Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 06 March 2020
Sec. Multiple Sclerosis and Neuroimmunology
This article is part of the Research Topic Corticospinal Excitability in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis View all 10 articles

Cognitive Impairment and Brain Reorganization in MS: Underlying Mechanisms and the Role of Neurorehabilitation

  • 1Department of Speech and Language Therapy, School of Health Sciences, University of Ioannina, Ioannina, Greece
  • 2Department of Neurology, The Multiple Sclerosis Center, AHEPA University Hospital, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
  • 3Neuropsychology Section, Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, University of Patras Medical School, Patras, Greece

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic, immune-mediated, inflammatory, and degenerative disease of the central nervous system (CNS) that affects both white and gray matter. Various mechanisms throughout its course, mainly regarding gray matter lesions and brain atrophy, result in cognitive network dysfunction and can cause clinically significant cognitive impairment in roughly half the persons living with MS. Altered cognition is responsible for many negative aspects of patients' lives, independently of physical disability, such as higher unemployment and divorce rates, reduced social activities, and an overall decrease in quality of life. Despite its devastating impact it is not included in clinical ratings and decision making in the way it should be. It is interesting that only half the persons with MS exhibit cognitive dysfunction, as this implies that the other half remain cognitively intact. It appears that a dynamic balance between brain destruction and brain reorganization is taking place. This balance acts in favor of keeping brain systems functioning effectively, but this is not so in all cases, and the effect does not last forever. When these systems collapse, functional brain reorganization is not effective anymore, and clinically apparent impairments are evident. It is therefore important to reveal which factors could make provision for the subpopulation of patients in whom cognitive impairment occurs. Even if we manage to detect this subpopulation earlier, effective pharmaceutical treatments will still be lacking. Nevertheless, recent evidence shows that cognitive rehabilitation and neuromodulation, using non-invasive techniques such as transcranial magnetic or direct current stimulation, could be effective in cognitively impaired patients with MS. In this Mini Review, we discuss the mechanisms underlying cognitive impairment in MS. We also focus on mechanisms of reorganization of cognitive networks, which occur throughout the disease course. Finally, we review theoretical and practical issues of neurorehabilitation and neuromodulation for cognition in MS as well as factors that influence them and prevent them from being widely applied in clinical settings.

Introduction

People living with Multiple sclerosis (pwMS) commonly exhibit cognitive deficits, which negatively affects them multidimensionally (1). Daily functioning, decision making, vocational activities, marital status, socialization, behavior, mood, balance and mobility, and compliance with medications can be affected. The medical community, due to their often subtle nature and the difficulty that exists in detecting these deficits during routine clinical practice, was initially slow to appreciate them as a core clinical symptom of MS. We recently proposed a practical algorithm for clinicians regarding “what,” “why,” “how,” and “when” to measure (2). Today, most of the evidence suggests that cognitive impairment in MS patients is present during all disease stages and across all disease clinical subtypes (35). CI can be detected even prior to diagnosis (6) in radiologically isolated syndrome (RIS) (7), clinically isolated syndrome (CIS) (8), “benign” MS (9), and the pediatric MS population (10). Deficits appear to be more frequent and widespread in the progressive rather than the relapsing form of the disease (1113). Even then, roughly half of the patients do not exhibit prominent CI. CI is also linked to disease duration (12, 14), tissue damage and atrophy (1517), and cognitive network efficiency (18, 19). Some cognitive domains appear to be more commonly compromised than others: information processing efficiency, episodic memory, attention, and executive functioning are found predominantly to be detrimentally affected in MS (20, 21). Among these domains the most common pattern involves circumscribed deficits as a combination of one or two of the abovementioned domains (e.g., attention/processing speed, learning/ memory, and or executive functions).

On the other hand, social cognitive deficits are an underestimated but important aspect of impairment in MS, reflecting how people process, store, and apply information in social interactions. Deficits in these domains have been associated with reduced quality of life, even after controlling for severity and duration of the disease, age, and neurocognitive performance (22, 23). This type of impairment is not entirely dependent on and parallel to general cognitive dysfunction—some patients experience disorganization in their social life before significant or detectable cognitive impairment is evident. The decrease in performance of social cognition (SC) tasks may reflect changes in brain activity and brain structure, either general or regional (22, 24).

In order to answer the question of why half of pwMS do not exhibit CI, approaching the disease within the context of its trilateral interference of tissue damage, tissue repair, and brain reorganization (25) may be helpful: tissue damage is indeed a matter of time and disease type and severity, and it can be partially influenced by the early introduction of efficacious disease modifying therapies (DMTs). Tissue repair is served by various mechanisms that are not yet well-illuminated, may vary in affected individuals, is altered by factors such as co-morbidity, stress, or lifestyle, and, unfortunately, was not targeted with specific medications until now; functional brain reorganization, in other words neuroplasticity, is the intrinsic force fighting the consequences of disease progression and can hopefully be managed through neurorehabilitation interventions.

In the following sections we have addressed issues concerning CI and functional brain reorganization in MS; we focused on cognitive network alterations, efficiency, and collapse, the role of inflammation, and mechanisms underlying synaptopathy and synaptogenesis. We further discussed the potential role of cognitive rehabilitation and neuromodulation in retaining and enhancing network efficiency in a clinically meaningful way.

