Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 19 October 2020
Sec. Epilepsy
This article is part of the Research Topic High-Frequency Oscillations in the Hippocampus as Biomarkers of Pathology and Healthy Brain Function View all 12 articles

Automatic vs. Manual Detection of High Frequency Oscillations in Intracranial Recordings From the Human Temporal Lobe

  • 1Department of Neurology, Christian-Doppler Medical Center, Paracelsus Medical University, Salzburg, Austria
  • 2Department of Mathematics, Paris-Lodron University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 3Department of Psychology, Paris-Lodron University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 4Intelligent Data Analytics Lab Salzburg, Paris-Lodron University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 5Department of Epileptology, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
  • 6Faculty of Psychology, University of Akureyri, Akureyri, Iceland

Background: High frequency oscillations (HFOs) have attracted great interest among neuroscientists and epileptologists in recent years. Not only has their occurrence been linked to epileptogenesis, but also to physiologic processes, such as memory consolidation. There are at least two big challenges for HFO research. First, detection, when performed manually, is time consuming and prone to rater biases, but when performed automatically, it is biased by artifacts mimicking HFOs. Second, distinguishing physiologic from pathologic HFOs in patients with epilepsy is problematic. Here we automatically and manually detected HFOs in intracranial EEGs (iEEG) of patients with epilepsy, recorded during a visual memory task in order to assess the feasibility of the different detection approaches to identify task-related ripples, supporting the physiologic nature of HFOs in the temporal lobe.

Methods: Ten patients with unclear seizure origin and bilaterally implanted macroelectrodes took part in a visual memory consolidation task. In addition to iEEG, scalp EEG, electrooculography (EOG), and facial electromyography (EMG) were recorded. iEEG channels contralateral to the suspected epileptogenic zone were inspected visually for HFOs. Furthermore, HFOs were marked automatically using an RMS detector and a Stockwell classifier. We compared the two detection approaches and assessed a possible link between task performance and HFO occurrence during encoding and retrieval trials.

Results: HFO occurrence rates were significantly lower when events were marked manually. The automatic detection algorithm was greatly biased by filter-artifacts. Surprisingly, EOG artifacts as seen on scalp electrodes appeared to be linked to many HFOs in the iEEG. Occurrence rates could not be associated to memory performance, and we were not able to detect strictly defined “clear” ripples.

Conclusion: Filtered graphoelements in the EEG are known to mimic HFOs and thus constitute a problem. So far, in invasive EEG recordings mostly technical artifacts and filtered epileptiform discharges have been considered as sources for these “false” HFOs. The data at hand suggests that even ocular artifacts might bias automatic detection in invasive recordings. Strict guidelines and standards for HFO detection are necessary in order to identify artifact-derived HFOs, especially in conditions when cognitive tasks might produce a high amount of artifacts.

1. Introduction

High frequency oscillations (HFOs) have gained considerable interest amongst neurologists and neuroscientists in the last decade. These relatively new electroencephalographic (EEG) markers are defined as single events of at least four oscillations with a frequency above 80 Hz that clearly stand out from the background EEG (1). Classically, HFOs have further been divided into two subgroups: ripples (80–250 Hz) and fast ripples (250–500 Hz; 2). Given these criteria, a high signal-to-noise ratio is key when attempting to detect HFOs. Hence, the first findings of HFOs stem from invasive EEG (iEEG) recordings with micro- or macroelectrodes (27).

As these recordings are only performed during presurgical evaluation in patients with drug resistant epilepsies, their occurrence has naturally been studied and linked to epilepsy and many findings indicate a link between HFOs and epileptogenity, both during ictal (8, 9) and interictal states (1012). Besides there association with epilepsy, several studies also suggested an existence of a second HFO population, reflecting physiologic processes (3, 1317). Especially entorhinal and hippocampal ripples have been associated with memory consolidation in animals (18, 19) and humans (2023).

Albeit these numerous investigations, the detection of HFOs remains a highly debatable subject, and many aspects need to be considered. Besides technical considerations regarding the signal-to-noise ratio and data sampling (2426), choosing the actual method of detection can be difficult. Considering the mentioned criteria (1), visual inspection requires enlarging the signal both in time scale and amplitude in order to discern these discrete events from the background EEG (27). Screening the data in such a way is highly time-consuming and visual detection can further be biased by the raters' subjective assessment of what “clearly stands out from the background EEG” (28, 29).

In contrast, automatic HFO detection is fast and objective. In facts, there exist a plethora of automatic detection algorithms for HFOs (3034). Though considerably minimizing the time necessary to perform HFO detection, automatic detectors are prone to biases from signal artifacts (3539), and they are seldom accurate on datasets they have not been trained on (24, 40). Furthermore, automatic detection algorithms are unable to differentiate between HFOs occurring as single elements and HFOs that are coupled with epileptiform discharges.

Given its more adaptive and strict results, manual detection may thus be necessary when dealing with data containing different (physiologic and pathologic) HFO populations and artifacts. For instance, when wanting to detect physiologic HFOs that are evoked by cognitive paradigms in patients with epilepsy. In the study at hand, we analyzed such a dataset. Using a dataset described by Axmacher et al. (20), we investigated stimulus-induced HFOs during encoding and retrieval to demonstrate possible differences between the two approaches of HFO detection, as well as to take advantage of the high sensitivity of automatic detectors and the specificity of a manual review when trying to link ripple occurrence to memory performance.

