Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 13 August 2019
Sec. Applied Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Dynamic Functioning of Resting State Networks in Physiological and Pathological Conditions View all 39 articles

Lower Functional Connectivity in Vestibular-Limbic Networks in Individuals With Subclinical Agoraphobia

  • 1Department of Medicine and Surgery, Saint Camillus International University of Health and Medical Sciences, Rome, Italy
  • 2Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
  • 3Department of Systems Medicine and Centre of Space BioMedicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
  • 4Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology and Otorhinolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, United States
  • 5Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 6Institute of Bioimaging and Molecular Physiology, National Research Council, Milan, Italy
  • 7Department of Biomedicine and Prevention, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
  • 8Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston, MA, United States

Background: Agoraphobia was described in 1871 as a condition of fear-related alterations in spatial orientation and locomotor control triggered by places or situations that might cause a patient to panic and feel trapped. In contemporary nosology, however, this original concept of agoraphobia was split into two diagnostic entities, i.e., the modern anxiety disorder of agoraphobia, consisting solely of phobic/avoidant symptoms in public spaces, and the recently defined vestibular disorder of persistent postural perceptual dizziness (PPPD), characterized by dizziness, and unsteadiness exacerbated by visual motion stimuli. Previous neuroimaging studies found altered brain activity and connectivity in visual-vestibular networks of patients with PPPD vs. healthy controls. Neuroticism and introversion, which pre-dispose to both agoraphobia and PPPD, influenced brain responses to vestibular and visual motion stimuli in patients with PPPD. Similar neuroimaging studies have not been undertaken in patients with agoraphobia in its current definition. Given their shared history and pre-disposing factors, we sought to test the hypotheses that individuals with agoraphobic symptoms have alterations in visual-vestibular networks similar to those of patients with PPPD, and that these alterations are influenced by neuroticism and introversion.

Methods: Drawing from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) database, we matched 52 participants with sub-clinical agoraphobia and 52 control subjects without agoraphobic symptoms on 19 demographic and psychological/psychiatric variables. We then employed a graph-theoretical framework to compare resting-state functional magnetic resonance images between groups and evaluated the interactive effects of neuroticism and introversion on the brain signatures of agoraphobia.

Results: Individuals with subclinical agoraphobia had lower global clustering, efficiency and transitivity relative to controls. They also had lower connectivity metrics in two brain networks, one positioned to process incoming visual space-motion information, assess threat, and initiate/inhibit behavioral responses (visuospatial-emotional network) and one positioned to control and monitor locomotion (vestibular-navigational network). Introversion interacted with agoraphobic symptoms to lower the connectivity of the visuospatial-emotional network. This contrasted with previous findings describing neuroticism-associated higher connectivity in a narrower visual-spatial-frontal network in patients with PPPD.

Conclusion: Functional connectivity was lower in two brain networks in subclinical agoraphobia as compared to healthy controls. These networks integrate visual vestibular and emotional response to guide movement in space.

Introduction

Agoraphobia (fear of the marketplace), was first described by the German neuroscientist C.F. Westphal in 1871 as a syndrome of altered spatial perception, cognitive distortions about safe locomotion, and fear-driven limitations of mobility (1). Westphal called this condition agoraphobia because he observed it in the busy marketplaces of town squares in nineteenth century European villages. Critically, he considered anxiety, altered spatial perception, and restricted mobility to be “part of one process.” In the modern era, agoraphobia is conceptualized as an anxiety disorder defined solely by fear and avoidance of public places outside of home (2), and the space and motion symptoms that concerned Westphal are no longer part of the disorder. For nearly a century, such symptoms were relegated to the vague notion of psychogenic dizziness, until conditions such as supermarket syndrome (3) and space phobia (4) appeared in the medical literature in the 1970's. These were followed by the syndromes of phobic postural vertigo (5), space-motion phobia (6), and chronic subjective dizziness (7) in which vestibular symptoms and difficulties with exposure to space and motion stimuli were core elements, either co-existing with or pre-disposed and precipitated by anxiety. All of these syndromes provided the background for the recently described vestibular disorder called persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD), which is defined solely by vestibular symptoms and sensitivity to visual space-motion stimuli (8). Thus, contemporary diagnostic nomenclature (9) separated Westphal's agoraphobia into two conditions, one with anxiety-phobic symptoms and one with vestibular-motion symptoms. This distinction is clinically useful, as agoraphobia and PPPD can be seen quite independently of one another in medical settings, but it should not be interpreted as a formal severing of potential etio-pathogenetic mechanisms. Indeed, Westphal and his contemporaries engaged in lively debates about the relative contributions of visual, vestibular, and psychological processes to the original concept of agoraphobia (10).

Several lines of evidence suggest a shared pre-disposition and partially overlapping clinical manifestations of agoraphobia and PPPD. For example, the personality traits of neuroticism and introversion may be associated with both conditions. A study of twin siblings found independent genetic contributions of neuroticism and introversion to agoraphobia (11). Studies of chronic subjective dizziness found that neurotic and introverted traits were significantly more common in patients with this precursor of PPPD than in patients with other chronic vestibular disorders who had similar levels of dizziness and anxiety or normative samples (12, 13). Studies of PPPD itself also identified levels of neuroticism that were higher than normal (14), and structural vestibular disorders may trigger both PPPD and agoraphobia (8, 15). Investigations of patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia found increased rates of vestibular symptoms, sensitivity to visual motion stimuli, alterations in postural control, and subtle abnormalities in vestibular laboratory tests compared to normal controls (1621). Conversely, research in patients with the precursors of PPPD found that 60% had anxiety disorders, and in particular panic disorder and agoraphobia (22). Treatment with the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) paroxetine was shown to normalize changes in postural control in patients with panic disorder (23). Indeed, SSRIs are the mainstay of pharmacological treatment of PPPD, even in patients without psychiatric comorbidity (24).