Networks-Connectivity-Brain Reorganization

Brain reorganization in MS is studied intensively by mainly functional neuro-imaging methods. Functional connectivity (FC) at rest and during tasks can detect both hyperconnectivity and hypoconnectivity in brain networks. This can compensate for tissue damage, allowing pwMS to adequately cope with everyday cognitive tasks despite continuing structural brain damage. These alterations can be adaptive or maladaptive. We recently summarized the basic concepts, and limitations, of functional brain reorganization in MS (26). Even from early disease phases—in patients with CIS—dynamic changes in functional brain networks have been observed, resulting in the maintenance of normal efficiency in the brain and consequently representing a compensatory effect (27). A mixed pattern of hypoactivity and hyperactivity was found, by means of rs-fMRI, in pwMS at different stages of disease. Relapsing Remitting Multiple Sclerosis (RRMS) individuals with short disease duration, and RRMS with similar disabilities but longer disease duration, were characterized by a clearly distinct pattern of FC that involved predominantly sensory and cognitive networks, respectively (28). In a longitudinal 1-year network connectivity study, measures were compared between early RRMS patients and healthy matched controls as well as between patients with and without disease activity (29). The study reported that the strengthening of local network properties was only detectable in the cortex of patients and occurred independently of their disease activity. Authors discuss these changes as an adaptive mechanism that is important for maintaining brain function in response to neuroinflammation. In another study, patients who converted to MS exhibited significantly greater network connectivity at baseline than non-converters (30). Cader et al. concluded that both forms of adaptive functional change—that is, the enhancement of interactions between brain regions normally recruited, and the recruitment of alternative areas, or the use of complementary cognitive strategies—could limit clinical expression of the disease, particularly that of CI (31). As a rule, PwMS perform better in cognitive tasks if they have preserved fMRI activity of their frontal lobes (32). Not only functional but also structural connectivity matters. Llufriu et al. investigating reorganization mechanisms at the structural level that are related to attention and executive performance in pwMS, and they found that the right pallidum and left insula within the frame of the brain's reorganization functioned as hubs in patients (33). However, we must keep in mind that several limitations exist that relate to the role of altered connectivity throughout the disease; it is still questionable whether the observed changes are relevant to cognitive performance and whether or not they are adaptive (26). As the disease progresses, network efficiency is challenged by tissue damage, restorative mechanisms become inadequate, and, finally, the network collapses (18).

Regional Tissue Damage and Atrophy

Gray matter (GM) lesions (15) and GM atrophy (16) play an important role in CI. Across the disease span, if left untreated, the white matter atrophy rate remains rather stable at 3-fold normal, but GM atrophy rate dramatically increases from 3.4-fold normal in CIS to 14-fold normal in SPMS (17). This localized GM atrophy has recently been found to be regionally selective, mainly involving deep structures, such as the thalamus, putamen, and caudate, and cortical regions, such as the sensorimotor cortex, insula, superior temporal, and cingulate gyrus, while these regions were functionally connected (34). In a 5-years follow-up study, it was shown that structural damage and especially cortical atrophy may predict cognitive decline in PwMS (35). Among strategic GM structures, the thalamus, basal ganglia, and hippocampus seem to play a central role. Thalamic volume declines faster in pwMS throughout the disease, and it was proposed to serve as a biomarker of degeneration (36). Its volume, shape, and function are related to cognitive performance in MS (3739). In early RRMS patients (duration of disease <3 years), CI was detected in 28% over a 2-years follow up period, and in this subgroup a significant reduction in the percentage of thalamus volume was observed compared with the cognitively intact group (40). In a large cohort of MS patients, with various forms and stages of the disease, Rocca et al. investigated rs-FC abnormalities within the principal brain networks in PwMS (41). They found a complex pattern of decreased and increased rs-FC at a regional level: reduced thalamic rs-FC correlated with better neuropsychological performance, whereas, for all the remaining networks, reduced FC correlated with more severe clinical/cognitive impairment. This finding was in line with the observation of Zhou et al. who found that increased thalamic intrinsic oscillation amplitude in RRMS patients was associated with slowed cognitive processing, representing ineffective reorganization (42). Resting-state magneto-encephalography recordings from pwMS and healthy controls offered similar evidence, illustrating “the relationship between thalamic atrophy, altered functional connectivity and clinical and cognitive dysfunction in MS” (43). The importance of thalamic involvement in disease progression and CI is highlighted by Minagar et al. (44), who recommended that thalamic volume should be utilized as a biomarker in MS clinical trials. In a recent study where neuropsychological and MRI data of 375 PwMS were analyzed, altered performance on neuropsychological tests assessing attention and executive function was associated with caudate volume and posterior cingulate/precuneus atrophy, while tests primarily evaluating memory strongly correlated with thalamic volume (45). In untreated CIS patients, load-dependent dysfunction of the putamen was related to impaired performance during attention tasks (46). Amygdala atrophy was found to be the main predictor of impairment of social cognition (SC) in PwMS (24). In agreement with this, Pitteri at al. correlated bilateral amygdala damage, as measured by cortical lesion volume (CLV), to affected SC in PwMS, even in the absence of CI (47). In a multicenter study of structural correlates of CI in MS, the best predictors of CI were found to be atrophy of the hippocampus and deep GM nuclei (48). The importance of structural and functional integrity of the hippocampus was highlighted by Sumowski et al. (49), who investigated the neural basis of reserve against memory decline in PwMS, linking greater intellectual enrichment and better memory to larger hippocampal volume and supporting the argument that larger hippocampal volume is a key component of reserve against memory decline in MS. The hippocampus of PwMS usually has a high lesion load, demyelination, neuronal damage, synaptic dysfunction, neurotransmitter level reduction, and disconnection, linking hippocampus pathology not only to CI but also to the reorganization capacity of broader networks (48, 5055). A biomarker indicating deep GM structures, especially thalamus and hippocampus volume and status, could ideally give provision of the cognitive status, insights for reorganization dynamics, and cues for therapeutic decisions.

