43. doi: 10.1007/s10548-015-0465-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Gazit E, Mirelman A, Hausdorff J, Giladi N. Increased activation of the frontal lobe is associated with freezing of gait in patients with Parkinson's disease: an fNIRS study (P6.074). Neurology. (2015) 84(14 Suppl.):P6.074.

Google Scholar

38. Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Gazit E, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM, Mirelman A. Changes in oxygenated hemoglobin link freezing of gait to frontal activation in patients with Parkinson disease: an fNIRS study of transient motor-cognitive failures. J Neurol. (2015b) 262:899–908. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7650-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM, Mirelman A. When is higher level cognitive control needed for locomotor tasks among patients with Parkinson's disease? Brain Topogr. (2017) 30:531–8. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0564-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Maidan I, Nieuwhof F, Bernad-Elazari H, Reelick MF, Bloem BR, Giladi N, et al. The role of the frontal lobe in complex walking among patients with Parkinson's disease and healthy older adults: an fNIRS study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2016) 30:963–71. doi: 10.1177/1545968316650426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mirelman A, Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Nieuwhof F, Reelick M, Giladi N, et al. Increased frontal brain activation during walking while dual tasking: an fNIRS study in healthy young adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2014) 11:85. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ferrari M, Quaresima V. A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. NeuroImage. (2012) 63:921–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Nieuwhof F, Reelick MF, Maidan I, Mirelman A, Hausdorff JM, Olde Rikkert MGM, et al. Measuring prefrontal cortical activity during dual task walking in patients with Parkinson's disease: feasibility of using a new portable fNIRS device. Pilot Feasibil Stud. (2016) 2:59. doi: 10.1186/s40814-016-0099-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Gagnon L, Cooper RJ, Yucel MA, Perdue KL, Greve DN, Boas DA. Short separation channel location impacts the performance of short channel regression in NIRS. Neuroimage. (2012) 59:2518–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Dalrymple-Alford JC, Macaskill MR, Nakas CT, Livingston L, Graham C, Crucian GP, et al. The MoCA: well-suited screen for cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology. (2010) 75:1717–25. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc29c9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, Stebbins GT, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:2129–70. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nieuwboer A, Rochester L, Herman T, Vandenberghe W, Emil GE, Thomaes T, et al. Reliability of the new freezing of gait questionnaire: agreement between patients with Parkinson's disease and their carers. Gait Posture. (2009) 30:459–63. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.07.108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CEJMD. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2649–53. doi: 10.1002/mds.23429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ye JC, Tak S, Jang KE, Jung J, Jang J. NIRS-SPM: Statistical parametric mapping for near-infrared spectroscopy. NeuroImage. (2009) 44:428–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Tsuzuki D, Dan I. Spatial registration for functional near-infrared spectroscopy: from channel position on the scalp to cortical location in individual and group analyses. Neuroimage. (2014) 85:92–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Singh AK, Okamoto M, Dan H, Jurcak V, Dan I. Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. NeuroImage. (2005) 27:842–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Friston KJ, Mechelli A, Turner R, Price CJ. Nonlinear responses in fMRI: the Balloon model, Volterra kernels, and other hemodynamics. NeuroImage. (2000) 12:466–77. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Goodwin JR, Gaudet CR, Berger AJ. Short-channel functional near-infrared spectroscopy regressions improve when source-detector separation is reduced. Neurophotonics. (2014) 1:015002. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.1.1.015002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Tak S, Ye JC. Statistical analysis of fNIRS data: a comprehensive review. NeuroImage. (2014) 85:72–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Naseer N, Hong K-S. fNIRS-based brain-computer interfaces: a review. Front Human Neurosci. (2015) 9:3. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kocsis L, Herman P, Eke A. The modified Beer-Lambert law revisited. Phys Med Biol. (2006) 51:N91–8. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/5/N02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Delpy DT, Cope M, Van Der Zee P, Arridge S, Wray S, Wyatt J. Estimation of optical pathlength through tissue from direct time of flight measurement. Phys Med Biol. (1988) 33:1433–42. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/33/12/008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Suzuki M, Miyai I, Ono T, Kubota K. Activities in the frontal cortex and gait performance are modulated by preparation. An fNIRS study. NeuroImage. (2008) 39:600–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Miyai I, Tanabe HC, Sase I, Eda H, Oda I, Konishi I, et al. Cortical mapping of gait in humans: a near-infrared spectroscopic topography study. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:1186–92. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Steinbrink J, Villringer A, Kempf F, Haux D, Boden S, Obrig H. Illuminating the BOLD signal: combined fMRI-fNIRS studies. Magn Reson Imaging. (2006) 24:495–505. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2005.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Field A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. London: Sage (2013).

Google Scholar

62. Plichta MM, Herrmann MJ, Baehne CG, Ehlis AC, Richter MM, Pauli P, et al. Event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS): are the measurements reliable? NeuroImage. (2006) 31:116–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Gilat M, Bell PT, Martens KAE, Georgiades MJ, Hall JM, Walton CC, et al. Dopamine depletion impairs gait automaticity by altering cortico-striatal and cerebellar processing in Parkinson's disease. NeuroImage. (2017) 152:207–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Muslimović D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand BJN. Cognitive profile of patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology. (2005) 65:1239–45. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180516.69442.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Koenraadt KLM, Roelofsen EGJ, Duysens J, Keijsers NLW. Cortical control of normal gait and precision stepping: an fNIRS study. NeuroImage. (2014) 85:415–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Vitorio R, Stuart S, Gobbi LTB, Rochester L, Alcock L, Pantall A. Reduced gait variability and enhanced brain activity in older adults with auditory cues: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2018) 32:976–87. doi: 10.1177/1545968318805159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lu CF, Liu YC, Yang YR, Wu YT, Wang RY. Maintaining gait performance by cortical activation during dual-task interference: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0129390. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Bhambhani Y, Maikala R, Farag M, Rowland G. Reliability of near-infrared spectroscopy measures of cerebral oxygenation and blood volume during handgrip exercise in nondisabled and traumatic brain-injured subjects. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2006) 43:845. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2005.09.0151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson's disease, fNIRS, turning, walking, pre-frontal cortex

Citation: Stuart S, Belluscio V, Quinn JF and Mancini M (2019) Pre-frontal Cortical Activity During Walking and Turning Is Reliable and Differentiates Across Young, Older Adults and People With Parkinson's Disease. Front. Neurol. 10:536. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00536

Received: 15 January 2019; Accepted: 03 May 2019;
Published: 22 May 2019.

Edited by:

Pedro J. Garcia-Ruiz, Hospital Universitario Fundación Jiménez Díaz, Spain

Reviewed by:

Jeannette R. Mahoney, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, United States
Marta Ruiz-Lopez, Hospital Universitario Fundación Jiménez Díaz, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Stuart, Belluscio, Quinn and Mancini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Martina Mancini, mancinim@ohsu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.