Given their shared history and the overlapping clinical features, it is possible for agoraphobia and PPPD to share underlying brain mechanisms. Neuroanatomical studies identified extensive connections between vestibular and anxiety systems, extending from the brainstem to the cortex (25), and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies in normal humans revealed significant effects of vestibular stimulation on activity and connectivity in both vestibular and anxiety regions, modulated by neuroticism and introversion (26, 27). Space and motion information in the brain is processed in a widely distributed network, and there is no unimodal primary sensory cortex for vestibular inputs as there are for other sensory modalities. Instead, visual space-motion data are processed by a “multimodal vestibular network” of brain areas that contain neurons that receive combinations of vestibular, visual, and somatosensory stimuli. These multimodal neurons have been found in several regions centered around the parietal opercula, posterior insula, and adjacent posterior perisylvian regions of the parietal and temporal cortex, which constitute the central connectivity nodes of the multimodal vestibular cortex (28, 29) and contribute to perception of gravity (3032). This network extends to the medial superior temporal area (MST) and posterior inferior temporal gyrus, ventral intraparietal area, superior parietal lobe, somato-motor cortex, hippocampal formation, anterior insula, inferior frontal gyrus, and cingulate cortex (2832). Some of these multimodal areas (e.g., anterior insula and hippocampus) are critically involved in emotional processing, which may underlie clinical observations of close associations between anxiety and vestibular disorders, including agoraphobia and PPPD (10, 25, 3337).

The structure and function of brain regions that comprise the multimodal visuo-vestibular network have been studied in patients with PPPD and its precursors using various neuroimaging methods (3844). In studies that controlled for group-averaged anxiety-related variables but not for their variances, patients with PPPD compared to healthy controls had lower activity, functional connectivity, and cortical folding within the parietal opercula (OP1-4), as well as in a wider network of visuo-vestibular regions including the posterior insula, posterior superior temporal sulcus, superior parietal cortex and motor vestibular regions (3840). In contrast, patients with PPPD compared to healthy controls had higher fronto-occipital connectivity linked to state and trait anxiety (41, 45). Furthermore, patients with phobic postural vertigo (essentially PPPD plus phobic/avoidance symptoms related to visual space-motion stimuli) had increased connectivity between motor cortex (Broadman area 4—BA4) and orbitofrontal, fronto-polar, and anterior cingulate cortices compared to healthy controls (44).

The brain mechanisms presumed to underlie agoraphobic anxiety have been described extensively (46, 47). In recent neuroimaging studies, patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia showed greater activation than normal controls in the insular cortices bilaterally as well as in the left inferior frontal gyrus, dorsomedial pre-frontal cortex, caudate and hippocampus in response to exposure to symptom-specific pictures compared to healthy controls (48). Activations in the striatum and insula may be stronger in anticipation than actual viewing of agoraphobia-specific stimuli (49). In a non-clinical sample, subclinical agoraphobic symptoms correlated positively with alterations in cortical volumes of the right lingual gyrus, left superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri, and bilateral calcarine sulci. Exploratory analyses extended those findings to the left pre-central and post-central gyri, the right orbitofrontal cortex, insula, and posterior cingulate gyrus, and bilateral precunei (50). A comparison of these results to those from patients with PPPD suggests potential similarities and differences in task-driven activation (e.g., visual cortical areas—positively correlated with symptom severity in both PPPD and agoraphobia; insula—decreased in PPPD, increased in agoraphobia). However, it is not known if connectivity differs between these disorders. Furthermore, most agoraphobia studies were not controlled for the potential confounds of neuroticism and introversion, which pre-dispose to both conditions (1114, 45).

In this study we aimed to explore the functional brain connectivity signatures of sub-clinical agoraphobia through a graph-theoretical framework applied to task-free functional MRI in a group of healthy participants selected from the database of the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (51). The extensive information available on subjects in the HCP allowed us to select subjects who reported agoraphobic symptoms and carefully match them to a comparison group with no agoraphobic symptoms on demographics, handedness, and 16 other variables that could interact with agoraphobia such as levels of panic, anxiety, and depressive symptoms, perceived stress, personality traits, negative affect, and self-efficacy. We compared the two groups that we selected from the HCP to test the hypothesis that people with agoraphobic symptoms show lower connectivity in areas of the multimodal vestibular network previously identified in patients with PPPD. In addition, we aimed to assess whether anxiety-related personality traits interact with agoraphobic symptoms to further decrease connectivity.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We used rsfMRI data from the S1200 HCP data release, which comprises MRI data and psychological assessments from 1,003 healthy volunteers (http://www.humanconnectome.org/documentation/S1200/). Due to the dominance of the right hemisphere representation of the vestibular function in right-handed individuals (and of the left hemisphere in left-handers) (52) we selected only right-handed individuals, i.e., individuals with handedness score higher than 50 (−100/100 range) (53), to avoid confounds due to different lateralization of functions. Among right-handers, 52 individuals reported agoraphobia symptoms. Experience of at least one episode of agoraphobia, panic or major depression and the number of lifetime depressive symptoms were evaluated by the HCP consortium through the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) (54) in agreement with DSM-5 Criteria (2). Anger, aggression, hostility and fear affect were assessed via the NIH Toolbox Fear-Affect Survey, comprising items from the PROMIS Anxiety Item Bank (55). Fear-somatic arousal and sadness were assessed through the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (56, 57) and PROMIS Depression Item Bank (58), respectively. Five Factor Model personality traits were assessed via the NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI) (59). Perceived stress and self-efficacy were scored on the Perceived Stress Scale (60) and on the General Self-Efficacy Scale (http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfscal.htm).