Synaptopathy in MS

We formerly recognize MS as a myelin-targeting autoimmune disease of the CNS, causing inflammation, white (and gray) matter tissue damage, and neurodegeneration, but the influence of GM pathology was recognized later. Loss and malfunction of synapses could offer an explanation for this role. Recent clinical and experimental studies link inflammation to neurodegeneration, illuminating the contribution not only of visible structural damage but also of synaptic dysfunction in the pathophysiology of both motor and cognitive functions in MS. Many studies have provided robust evidence for diffuse synaptic dysfunction being present in both MS and EAE (experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis, the animal model of MS) throughout the disease course (5659). Stampanoni Bassi et al. and Mandolesi et al. have discussed thoroughly the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying alterations in synaptic function and structure (58, 59). They emphasized the role of inflammation in neurotransmitters' imbalance; increased glutamate-mediated and reduced GABA-mediated signaling along with the excitotoxic effects of increased glutamate levels in the synaptic cleft may lead to synaptic degeneration, which, interestingly, may occur independently of GM demyelination and neuronal loss. Of course, synaptopathy can also be the consequence of axonal damage, but it is present from the initial phase of the disease when one could not yet expect that much of axonal damage. This supports the idea that synaptopathy, rather than axonal loss, leads to accumulation of disability, at least early in the disease course. Focusing our attention to the synaptic level, we must keep in mind the role of long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) which represent core underlying mechanisms of synaptic plasticity, i.e., maintaining synaptic strengths, efficacy, and stability, adjusted dynamically by neural activity. The ways structural and functional damage result in synaptic failure, network dysfunction, and, therefore, CI have recently been reviewed by Di Filippo et al. (60).

Fortunately, and unlike the loss of neurons, the loss of synapses is reversible. New synapses can be generated, and dysfunctional synapses can be repaired, resulting in restoration of functions or even reversing the progression of the disease, as has been shown both in EAE animals (61) and in pwMS (62). Targeting synapses therapeutically can be achieved by at least some of the DMTs, especially those that pass the blood–brain barrier, by reducing inflammation and tissue damage or even by exerting direct neuroprotective effects (59). Since MS-related disability progression can be modulated by plasticity, and plasticity can be enhanced by neurorehabilitation and neuromodulation, these latter approaches could be therapeutically used to delay progression; this promotes brain reorganization, mainly at the synaptic level, since their mechanisms of action include LTP/LTD.

Neurorehabilitation and Neuromodulation for MS-Related Cognitive Impairment

Three decades have passed since the first published reports under the search items “cognitive rehabilitation” and “MS” appeared in Pubmed (1,086 research items in total, two in year 1990, 160 in 2019, page visited on 1.1.2020). In 1993, DeLuca and Johnson stated that, since cognitive dysfunction negatively impacts the lives of pwMS, it must be targeted by neurorehabilitation (63). They described the complicated landscape of cognitive rehabilitation (CR) in MS since, due to “the heterogeneous nature of the CNS lesions, each person with MS brings a unique pattern of cognitive difficulties,” and, furthermore, “effective CR in MS goes beyond simple assessment and treatment of specific deficits” (63). Since then, more questions than answers have arisen. Questions regarding the evaluation of CI and the type of CR should be investigated (64); evidence and methodological restrictions of CR protocols (1, 6568) as well as many practical issues of CR, such as the mechanisms of action, duration, intensity, frequency, repeatability, consistency and duration of effects, ecological validity, and “the transportability of such interventions under real-world conditions,” (69) should be fully explored. There is another major practical restriction: in most countries, there is lack of providers (clinical neuropsychologists and trained speech language therapists) able to apply these methods (64). These restrictions are reflected in the low rate of pwMS exposed to CI, even in countries, such as Finland, with high incidences of MS and advanced health services (70); pharmacological treatments for MS-related CI are still lacking (64, 69, 71). In order to move faster from the research fields to clinical grounds and offer CR as standard-of-care treatment, a roadmap was recently proposed by Sandroff and DeLuca (69). One major point concerning CR for MS-related CI is its mechanism of action, which seems to be the enhancement of neuroplasticity. Prosperini et al. reviewed the literature, showing that both motor and cognitive rehabilitation enhance functional and structural brain plasticity in pwMS, and this enhancement is specifically linked to the trained domain (72). Recently, Prosperini and Di Filippo updated evidence from animal models and pwMS on plasticity following rehabilitation (73).

Neuromodulation is technology acting directly upon the nervous system. Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) refers to the application on the scalp of a changing magnetic field [transcranial magnetic stimulation, TMS; (74)], or low-intensity electrical current [transcranial direct current stimulation, tDCS; (75)] over a short period of time, both of which are methods capable of altering brain function since they have cumulative and long lasting effects. The reader is referred to relevant, recently-published reviews for the use of rTMS (26) and tDCS (76) in the management of MS-related symptoms. Both techniques are easily applicable, affordable, and rather safe, with tDCS being much cheaper, able to be self-administered at home by remote supervision (77) and, more importantly, while performing a task (“on-line”), while rTMS must be carried out in the presence of a skilled clinician and during rest (“off-line”). Changing stimulus parameters and/or electrode polarity, excitation (high-frequency rTMS or anodal tDCS), or inhibition protocols (low-frequency rTMS or cathodal tDCS) can be designed, inducing LTP-like and LTD-like plasticity, respectively; this also influences brain plastic changes, acting therapeutically, either alone or in combination with CR and/or exercise. The neurobiological basis and the effectiveness of NIBS for MS-related symptoms have been updated by Leocani et al. (78). Among the issues are the differences from patient to patient in the electrical current flow induced by NIBS techniques; these depend on the volume and topography of the lesions, and different patients may need different NIBS protocols. However, since the use of tDCS during cognitive rehabilitation may improve outcomes and provide beneficial results in a short time (79), newer large-scale studies should further be performed in order to provide robust evidence to support the implementation of NIBS in routine practice (80).

Discussion

Here, we have discussed the mechanisms underlying cognitive impairment and the reorganization of cognitive networks in MS and issues for the implementation of neurorehabilitation and neuromodulation in clinical settings. Recently, Harel et al. presented “the bright side” of cognitive function in multiple sclerosis (81). Indeed, nearly 20 years after the disease onset, more than three out of 10 the pwMS of their large sample were cognitively intact. But there is also a “dark side”: two out of 10 were seriously cognitively handicapped, while one of 10 were both severely affected cognitively and physically. Furthermore, the disease does not last only 20 years, but it is lifelong; the mean age of pwMS in this study was noted as 49.3 years, meaning that the majority of them will still be alive 10–20 years later, the proportion of disabled will definitely increase, as will co-morbidities (82), and their treatment opportunities will be narrowed. Unfortunately, as it has been shown in the EAE model of MS, the loss of synapses can occur early in the disease course, irrespectively of demeylination (83). Also, neurodegeneration and resulted atrophy is proven to be evident, subclinically, even from the radiologically isolated syndrome (84). These two mechanisms could explain why CI can be present even before the time of diagnosis. Additionally, what we have undoubtedly learned is that CI at diagnosis predicts worse future disease progression (85), impairment of specific cognitive sub-domains might better predict progression (86), and patients with pediatric onset MS are more likely to have CI than patients with disease onset in adulthood, independent of age, or disease duration (87). A patient with less severe tissue damage, less atrophy, spared key brain loci, more effective tissue repair mechanisms, and enhanced brain reorganization capacities could remain cognitively intact, even decades after disease onset, and vice versa. In the first instance, the patient will probably constitute “the bright side” of the ~50% cognitively intact pwMS. What about the other 50%? For them, more than the others, “time is brain,” and delays in appropriate clinical decisions will probably cost their transition to the “dark side” of CI. In our opinion, early clinical detection of CI, coupled with evidence of structural damage in key brain regions (thalamus, hippocampus, amygdala, etc.) and altered network connectivity, could serve toward this goal. Therefore, there is an urgent need to identify this high-risk subpopulation of pwMS, and, since it is not possible to be achieved early in the disease course through clinical and conventional neuroimaging grounds only (88), we have to find biomarkers (molecular, metabolic, imaging, and clinical) to detect earlier CNS pathology “before structural tissue damage has become definite” (89). Impaired cognition is associated with early increases in FC, which then decreases due to the exhaustion of compensating mechanisms, forming the “inverted U” rs-FC curve (89). Indeed, patients who converted to MS exhibited “significantly greater network connectivity at baseline than non-converters” and a “subsequent connectivity loss over time, not observed in the non-converters' network” (30, 90). Therefore, despite methodological difficulties (91), widely available imaging markers could soon offer more (92). As we have previously stated, a biomarker composed of deep GM structures, and especially the thalamus and hippocampus volume and status, could “reflect” the cognitive status and provide insight into reorganization dynamics. Once recognized, this subpopulation should be treated more aggressively with highly efficacious DMTs and, ideally, neurorehabilitation and neuromodulation procedures.

We are not optimistic about the introduction of CR and NBIS in routine clinical care, at least in the near future, and there are reasons for this: neurorehabilitation is ultimately “treatment of the whole person” (69) and should, in other words, be tailored to every individual person. This means that it is almost impossible to include parameters of every pwMS—as they all have distinct disease characteristics, personalities, and life variables—into clinical trials and then translate their results back to a highly individualized procedure, maximizing the possibilities to identify the right patient and carry out the appropriate treatment. Moreover, as we discussed, there are many unsolved practical issues for their implementation in clinical practice. For NIBS techniques, additional issues arise, including identifying sites for brain stimulation, depending on brain lesion topography of every single pwMS, and simultaneously choosing excitatory or inhibitory protocols, or combination of both. These sophisticated individualized treatments must be carried out by a large number of trained clinicians, who do not yet exist. Finally, the (large) financial cost must be covered somehow.

What is feasible? Diagnose MS sooner, detect CI earlier and include it in clinical decisions, start treatment early and constantly follow through with more effective DMTs, find medications for CNS tissue repair, adopt strategies for “reserve and brain maintenance” (93), and, of course, do more research on neurorehabilitation and neuromodulation since they seem to be at least in part effective, even in advanced disease stages (94), and may enhance the brain's plasticity and alter disease course. This is all with the ultimate goal of implementing these methods in routine MS management.

Author Contributions

GN conceptualized this mini-review article. GN, CB, and LM wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors were involved in the critical reading and the revision process.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

CI, Cognitive impairment; CIS, clinically isolated syndrome; CNS, Central nervous system; CR, cognitive rehabilitation; DMTs, disease modifying therapies; FC, Functional connectivity; EAE, Experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis; GABA, gamma-Aminobutyric acid; GM, Gray matter; LTD, long-term depression; LTP, long-term potentiation; MEP, Motor evoked potentials; MS, Multiple sclerosis; NIBS, Non-invasive brain stimulation; PPMS, Primary progressive MS; PwMS, people living with MS; RIS, radiologically isolated syndrome; RRMS, Relapsing-remitting MS; Rs-FC, resting-state Functional Connectivity; rs-fMRI, resting-state functional MRI; rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; SC, Social cognition; SPMS, Secondary progressive MS; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation.