Matching Procedure

We matched agoraphobic subjects to 52 healthy right-handed individuals by age, gender, handedness, psychological variables (anger-affect, anger-hostility, anger-aggressivity, fear-affect, fear-somatic arousal, sadness, perceived stress, self-efficacy), presence of psychiatric disorders (panic disorder, one major depressive episode over lifetime, total number of depressive symptoms over lifetime), and personality scores (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, conscientiousness, agreeableness). The matching procedure was as follows: (i) repeated random sampling (with replacement) of 52 non-agoraphobic subjects from the whole (n = 1,003 HCP database; (ii) for each sample, group-wise comparison (against the agoraphobia group) of mean values (Mann-Whitney U-Test for continuous variables and Chi-Squared test for dichotomous variables) and variances (Brown Forsythe test) for all matching variables, and (iii) acceptance of the first “matched” sample of 52 healthy subjects when all resulting p-values (19 comparisons for both means and variances) were p ≥ 0.05 (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Matching of agoraphic and healthy control groups was performed through repeated random sampling (with replacement) of 52 non-agoraphobic subjects and subsequent statistical testing on matching variables. (B) Distributions of the demographic, psychological, psychiatric, and personality variables for the two matched groups. Variables relative to agoraphobic and healthy subjects are shown in orange and in gray, respectively. pm and pv refer to median and variance comparison, respectively.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) Scanning and Definition of Nodes

Within HCP scanning procedures, fMRI data were acquired on a Siemens Skyra 3T in four runs of ~15 min each, through a Gradient-echo EPI sequence (1,200 volume per run, TR = 720 ms; TE = 33.1 ms, FA = 52 deg, FOV = 208 × 180 mm (RO × PE), resolution (x, y, z) = 2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm3, Multiband factor = 8, bandwidth = 2290 Hz/Px).

Each 15-min run of each subject's rfMRI data was pre-processed according to Smith et al. (61); it was minimally-pre-processed (62), and had artifacts removed using ICA + FIX (63, 64). ICA + FIX is a data-driven algorithm, which is based on an automated classifier specifically and manually trained to discern, amongst independent component analysis (ICA) results, diverse sources of noise (acquisition, movement, physiological artifacts). The version trained for HPC data has been seen to guarantee an accuracy (as well as sensitivity and specificity) of around 99% (61, 63, 64). Inter-subject registration of cerebral cortex was carried out using areal feature-based alignment and the Multimodal Surface Matching algorithm (“MSMAll”) (65, 66). For feeding into group-PCA, each dataset was then temporally demeaned and had variance normalization applied according to Beckmann and Smith (67), after which group-PCA output was generated by MIGP (MELODIC's Incremental Group-PCA) from 1,003 subjects (68). More details can be found at https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult/article/release-s1200-extensively-processed-rfmri-data. While each resulting spatial component map (node map) is obtained through regression and therefore defined over the whole brain, there is a steep roll-off in areas which are mostly related to other components. In order to highlight the brain areas which are dominant/unique in each component, for visualization and description purposes in this paper we thresholded each map at the 98th percentile. For analysis, we employed the individual adjacency matrices computed by the HCP consortium based on group-PCA at dimensionality 100. These which were based on partial correlation coefficients between node timeseries (“netmats2”). The graph representation of each adjacency matrix therefore comprised 100 nodes. Each node can be considered a resting state network in itself [see e.g., (69) for an example at dimensionality 15].

Graph Theoretical Metrics

After matching, in all adjacency matrices negative correlations were set to zero. For each subject matrix, we calculated node-wise, local graph metrics quantifying the centrality of a node within a network (local strength and betweenness centrality), its ability to transmit information at local level (local efficiency), its integration or segregation properties (clustering coefficient), and its overall influence in a network (Eigenvector centrality) (70). Similarly, for each subject we calculated three overall graph metrics: strength, efficiency and transitivity. These are called global graph metrics and provide information about the general topological properties of each subject's connectivity matrix. The first two (strength and efficiency) are calculated by averaging over all nodes, while transitivity is defined for the whole graph. The clustering coefficient of a node estimates how much a single node tends to aggregate, through edges (i.e., connections), with its nearest neighbors. Transitivity is a variant of the clustering coefficient in which a network-wide normalization strategy mitigates the influence of outliers (e.g., of nodes with very low number of connections) on the clustering coefficient. Local efficiency is a similar measure, which estimates, in each location within the graph, how well a node's neighbors can exchange information when the node itself is removed (i.e., it can also be thought of as a measure of resilience). All graph-theoretical measures were computed via the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (70) (https://sites.google.com/site/bctnet/). In addition, given that no consensus exist on thresholding strategies before computing graph-theoretical computations, we repeated the main analyses after thresholding all adjacency matrices at 50% density.

Statistical Analysis

All metrics were compared between groups (agoraphobia vs. healthy controls) through the non-parametric Mann-Whitney U test. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons across the number of nodes (100) using an FDR approach at p < 0.05. Whenever a significant group effect was found (p < 0.05, FDR-corrected), we separately tested the interaction of agoraphobia and anxiety-related personality traits (neuroticism and introversion) using a general linear model which included group, trait and group*trait interaction. Additionally, whenever a significant group effect was found in local graph metrics in a certain node (p < 0.05, corrected), we separately ranked the adjacency matrix elements for the agoraphobic and control group in order to identify, for each group, nodes with greatest overall connectivity to the rest of the brain.

Results

Globally, we found lower clustering coefficient, efficiency and transitivity in the agoraphobic group compared to the matched control group (p = 0.01, 0.01, and 0.02, respectively, Mann-Whitney U test) (Figure 2). These results were confirmed when using matrices thresholded at 50% density. In this case, the p-values resulting from the Mann-Whitney U tests are p = 0.014; 0.018; 0.010 for the comparison between global clustering coefficient, efficiency and transitivity of the two groups, respectively. We found two separate networks (ICA components) with significant lower local clustering coefficient and efficiency in subjects with agoraphobic symptoms vs. matched controls. These extended across multiple cortical regions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Global clustering coefficient, efficiency, and transitivity were lower in patients with subclinical agoraphobic compared to controls. Figures show the median value (line), quartiles (boxes) and extremes (whiskers) of the three metrics for the agoraphobic and control groups. Comparison between the two subjects groups were performed through non-parametric Mann-Whitney U tests.

The first of these composite networks spanned visual, vestibular, motor, navigation and emotion processing areas (component 23, depicted in red in Figure 3A). The clustering coefficient (pcorr = 0.0002) and efficiency (pcorr = 0.0003) of this network were both lower in the agoraphobic than control groups (Figures 3B,C). This component contained a small area at the interface between V1, V2, and MST and extended anteriorly into the ventral visual stream in the fundus of the superior temporal sulcus (FST), basal parietal regions (area parietalis basalis in limine temporali PHT, PH) and posterior area temporalis proper (TE1p, TE2p) [Von Economo nomenclature (71)]. Medially, it encompassed the parieto-occipital sulcus POS1 in the precuneus on the left. Additional parietal regions included the posterior and superior angular gyrus (PGp andPGs) in the caudal inferior-parietal lobe (cIPL); the supramarginal gyrus (PF), PF opercularis (PFop), and tenuicorticalis (PFt) in the rostral IPL and lateral, medial and ventral intra-parietal area (LIP,MIP, and VIP) in the superior parietal lobe (SPL); and area 3a. The posterior insula was included on the right (PoI1-2). This composite network extended into the rostral and ventral parts of area BA 6 (6r and 6v) in the pre-motor cortex and into the frontal operculum (Fop2) and inferior frontal sulcus (IFS) in the pre-frontal cortex. Subcortical structures, specifically the amygdala, head of the caudate nucleus, putamen, and anterior thalamus also were part of the network. We termed this component the “visuospatial-emotional network” because it encompassed brain regions that integrate incoming multi-modality space and motion information (V1/V2, ventral visual stream, cIPL, and posterior insula) (72, 73), areas that process emotions (amygdala and associated subcortical nuclei), and regions that initiate/inhibit movements (pre-motor and pre-frontal cortices).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) The visuospatial-emotional network [node 23, shown in red on the inflated brain (66)] and vestibular-navigational network (node 41, in green) showed reduced clustering coefficients in patients with subclinical agoraphobic symptoms compared to the control group. The visuospatial-emotional network also had a lower efficiency subjects with agoraphobic symptoms. ICAs include values higher than the 98'th percentile. (B) The median value (line), quartiles (boxes), and extremes (whiskers) of the local metrics for both nodes are shown. The comparisons between the two subject groups were performed using non-parametric Mann-Whitney U tests. P-values were corrected for the number of nodes (100) using FDR (p-value** < 0.01). (C) Examples of networks in which local efficiency and clustering coefficient vary as a function of connections.

The second composite network encompassed the left posterior insula, retroinsula, and granular insula, and bilateral parietal operculum (OP1,2,3,4), primary somatosensory and motor cortex (BA 1, 2, 3b, 4), pre-motor cortex (6d), medial SPL, cingulate cortex (5m, 5L, 24), and rostral hippocampus (component 41, depicted in green in Figure 3A). In this network, only the clustering coefficient (pcorr = 0.0009) was lower in subjects with agoraphobic symptoms compared to controls (Figure 3B). We named this component the “vestibular-navigational network” because it linked the principal vestibular cortical regions and the hippocampus to somatosensory and motor cortices. In summary the visuospatial-emotional and vestibular-navigational networks both extended across parietal, insular and pre-frontal cortices of the brain.

These networks are mostly connected to three common components (in yellow in Figure 4) plus additional components that spread again over the parietal, insular and pre-frontal cortices (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) The visuospatial-emotional network centered about ICA 23 (in green) and the vestibular-navigational network centered about ICA 41 (in red) are superimposed on flat brain (66). The visuospatial-emotional network was strongly connected with regions 18, 8, 7, whereas the vestibular-navigational network was connected with regions 43, 75, 27. The two networks overlapped at nodes 34, 35, and 14 (in yellow). (B) The shared nodes are depicted again superimposed on the inflated brain (66). For visualization purposes we only reported the 6 nodes most closely connected to ICA 23 or 41. ICAs include values higher than the 98'th percentile.

These results were confirmed when using matrices thresholded at 50% density. In this case, individuals with subclinical agoraphobia were found to have lower clustering coefficient (pcorr = 0.023) and local efficiency (pcorr = 0.023) in visuospatial-emotional network (node number 23) as compared to controls. In addition, in subclinical agoraphobia the local clustering coefficient was lower (as compared to controls, pcorr = 0.026) also in the vestibular-navigational network (node 41).

We did not find a main effect of introversion (all p's > 0.6) or neuroticism (all p's > 0.1) on graph theoretical-metrics. In the visuospatial-emotional network, however, introversion interacted significantly with the main effect of agoraphobia to reduce the clustering coefficient and efficiency of subjects with agoraphobic symptoms compared to controls (F = 7.9, p = 0.006 and F = 7.0, p = 0.009, respectively) (Figure 5). Thus, individuals with agoraphobic symptoms and introverted personality traits had the lowest connectivity metrics. Introversion did not interact significantly with agoraphobia in the vestibular-navigational network. Neuroticism had no effect on either network.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Results of the interaction analysis of agoraphobic group and introversion, where red and black symbols refer to the agoraphobic and control groups, respectively. In node 23 both local efficiency (p = 0.009) and clustering coefficient (p = 0.006) were significantly affected by the interaction of agoraphobic group and introversion.

These results were confirmed when using matrices thresholded at 50% density. Also in this case, in the visuospatial-emotional network both local efficiency (p = 0.012) and clustering coefficient (p = 0.009) were significantly affected by the interaction of agoraphobic group and introversion.