References

1. Messinis L, Papathanasopoulos P, Kosmidis MH, Nasios G, Kambanaros M. Neuropsychological features of multiple sclerosis: impact and rehabilitation. Behav Neurol. (2018) 2018:4831647. doi: 10.1155/2018/4831647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bakirtzis C, Ioannidis P, Messinis L, Nasios G, Konstantinopoulou E, Papathanasopoulos P, et al. The rationale for monitoring cognitive function in multiple sclerosis: practical issues for clinicians. Open Neurol J. (2018) 12:31–40. doi: 10.2174/1874205X01812010031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Prakash RS, Snook EM, Lewis JM, Motl RW, Kramer AF. Cognitive impairments in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis: a meta analysis. Multiple Sclerosis. (2008) 14:1250–61. doi: 10.1177/1352458508095004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Langdon DW. Cognition in multiple sclerosis. Curr Opin Neurol. (2011) 24:244–9. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328346a43b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rosti-Otajarvi E, Ruutianen J, Huhtala H, Hamalainen P. Cognitive performance profile in different phenotypes of MS with cognitive complaints. Multiple Sclerosis Related Disord. (2014) 3:463–72. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2014.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cortese M, Riise T, Bjørnevik K, Bhan A, Farbu E, Grytten N, et al. Preclinical disease activity in multiple sclerosis: a prospective study of cognitive performance prior to first symptom. Ann Neurol. (2016) 80:616–24. doi: 10.1002/ana.24769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Amato MP, Hakiki B, Goretti B, Rossi F, Stromillo ML, Giorgio A, et al. Association of MRI metrics and cognitive impairment in radiologically isolated syndromes. Neurology. (2012) 78:309–14. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31824528c9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zipoli V, Goretti B, Hakiki B, Siracusa G, Sorbi S, Portaccio E, et al. Cognitive impairment predicts conversion to multiple sclerosis in clinically isolated syndromes. Mult Scler. (2010) 16:62–7. doi: 10.1177/1352458509350311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rovaris M, Riccitelli G, Judica E, Possa F, Caputo D, Ghezzi A, et al. Cognitive impairment and structural brain damage in benign multiple sclerosis. Neurology. (2008) 71:1521–6. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000319694.14251.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Charvet LE, O'Donnell EH, Belman AL, Chitnis T, Ness JM, Parrish J, et al. Longitudinal evaluation of cognitive functioning in pediatric multiple sclerosis: report from the US Pediatric Multiple Sclerosis Network. Mult Scler. (2014) 20:1502–10. doi: 10.1177/1352458514527862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Papathanasiou A, Messinis L, Georgiou LV, Papathanasopoulos P. Cognitive impairment in relapsing remitting and secondary progressive multiple sclerosis patients: efficacy of a computerized cognitive screening battery. ISRN Neurology. (2014) 2014:151379. doi: 10.1155/2014/151379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Potagas C, Giogkaraki E, Koutsis G, Mandellos D, Tsirempolou E, Sfagos C, et al. Cognitive impairment in different MS subtypes and clinically isolated syndromes. J Neurol Sci. (2008) 267:100–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ntoskou A, Messinis L, Nasios G, Martzoukou M, Makris G, Panagiotopoulos E, et al. Cognitive and language deficits in multiple sclerosis: comparison of relapsing remitting and secondary progressive subtypes. Open Neurol J. (2018) 12:19–30. doi: 10.2174/1874205X01812010019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Achiron A, Chapman J, Magalashvili D, Dolev M, Lavie M, Bercovich E, et al. Modeling of cognitive impairment by disease duration in multiple sclerosis: a cross-sectional study (2013). PLoS ONE. 8:e71058. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Calabrese M, Favaretto A, Martini V, Gallo P. Grey matter lesions in MS: from histology to clinical implications. Prion. (2013) 7:20–7. doi: 10.4161/pri.22580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Calabrese M, Agosta F, Rinaldi F, Mattisi I, Grossi P, Favaretto A, et al. Cortical lesions and atrophy associated with cognitive impairment in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol. (2009) 66:1144–50. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fisher E1, Lee JC, Nakamura K, Rudick RA. Gray matter atrophy in multiple sclerosis: a longitudinal study. Ann Neurol. (2008) 64:255–65. doi: 10.1002/ana.21436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Schoonheim MM, Meijer KA, Geurts JJ. Network collapse and cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Front Neurol. (2015) 6:82. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Louapre C, Perlbarg V, García-Lorenzo D, Urbanski M, Benali H, Assouad R, et al. Brain networks disconnection in early multiple sclerosis cognitive deficits: an anatomofunctional study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:4706–17. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. (2008) 7:1139–51. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Guimares J, Jose SA. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Front Neurol. (2012) 3:74. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Chalah MA, Ayache SS. Deficits in social cognition: an unveiled signature of multiple sclerosis. J Intern Neuropsychol Soc. (2017) 23:266–86. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716001156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Giazkoulidou A, Messinis L, Nasios G. Cognitive functions and social cognition in multiple sclerosis: an overview. Hell J Nucl Med. (2019) 22(Suppl.102–10).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. Batista S, d'Almeida OC, Afonso A, Freitas S, Macário C, Sousa L, et al. Impairment of social cognition in multiple sclerosis: Amygdala atrophy is the main predictor. Mult Scler. (2017) 1358–66. doi: 10.1177/1352458516680750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Rocca MA, De Meo E, Filippi M. Functional MRI in investigating cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Acta Neurol Scand. (2016) 134(Suppl.200):39–46. doi: 10.1111/ane.12654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Nasios G, Messinis L, Dardiotis E, Papathanasopoulos P. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, cognition, and multiple sclerosis: an overview. Behav Neurol. (2018) 2018:8584653. doi: 10.1155/2018/8584653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Koubiyr I, Deloire M, Besson P, Coupé P, Dulau C, Pelletier J, et al. Longitudinal study of functional brain network reorganization in clinically isolated syndrome. Mult Scler. (2018) 27:1352458518813108. doi: 10.1177/1352458518813108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Castellazzi G, Debernard L, Melzer TR, Dalrymple-Alford JC, D'Angelo E, Miller DH, et al. Functional connectivity alterations reveal complex mechanisms based on clinical and radiological status in mild relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:690. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fleischer V, Koirala N, Droby A, Gracien RM, Deichmann R, Ziemann U, et al. Longitudinal cortical network reorganization in early relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. (2019) 12:1756286419838673. doi: 10.1177/1756286419838673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Tur C, Eshaghi A, Altmann DR, Jenkins TM, Prados F, Grussu F, et al. Structural cortical network reorganization associated with early conversion to multiple sclerosis. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:10715. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29017-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Cader S, Cifelli A, Abu-Omar Y, Palace J, Matthews PM. Reduced brain functional reserve and altered functional connectivity in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain. (2006) 129:527–37. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Rocca MA, Valsasina P, Hulst HE, Abdel-Aziz K, Enzinger C, Gallo A, et al. Functional correlates of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: a multicenter fMRI Study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:5799–814. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Llufriu S, Martinez-Heras E, Solana E, Sola-Valls N, Sepulveda M, Blanco Y, et al. Structural networks involved in attention and executive functions in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage Clin. (2016) 13:288–96. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Chiang FL, Wang Q, Yu FF, Romero RS, Huang SY, Fox PM, et al. Localised grey matter atrophy in multiple sclerosis is network-based: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Clin Radiol. (2019) 74:816.e19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.crad.2019.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Eijlers AJC, van Geest Q, Dekker I, Steenwijk MD, Meijer KA, Hulst HE, et al. Predicting cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis: a 5-year follow-up study. Brain. (2018) 141:2605–18. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Azevedo CJ, Cen SY, Khadka S, Liu S, Kornak J, Shi Y, et al. Thalamic atrophy in multiple sclerosis: a magnetic resonance imaging marker of neurodegeneration throughout disease. Ann Neurol. (2018) 83:223–34. doi: 10.1002/ana.25150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Bisecco A, Capuano R, Caiazzo G, d'Ambrosio A, Docimo R, Cirillo M, et al. Regional changes in thalamic shape and volume are related to cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2019) 3:1352458519892552. doi: 10.1177/1352458519892552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bergsland N, Zivadinov R, Dwyer MG, Weinstock-Guttman B, Benedict RH. Localized atrophy of the thalamus and slowed cognitive processing speed in MS patients. Mult Scler. (2016) 22:1327–36. doi: 10.1177/1352458515616204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Schoonheim MM, Hulst HE, Brandt RB, Strik M, Wink AM, Uitdehaag BM, et al. Thalamus structure and function determine severity of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology. (2015) 84:776–83. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Rojas JI, Murphy G, Sanchez F, Patrucco L, Fernandez MC, Miguez J, et al. Thalamus volume change and cognitive impairment in early relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients. Neuroradiol J. (2018) 31:350–5. doi: 10.1177/1971400918781977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Rocca MA, Valsasina P, Leavitt VM, Rodegher M, Radaelli M, Riccitelli GC, et al. Functional network connectivity abnormalities in multiple sclerosis: correlations with disability and cognitive impairment. Mult Scler. (2018) 24:459–71. doi: 10.1177/1352458517699875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zhou F, Zhuang Y, Wu L, Zhang N, Zeng X, Gong H, et al. Increased thalamic intrinsic oscillation amplitude in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis associated with the slowed cognitive processing. Clin Imaging. (2014) 38:605–10. doi: 10.1016/j.clinimag.2014.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Tewarie P, Schoonheim MM, Stam CJ, van der Meer ML, van Dijk BW, Barkhof F, et al. Cognitive and clinical dysfunction, altered MEG resting-state networks and thalamic atrophy in multiple sclerosis. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e69318. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Minagar A, Barnett MH, Benedict RH, Pelletier D, Pirko I, Sahraian MA, et al. The thalamus and multiple sclerosis: modern views on pathologic, imaging, and clinical aspects. Neurology. (2013) 80:210–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827b910b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Matías-Guiu JA, Cortés-Martínez A, Montero P, Pytel V, Moreno-Ramos T, Jorquera M, et al. Identification of cortical and subcortical correlates of cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis using voxel-based morphometry. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:920. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Tortorella C, Romano R, Direnzo V, Taurisano P, Zoccolella S, Iaffaldano P, et al. Load-dependent dysfunction of the putamen during attentional processing in patients with clinically isolated syndrome suggestive of multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2013) 19:1153–60. doi: 10.1177/1352458512473671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Pitteri M, Genova H, Lengenfelder J, DeLuca J, Ziccardi S, Rossi V, et al. Social cognition deficits and the role of amygdala in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients without cognitive impairment. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2019) 29:118–23. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2019.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Damjanovic D, Valsasina P, Rocca MA, Stromillo ML, Gallo A, Enzinger C, et al. Hippocampal and deep gray matter nuclei atrophy is relevant for explaining cognitive impairment in MS: a multicenter study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. (2017) 38:18–24. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A4952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Sumowski JF, Rocca MA, Leavitt VM, Riccitelli G, Sandry J, DeLuca J, et al. Searching for the neural basis of reserve against memory decline: intellectual enrichment linked to larger hippocampal volume in multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol. (2016) 23:39–44. doi: 10.1111/ene.12662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. van Geest Q, Hulst HE, Meijer KA, Hoyng L, Geurts JJG, Douw L. The importance of hippocampal dynamic connectivity in explaining memory function in multiple sclerosis. Brain Behav. (2018) 8:e00954. doi: 10.1002/brb3.954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Muhlert N, Atzori M, De Vita E, Thomas DL, Samson RS, Wheeler-Kingshott CA, et al. Memory in multiple sclerosis is linked to glutamate concentration in grey matter regions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2014) 85:833–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-306662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Sicotte NL, Kern KC, Giesser BS, Arshanapalli A, Schultz A, Montag M, et al. Regional hippocampal atrophy in multiple sclerosis. Brain. (2008) 131:1134–41. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Geurts JJ, Bö L, Roosendaal SD, Hazes T, Daniëls R, Barkhof F, et al. Extensive hippocampal demyelination in multiple sclerosis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. (2007) 66:819–27. doi: 10.1097/nen.0b013e3181461f54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rocca MA, Barkhof F, De Luca J, Frisén J, Geurts JJ, Hulst HE, et al. The hippocampus in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. (2018) 17:918–26. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30309-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Karavasilis E, Christidi F, Velonakis G, Tzanetakos D, Zalonis I, Potagas C, et al. Hippocampal structural and functional integrity in multiple sclerosis patients with or without memory impairment: a multimodal neuroimaging study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2019) 13:1049–59. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9924-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Centonze D, Muzio L, Rossi S, Cavasinni F, De Chiara V, Bergami A, et al. Inflammation triggers synaptic alteration and degeneration in experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:3442–52. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5804-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Di Filippo M, de Iure A, Durante V, Gaetani L, Mancini A, Sarchielli P, et al. Synaptic plasticity and experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis: implications for multiple sclerosis. Brain Res. (2015) 1621:205–13. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Stampanoni Bassi M, Mori F, Buttari F, Marfia GA, Sancesario A, Centonze D, et al. Neurophysiology of synaptic functioning in multiple sclerosis. Clin Neurophysiol. (2017) 128:1148–57. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Mandolesi G, Gentile A, Musella A, Fresegna D, De Vito F, Bullitta S, et al. Synaptopathy connects inflammation and neurodegeneration in multiple sclerosis. Nat Rev Neurol. (2015) 11:711–24. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Di Filippo M, Portaccio E, Mancini A, Calabresi P. Multiple sclerosis and cognition: synaptic failure and network dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2018) 19:599–609. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0053-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Kerschensteiner M, Bareyre FM, Buddeberg BS, Merkler D, Stadelmann C, Brück W, et al. Remodeling of axonal connections contributes to recovery in an animal model of multiple sclerosis. J Exp Med. (2004) 200:1027–38. doi: 10.1084/jem.20040452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Morgen K, Kadom N, Sawaki L, Tessitore A, Ohayon J, McFarland H, et al. Training-dependent plasticity in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain. (2004) 127:2506–17. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Deluca J, Johnson SK. Cognitive impairments in multiple sclerosis. implications for rehabilitation. NeuroRehabilitation. (1993) 3:9–16. doi: 10.3233/NRE-1993-3404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Bensa C, Bodiguel E, Brassat D, Laplaud D, Magy L, Ouallet JC, et al. Recommendations for the detection and therapeutic management of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Rev Neurol. (2012) 168:785–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2012.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rosti-Otajärvi EM, Hämäläinen PI. Neuropsychological rehabilitation for multiple sclerosis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2014) 1:CD009131. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009131.pub3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Dardiotis E, Nousia A, Siokas V, Tsouris Z, Andravizou A, Mentis AA, et al. Efficacy of computer-based cognitive training in neuropsychological performance of patients with multiple sclerosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2018) 20:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2017.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Amatya B, Khan F, Galea M. Rehabilitation for people with multiple sclerosis: an overview of Cochrane Reviews. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2019) 1:CD012732. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012732.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Goverover Y, Chiaravalloti ND, O'Brien AR, DeLuca J. Evidenced-based cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: an updated review of the literature from 2007 to 2016. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. (2018) 99:390–407. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Sandroff BM, DeLuca J. Will behavioral treatments for cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis become standards-of-care? Int J Psychophysiol. (2019). doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.02.010. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Castrén E, Heinonen T, Mäkinen K, Hämäläinen P, Kuusisto H. The rate of neuropsychological assessments in multiple sclerosis has increased-A retrospective study in a Finnish Central Hospital. Acta Neurol Scand. (2019) 141:156–61. doi: 10.1111/ane.13175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Amato MP, Langdon D, Montalban X, Benedict RH, DeLuca J, Krupp LB, et al. Treatment of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: position paper. J Neurol. (2013) 260:1452–68. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6678-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Prosperini L, Piattella MC, Giannì C, Pantano P. Functional and structural brain plasticity enhanced by motor and cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis. Neural Plast. (2015) 2015:481574. doi: 10.1155/2015/481574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Prosperini L, Di Filippo M. Beyond clinical changes: rehabilitation-induced neuroplasticity in MS. Mult Scler. (2019) 25:1348–62. doi: 10.1177/1352458519846096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hallett M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature. (2000) 406:147–50. doi: 10.1038/35018000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Nitsche MA, Cohen LG, Wassermann EM, Priori A, Lang N, Antal A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. (2008) 1:206–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Ayache SS, Chalah MA. The place of transcranial direct current stimulation in the management of multiple sclerosis-related symptoms. Neurodegener Dis Manag. (2018) 8:411–22. doi: 10.2217/nmt-2018-0028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Charvet L, Shaw M, Dobbs B, Frontario A, Sherman K, Bikson M, et al. Remotely supervised transcranial direct current stimulation increases the benefit of at-home cognitive training in multiple sclerosis. Neuromodulation. (2018) 21:383–9. doi: 10.1111/ner.12583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Leocani L, Chieffo R, Gentile A, Centonze D. Beyond rehabilitation in MS: insights from non-invasive brain stimulation. Mult Scler. (2019) 25:1363–71. doi: 10.1177/1352458519865734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Mattioli F, Bellomi F, Stampatori C, Capra R, Miniussi C. Neuroenhancement through cognitive training and anodal tDCS in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2016) 22:222–30. doi: 10.1177/1352458515587597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Iodice R, Manganelli F, Dubbioso R. The therapeutic use of non-invasive brain stimulation in multiple sclerosis—a review. Restor Neurol Neurosci. (2017) 35:497–509. doi: 10.3233/RNN-170735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Harel Y, Kalron A, Menascu S, Magalashvili D, Dolev M, Doniger G, et al. Cognitive function in multiple sclerosis: a long-term look on the bright side. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0221784. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Luczynski P, Laule C, Hsiung GR, Moore GRW, Tremlett H. Coexistence of multiple sclerosis and Alzheimer's disease: a review. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2019) 27:232–8. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2018.10.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Mandolesi G, Grasselli G, Musumeci G, Centonze D. Cognitive deficits in experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis: neuroinflammation and synaptic degeneration. Neurol Sci. (2010) 31(Suppl.2):S255–9. doi: 10.1007/s10072-010-0369-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Azevedo CJ, Overton E, Khadka S, Buckley J, Liu S, Sampat M, et al. Early CNS neurodegeneration in radiologically isolated syndrome. Neurol Neuroimmunol Neuroinflamm. (2015) 9:2:e102. doi: 10.1212/NXI.0000000000000102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Moccia M, Lanzillo R, Palladino R, Chang KC, Costabile T, Russo C, et al. Cognitive impairment at diagnosis predicts 10-year multiple sclerosis progression. Mult Scler. (2016) 22:659–67. doi: 10.1177/1352458515599075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Carotenuto A, Moccia M, Costabile T, Signoriello E, Paolicelli D, Simone M, et al. Associations between cognitive impairment at onset and disability accrual in young people with multiple sclerosis. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:18074. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54153-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. McKay KA, Manouchehrinia A, Berrigan L, Fisk JD, Olsson T, Hillert J. Long-term cognitive outcomes in patients with pediatric-onset vs adult-onset multiple sclerosis. JAMA Neurol. (2019). 76:1028–34. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Johnen A, Bürkner PC, Landmeyer NC, Ambrosius B, Calabrese P, Motte J, et al. Can we predict cognitive decline after initial diagnosis of multiple sclerosis? Results from the German National early MS cohort (KKNMS). J Neurol. (2019) 266:386–97. doi: 10.1007/s00415-018-9142-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Van Schependom J, Guldolf K, Béatrice D'hooghe M, Nagels G, D'haeseleer M. Detecting neurodegenerative pathology in multiple sclerosis before irreversible brain tissue loss sets in. Transl Neurodegener. (2019) 8:37. doi: 10.1186/s40035-019-0178-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Fleischer V, Gröger A, Koirala N, Droby A, Muthuraman M, Kolber P, et al. Increased structural white and grey matter network connectivity compensates for functional decline in early multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2017) 23:432–41. doi: 10.1177/1352458516651503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Wattjes MP, Rovira À, Miller D, Yousry TA, Sormani MP, de Stefano MP, et al. Evidence-based guidelines: MAGNIMS consensus guidelines on the use of MRI in multiple sclerosis–establishing disease prognosis and monitoring patients. Nat Rev Neurol. (2015) 11:597–606. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Filippi M, Preziosa P, Rocca MA. Brain mapping in multiple sclerosis: lessons learned about the human brain. Neuroimage. (2019) 190:32–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Brandstadter R, Katz Sand I, Sumowski JF. Beyond rehabilitation: a prevention model of reserve and brain maintenance in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2019) 25:1372–8. doi: 10.1177/1352458519856847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Messinis L, Kosmidis MH, Nasios G, Konitsiotis S, Ntoskou A, Bakirtzis C, et al. Do secondary progressive multiple sclerosis patients benefit from computer- based cognitive neurorehabilitation? A randomized sham controlled trial. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2020) 39:101932. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2020.101932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multiple sclerosis, cognitive impairment, brain reorganization, cognitive rehabilitation, neuromodulation

Citation: Nasios G, Bakirtzis C and Messinis L (2020) Cognitive Impairment and Brain Reorganization in MS: Underlying Mechanisms and the Role of Neurorehabilitation. Front. Neurol. 11:147. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00147

Received: 06 January 2020; Accepted: 14 February 2020;
Published: 06 March 2020.

Edited by:

Moussa Antoine Chalah, Hôpitaux Universitaires Henri Mondor, France

Reviewed by:

Rosa Iodice, University of Naples Federico II, Italy
Ralf Lürding, University of Regensburg, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Nasios, Bakirtzis and Messinis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Grigorios Nasios, grigoriosnasios@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.