Discussion

In this study, we were able to utilize the detailed information of the HCP database to identify a group of 52 subjects with sub-clinical agoraphobic symptoms and match them closely to a control group of 52 subjects across a large number of potential confounds. Critically, matching was done not only for mean values of potentially confounding variables, but also for their variances. These strict criteria enabled us to study the neural correlates of agoraphobic symptoms far more independently of confounding influences than previous studies of agoraphobia and PPPD. In the best controlled investigations of PPPD published to date, for example, patient and control groups were matched for mean scores of measures of state anxiety and depression and five personality factors (39, 40, 45). We also were able to exploit the image quality of the HCP database to extract a high granularity yet robust decomposition of brain nodes and networks through group-ICA (dimensionality 100).

First, we found that agoraphobic symptoms were related to lower global efficiency, clustering coefficient and transitivity, reflecting overall lower integrative functioning across the entire brain. Second, through analyses of local, component-wise effects, we identified two networks which, when comparing patients with agoraphobic symptoms to healthy controls, scored lower in at least one connectivity measure. The visuospatial-emotional network included portions of the primary visual cortex, ventral visual stream, multiple regions of the parietal lobe, and parts of the pre-motor and pre-frontal cortices as well as the anterior thalamus, basal ganglia, and amygdala. This network would be well-positioned to process incoming visual stimuli (occipital lobe and visual stream), link them to vestibular and somatosensory inputs (parietal association areas and posterior insula), and use this information to plan and initiate or inhibit locomotor commands (pre-motor and pre-frontal regions) in response to desires and threats in the environment (amygdala and associated subcortical structures). However, the lower level of efficiency and clustering of this network suggests that patients with sub-clinical agoraphobic symptoms may not incorporate all of this information into high level management of their behaviors, but perhaps react more instinctively to agoraphobic stimuli. The fact that introversion further reduced efficiency and clustering of this network raises the possibility that individuals with this personality trait are even more strongly driven by innately determined reactions to agoraphobic stimuli.

The vestibular-navigational network encompassed core regions of the multimodal vestibular cortex in the parietal operculum bilaterally plus areas of the primary somatosensory cortex, motor cortex, pre-motor cortex, cingulate and hippocampus. This suggests a pre-dominant role on the output side of locomotor control. The vestibular-navigational network is well-positioned to control (motor and pre-motor cortices) and monitor (somatosensory and vestibular cortices) movements in space (hippocampus) and compare intended to actual outcomes (cingulate cortex). The lower level of clustering of this network corroborates the fact that agoraphobic stimuli are salient for networks that are dedicated to control of movement in space.

Spatial and navigation systems are characterized by a high degree of redundancy. Detection of movement of self is supported by the multimodal nature of the vestibular cortex in which vestibular signals, somatosensory inputs, and optic flow all provide information about self-motion (74). Detection of movement in the environment is much more dependent on visual information (hearing plays a lesser role in humans), but vestibular and somatosensory inputs are necessary to stabilize the eyes on targets of interest. Navigation is supported by neurons that are sensitive to the direction and speed of self-motion. These are redundantly located not only in the hippocampal formation and entorhinal cortex, but also across the parietal cortex and subcortical structures (73). Thus, reductions in integrative functioning of the visuospatial-emotional and vestibular-navigational networks in individuals with self-reported agoraphobic symptoms does not preclude adequate processing of visual space-motion data or acceptable control of movement but indicates that these networks may weigh data that they process differently than normal. One example is visual dependency, i.e., the tendency to rely more strongly on visual than vestibular or somatosensory information for spatial orientation. This over-reliance on visual inputs has been reported in patients with agoraphobia, perhaps because vision can detect threats at a distance whereas vestibular and somatosensory systems require contact with the body. Regardless, over-weighting of visual information may cause spatial disorientation in environments with complex patterns or multiple moving objects (75).

The visuospatial-emotional and vestibular-navigational networks identified in patients with sub-clinical agoraphobia have comparable, though not identical, counterparts in patients with PPPD. Studies using sound-evoked vestibular stimuli found reduced activity and connectivity in a network encompassing the parietal opercula, posterior insula, posterior superior temporal sulcus, superior parietal cortex and motor vestibular regions in patients with chronic subjective dizziness, a PPPD precursor, compared to healthy controls (38). This is roughly analogous to the vestibular-navigational network identified in this study of patients with agoraphobic symptoms. In addition to the overlapping brain structures that comprised these networks, neither was influenced by neuroticism or introversion. Investigations employing visual motion stimuli in patients with PPPD identified a link between visual (V3 and middle occipital gyrus) and frontal regions (inferior frontal gyrus/anterior insula) (40), but this was more limited than the visuospatial-emotional network found in patients with agoraphobic symptoms in that it did not extend into the parietal lobe, posterior insula or pre-motor areas. Furthermore, neuroticism was associated with increased connectivity in the visual-frontal network of patients with PPPD (45) whereas introversion interacted with agoraphobic symptoms to decrease connectivity in the visuospatial-emotional network of patients with subclinical agoraphobia. Interestingly, neuroimaging findings in a study of patients with phobic postural vertigo (i.e., individuals with PPPD plus substantial phobic/avoidant symptoms) were centered about vestibular cortical regions of the parietal operculum and insula, but also extended into the motor cortex, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate cortices (44). Those results were not adjusted for state anxiety though half of the patients were too anxious to tolerate the confined spaces of the MRI scanner, suggesting that the associated phobic avoidant symptoms rather than core elements of PPPD were responsible for the broad changes in brain structure and function.

Taken together with previous neuroimaging work on PPPD and its precursors (3845), the results of this investigation provide information about the architecture of brain networks subserving spatial orientation, control of locomotion, and threat assessment as they relate to PPPD and agoraphobic symptoms. They indicate that Westphal's observation that one process linked alterations in sense of space, control of movement in motion-rich environments, and instinctive fear reactions (1) was not an error but may involve two linked networks in the brain. At the same time, they also suggest that Westphal's agoraphobia had two components that reflect the contemporary separation of agoraphobia and PPPD into separate clinical entities. The first component involves the detection and processing of afferent visual space-motion information and any threats contained therein, plus initiation or inhibition of behavioral responses. The brain network underlying these functions is less well-connected in patients with agoraphobic symptoms than in normal individuals, an effect that is amplified by the severity of introversion. In contrast, an overlapping, but narrower network of visual-frontal regions shows greater connectivity in patients with PPPD compared to normal controls in proportion to the severity of neuroticism (45). With regard to this component, patients with phobic postural vertigo (i.e., PPPD plus phobic/avoidance symptoms) seem to possess alterations in brain functioning that are closer to agoraphobia than PPPD alone (44). The second component involves the motor control and monitoring of locomotion. The brain network underlying these functions is similar in patients with subclinical agoraphobia and PPPD. It is less well-connected in both disorders, but not affected by anxiety-related personality traits in either one.

Limitations of the Study

The investigation of a cohort of individuals with subclinical agoraphobic symptoms may be considered a strength and a weakness of study design. On the positive side, we were able to study the neural correlates of visual space-motion processing in our subjects without the potential confounds of serious panic/phobic symptoms. On the negative side, our findings are limited to sub-threshold symptoms and may not apply to patients with fully developed diagnoses of agoraphobia. This limitation is mitigated by the observation of other investigators that agoraphobic symptoms lie on a continuum from normal to pathological levels (50); that is, patients with the clinical diagnosis of agoraphobia have quantitatively, not qualitatively, different symptoms than individuals with subclinical presentations. Thus, we likely investigated one end of a continuous spectrum, not a distinct entity. In the HCP database, agoraphobia was evaluated through the SSAGA interview; however, we did not have access to the complete results of the interview, which examines autonomic and vestibular symptoms associated with avoidance in detail. Thus, our findings may be limited by the extent of symptoms that were available to us to characterize our agoraphobic group.

Data Availability

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: http://www.humanconnectome.org/.

Ethics Statement

Data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657).

Author Contributions

II and NT managed the overall project (conceptualization and methodology). AC performed graph metric calculations. AC and NT performed statistical analysis. II, AC, and NT prepared the original draft, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JS, FL, and LP provided methodological perspective on the study, critically reviewed, and edited the initial manuscript and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors contributed to manuscript revision and editing.

Funding

Data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. Research supported by the Italian Ministry of Health (PE-2013-02355372 and IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia Ricerca corrente) and by the U.S. Department of Defense Congressionally Directed Medical Research Program W81XWH1810760 PT170028. LP was funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC) (MR/P01271X/1) at the University of Cambridge.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Westphal C. Die Agoraphobie, eine neuropathische Erscheinung. Zeitschrift für Psychiatr. (1871) 3:138–61. doi: 10.1007/BF02156040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. McCabe BF. Diseases of the end organ and vestibular nerve. In: Naunton RF, editor. The Vestibular System. New York, NY: Academic Press (1975). p. 299–302. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-514950-1.50027-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Marks IM, Bebbington P. Space phobia: syndrome or agoraphobic variant? Br Med J. (1976) 2:345–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.6031.345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Brandt T, Dieterich M. Phobischer Attacken Schwankschwindel, ein neues Syndrom? Munch Med Wschr. (1986) 28:247–50.

Google Scholar

6. Jacob RG, Lilienfeld SO, Furman JMR, Durrant JD, Turner SM. Panic disorder with vestibular dysfunc- tion: Further clinical observation and description of space and motion phobic stimuli. J Anxiety Disorders. (1989) 3:117–30. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(89)90006-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Staab JP, Ruckenstein MJ. Expanding the differen- tial diagnosis of dizziness. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2007) 133:170–6. doi: 10.1001/archotol.133.2.170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Staab JP, Eckhardt-Henn A, Horii A, Jacob R, Strupp M, Brandt T, et al. Diagnostic criteria for persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD): Consensus document of the committee for the Classification of Vestibular Disorders of the Bárány Society. J Vestib Res Equilib Orientat. (2017) 27:191–208. doi: 10.3233/VES-170622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases. 11th Edition for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics. Available online at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en (accessed July 31, 2019).

Google Scholar

10. Balaban CD, Jacob RG. Background and history of the interface between anxiety and vertigo. J Anxiety Disorders. (2001) 15:27–51. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(00)00041-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bienvenu OJ, Hettema JM, Neale MC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Low extraversion and high neuroticism as indices of genetic and environmental risk for social phobia, agoraphobia, and animal phobia. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:1714–21. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06101667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Staab JP, Rohe DE, Eggers SD, Shepard NT. Anxious, introverted personality traits in patients with chronic subjective dizziness. J Psychosom Res. (2014) 76:80–3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Chiarella G, Petrolo C, Riccelli R, Giofrè L, Olivadese G, Gioacchini FM, et al. Chronic subjective dizziness: analysis of underlying personality factors. J Vestib Res. (2016) 26:403–8. doi: 10.3233/VES-160590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Yan Z, Cui L, Yu T, Liang H, Wang Y, Chen CC. Analysis of the characteristics of persistent postural- perceptual dizziness: a clinical-based study in China. Int J Audiol. (2016) 56:1–5. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2016.1211763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Staab JP, Ruckenstein MJ. Which comes first? Psy- chogenic dizziness versus otogenic anxiety. Laryngoscope. (2003) 113:1714–8. doi: 10.1097/00005537-200310000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Yardley L, Britton J, Lear S, Bird J, Luxon LM. Relationship between balance system function and agoraphobic avoidance. Behav Res Ther. (1995) 33:435–9. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00060-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Jacob RG, Furman JM, Durrant JD, Turner SM. Panic, agoraphobia, and vestibular dysfunction. Am J Psychiatry. (1996) 153:503–12. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.4.503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Perna G, Dario A, Caldirola D, Stefania B, Cesarani A, Bellodi L. Panic disorder: the role of the balance system. J Psychiatr Res. (2001) 35:279–86. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00031-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Jacob RG, Redfern MS, Furman JM. Space and motion discomfort and abnormal balance control in patients with anxiety disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2009) 80:74–8. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.136432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Jacob RG, Furman JM, Durrant JD, Turner SM. Surface dependence: a balance control strategy in panic disorder with agoraphobia. Psychosom Med. (1997) 59:323–30. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199705000-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hoffman DL, O'Leary DP, Munjack DJ. Autorotation test abnormalities of the horizontal and vertical vestibulo-ocular reflexes in panic disorder. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (1994) 110:259–69. doi: 10.1016/S0194-5998(94)70768-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Staab JP. Chronic subjective dizziness, Continuum: Life- long Learning in Neurology (Minneap Minn). Neuro Otol. (2012) 18:1118–41. doi: 10.1212/01.CON.0000421622.56525.58

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Perna G, Alpini D, Caldirola D, Raponi G, Cesarani A, Bellodi L. Serotonergic modulation of the balance system in panic disorder: an open study. Depress Anxiety. (2003) 17:101–6. doi: 10.1002/da.10092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Popkirov S, Staab JP, Stone J. Persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD): a common, characteristic and treatable cause of chronic dizziness. Pract Neurol. (2018) 18:5–13. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2017-001809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Staab JP, Balaban CD, Furman JM. Threat assessment and locomotion: clinical applications of an integrated model of anxiety and postural control. Semin Neurol. (2013) 33:297–306. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1356462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Indovina I, Riccelli R, Staab JP, Lacquaniti F, Passamonti L. Personality traits modulate subcortical and cortical vestibular and anxiety responses to sound-evoked otolithic receptor stimulation. J Psychosom Res. (2014) 77:391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Riccelli R, Indovina I, Staab JP, Nigro S, Augimeri A, Lacquaniti F, et al. Neuroticism modulates brain visuo-vestibular and anxiety systems during a virtual rollercoaster task. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 38:715–26. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lopez C, Blanke O, Mast FW. The human vestibular cortex revealed by coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Neuroscience. (2012) 212:159–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. zu Eulenburg P, Caspers S, Roski C, Eickhoff SB. Meta-analytical definition and functional connectivity of the human vestibular cortex. NeuroImage. (2012) 60:162–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Indovina I, Maffei V, Pauwels K, Macaluso E, Orban GA, Lacquaniti F. Simulated self-motion in a visual gravity field: Sensitivity to vertical and horizontal heading in the human brain. Neuroimage. (2013) 71:114–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lacquaniti F, Bosco G, Gravano S, Indovina I, La Scaleia B, Maffei V, et al. Multisensory integration and internal models for sensing gravity effects in primates. Biomed Res Int. (2014) 2014:615854. doi: 10.1155/2014/615854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Maffei V, Indovina I, Macaluso E, Ivanenko YP, A Orban G, Lacquaniti F. Visual gravity cues in the interpretation of biological movements: neural correlates in humans. Neuroimage. (2015) 104:221–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Balaban CD. Projections from the parabrachial nucleus to the vestibular nuclei: potential substrates for autonomic and limbic influences on vestibular responses. Brain Res. (2004) 996:126–37. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2003.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Balaban CD. Neural substrates linking balance control and anxiety. Physiol Behav. (2002) 77:469–75. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00935-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Balaban CD, Thayer JF. Neurological bases for balance-anxiety links. J Anxiety Disord. (2001) 15:53–79. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(00)00042-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Craig ADB. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Indovina I, Robbins TW, Núñez-Elizalde AO, Dunn BD, Bishop SJ. Fear-conditioning mechanisms associated with trait vulnerability to anxiety in humans. Neuron. (2011) 69:563–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Indovina I, Riccelli R, Chiarella G, Petrolo C, Augimeri A, Giofrè L, et al. Role of the Insula and vestibular system in patients with chronic subjective dizziness: an fMRI Study Using Sound-Evoked Vestibular Stimulation. Front Behav Neurosci. (2015) 9:334. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Nigro S, Indovina I, Riccelli R, Chiarella G, Petrolo C, Lacquaniti F, et al. Reduced cortical folding in multi-modal vestibular regions in persistent postural perceptual dizziness. Brain Imaging Behav. (2018) 13:798–809. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9900-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Riccelli R, Passamonti L, Toschi N, Nigro S, Chiarella G, Petrolo C, et al. Indovina Altered I insular and occipital responses to simulated vertical self-motion in patients with persistent postural-perceptual dizziness. Front Neurol. (2017) 8:529. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lee J-O, Lee E-S, Kim J-S, Lee Y-B, Jeong Y, Choi BS, et al. Altered brain function in persistent postural perceptual dizziness: A study on resting state functional connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:3340–53. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Na S, Im JJ, Jeong H, Lee E-S, Lee T-K, Chung Y-A, et al. Cerebral perfusion abnormalities in patients with persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD): a SPECT study. J Neural Transm Vienna Austria. (2019) 126:123–9. doi: 10.1007/s00702-018-1948-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wurthmann S, Naegel S, Schulte Steinberg B, Theysohn N, Diener H-C, Kleinschnitz C, et al. Cerebral gray matter changes in persistent postural perceptual dizziness. J Psychosom Res. (2017) 103:95–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Popp P, Zu Eulenburg P, Stephan T, Bögle R, Habs M, Henningsen P, et al. Cortical alterations in phobic postural vertigo - a multimodal imaging approach. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. (2018) 5:717–29. doi: 10.1002/acn3.570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Passamonti L, Riccelli R, Lacquaniti F, Staab JP, Indovina I. Brain responses to virtual reality visual motion stimulation are affected by neurotic personality traits in patients with persistent postural-perceptual dizziness. J Vestib Res. Equilib Orientat. (2019) 28:369–78. doi: 10.3233/VES-190653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Damsa C, Kosel M, Moussally J. Current status of brain imaging in anxiety disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2009) 22:96–110. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328319bd10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ferrari MCF, Busatto GF, McGuire PK, Crippa JAS. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in anxiety disorders: an update of research findings. Rev Bras Psiquiatr São Paulo Braz. (2008) 30:251–64. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462008000300013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Engel KR, Obst K, Bandelow B, Dechent P, Gruber O, Zerr I, et al. Functional MRI activation in response to panic-specific, non-panic aversive, and neutral pictures in patients with panic disorder and healthy controls. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2016) 266:557–66. doi: 10.1007/s00406-015-0653-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Wittmann A, Schlagenhauf F, Guhn A, Lueken U, Gaehlsdorf C, Stoy M, et al. Anticipating agoraphobic situations: the neural correlates of panic disorder with agoraphobia. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:2385–96. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713003085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Besteher B, Squarcina L, Spalthoff R, Bellani M, Gaser C, Nenadić I, et al. Subclinical agoraphobia symptoms and regional brain volumes in non-clinical subjects: between compensation and resilience? Front Psychiatry. (2018) 9: 541. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TEJ, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, et al. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. Neuroimage. (2013) 80:62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Dieterich M, Bense S, Lutz S, Drzezga A, Stephan T, Bartenstein P, et al. Dominance for vestibular cortical function in the non-dominant hemisphere. Cereb Cortex N Y N. (2003) 13:994–1007. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.9.994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI, et al. A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA. J Stud Alcohol. (1994) 55:149–58. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Schalet BD, Pilkonis PA, Yu L, Dodds N, Johnston KL, Yount S, et al. Clinical validity of PROMIS Depression, Anxiety, and Anger across diverse clinical samples. J Clin Epidemiol. (2016) 73:119–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Clark LA, Watson D. Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. J Abnorm Psychol. (1991) 100:316–36. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.100.3.316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lee S-A, Kim K-H, Cho S-M. Validation of the mood and anxiety symptom questionnaire in Korean adolescents. Psychiatry Invest. (2015) 12:218–26. doi: 10.4306/pi.2015.12.2.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Pilkonis PA, Yu L, Dodds NE, Johnston KL, Maihoefer CC, Lawrence SM. Validation of the depression item bank from the Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) in a three-month observational study. J Psychiatr Res. (2014) 56:112–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR. Stability and change in personality assessment: the revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. J Pers Assess. (1997) 68:86–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6801_7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. (1983) 24:385–96. doi: 10.2307/2136404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Smith SM, Beckmann CF, Andersson J, Auerbach EJ, Bijsterbosch J, Douaud G, et al. Resting-state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. (2013) 80:144–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. (2013) 80:105–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Salimi-Khorshidi G, Douaud G, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Griffanti L, Smith SM. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. Neuroimage. (2014) 90:449–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, et al. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. Neuroimage. (2014) 95:232–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Robinson EC, Jbabdi S, Glasser MF, Andersson J, Burgess GC, Harms MP, et al. MSM: a new flexible framework for Multimodal Surface Matching. Neuroimage. (2014) 100:414–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Robinson EC, Hacker CD, Harwell J, Yacoub E, et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. (2016) 536:171–8. doi: 10.1038/nature18933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (2004) 23:137–52. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Smith SM, Hyvärinen A, Varoquaux G, Miller KL, Beckmann CF. Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. Neuroimage. (2014) 101:738–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Toschi N, Duggento A, Passamonti L. Functional connectivity in amygdalar-sensory/(pre)motor networks at rest: new evidence from the Human Connectome Project. Eur J Neurosci. (2017) 45:1224–9. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. (2010) 52:1059–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. von Economo C, Koskinas GN. Chapter 2: Atlas of cytoarchitectonics of the adult human cerebral cortex. In: Geyer S, Turner R, editors. Microstructural Parcellation of the Human Cerebral Cortex. Basel: Karger (2008). p. 33–53.

Google Scholar

72. Kravitz DJ, Saleem KS, Baker CI, Mishkin M. A new neural framework for visuospatial processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:217–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn3008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Indovina I, Maffei V, Mazzarella E, Sulpizio V, Galati G, Lacquaniti F. Path integration in 3D from visual motion cues: a human fMRI study. Neuroimage. (2016) 142:512–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Britten KH. Mechanisms of self-motion perception. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2008) 31:389–410. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Redfern MS, Furman JM, Jacob RG. Visually induced postural sway in anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. (2007) 21:704–16. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: agoraphobia, persistent postural perceptual dizziness, vestibular network, functional connectivity, resting state fMRI, graphs theory

Citation: Indovina I, Conti A, Lacquaniti F, Staab JP, Passamonti L and Toschi N (2019) Lower Functional Connectivity in Vestibular-Limbic Networks in Individuals With Subclinical Agoraphobia. Front. Neurol. 10:874. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00874

Received: 30 April 2019; Accepted: 26 July 2019;
Published: 13 August 2019.

Edited by:

Roberto Esposito, A.O. Ospedali Riuniti Marche Nord, Italy

Reviewed by:

Liang Zhan, University of Pittsburgh, United States
Hui Shen, National University of Defense Technology, China

Copyright © 2019 Indovina, Conti, Lacquaniti, Staab, Passamonti and Toschi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Iole Indovina, iole.indovina@unicamillus.it; Luca Passamonti, lp337@medschl.cam.ac.uk

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.