For this purpose, we assessed for both detection approaches: (1) whether ripple occurrence rates during encoding or retrieval phases differed between correct and incorrect responses in the memory task; (2) whether the event rates detected during encoding were predictive for the performance in the subsequent retrieval trials on a trial level; and (3) whether the amount of detected events was related to the response times in the memory task. We hypothesized the results to differ between automatically detected and manually detected events. Assuming that automatic detection results in less valid detections, we hypothesized that event rates revealed no or less of an association with memory performance as compared to events detected visually. Confirming our hypothesis would emphasize the importance for an accurate detection in order to differentiate physiologic, e.g., memory-related, from pathologic HFOs.

2. Methods

2.1. Subjects and Experimental Procedure

Ten patients with pharmacoresistant temporal lobe epilepsies (five women, mean age = 39.4 years, SD = 10.83), enrolled in a study that took place at the University Hospital Bonn between 2004 and 2006, were retrospectively analyzed. All patients received bilateral intracranial EEG (iEEG) recordings for presurgical evaluation. Patients enrolled in the study were asked to perform a visual memory task on a recording day previous to which no seizures had been experienced for 24 h. Detailed information on the patient sample may be found in Table 1. The study was approved by the local ethics committee, and all patients gave written informed consent before participating.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient information.

The visual working memory task contained two encoding as well as one retrieval phase, intertwined by a nap time. During encoding, patients were presented with 80 pictures of either landscapes or houses. Each image was presented for 1,200 ms with a variable interstimulus interval of 1,800 ± 200 ms. In order to ensure that patients stayed focused they were asked to indicate via button press whether they saw an image depicting a house or landscape. After this initial encoding phase, patients were asked to rest in a darkened room for 60 min and try to nap. Following a pause of 15 min after this period of resting there was another encoding phase with 80 novel images. After another break of 15 min, patients were presented with all 160 images they had learned previously plus an addition of 80 unlearned images. During this retrieval phase, patients were asked to indicate whether they recognized the presented images from the encoding phases before.

2.2. iEEG Recordings and HFO Detection

Invasive EEG recordings were performed via inserted multicontact depth electrodes (AD-Tech; 10 platinum-iridium contacts each). Depth electrodes were inserted from a posterior approach into the hippocampus and rhinal cortex, and electrode locations were documented via post-implantation MRI scans. Furthermore, six patients (patients 1, 4, 5, 6, 8, and 10) received also ECoG (24–102 channels, mean = 45.67) recordings, covering additional temporal lobe areas. In all of these cases, strips covered at least the anterior temporal cortex as well as the lateral temporal cortex. Patients 6 and 10 only received unilateral depth electrode implantations, but had additional large ECoG grids over the respective other hemisphere. In patient 10, depth electrodes were implanted in the left hemisphere and thus could be included in the analyses. In addition to the described invasive EEG recordings, 3–7 scalp electrodes, vertical and horizontal eye movements, an ECG, as well as a facial electromyogram were recorded in each patient during the experiment. Invasive EEG channels were recorded at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz, and a linked mastoid signal served as reference.

For each patient one encoding and the respective retrieval session were exported to .edf format and then imported to an in-house built software called MEEGIPS (41), for HFO detection. The individual encoding recordings lasted between 305 and 387 s (mean = 329.3 s), whereas the retrieval phase lasted between 903 and 1,011 s (mean = 927 s). On average, HFO analysis was performed on 21 min of EEG data for each participant. The imported EEG data was then analyzed in two ways. First, events of interest were marked visually by one experienced rater, and second, another person conducted an automatic HFO detection.

For visual inspection, the EEG data, as well as additional EMG, ECG, and EOG channels, were prepared in two ways: First, the data was high-pass filtered at 0.1 Hz, and a FIR multiline band reject filter was applied in order to filter out the powerline noise at 50 Hz as well as its respective harmonics. This data was considered the “raw signal.” Second, the data was filtered between 80 and 250 Hz to extract the ripple-band signal, which will be referred to hereafter as the “filtered EEG.” For inspection, both of these signals were displayed next to each other on a screen, and the time cursor was synchronized. Up to eight iEEG channels at a time and the additional EMG, ECG, and EOG channels were visually inspected. In addition to the EEG signals, small windows for the empirical mode decomposition, the discrete Fourier transform, the discrete wavelet power density, and the continuous wavelet transform, calculated from any marked segment of the raw signal, were displayed on the right of the screen. iEEG channels with continuous artifacts corrupting the signal and channels with a generally poor signal-to-noise ratio were excluded.

Ripples were then marked according to the following criteria: (1) consisting of at least four consecutive oscillations both seen in the filtered signal and in the empirical mode decomposition; (2) displaying a regular morphology clearly discernable from the background EEG; (3) revealing an isolated “blob” either in the discrete wavelet power density (DWPD) or in the continuous wavelet transformed signal (CWT; 37); (4) showing a superimposed fast activity in the raw data; and (5) not directly linked to artifacts observed in the EEG, EMG, ECG or EOG channels. Based on these criteria three event categories were identified and marked: (i) ripples, fulfilling all criteria; (ii) unclear HFOs (uHFO), events that did not meet all criteria based on signal quality or unclear evidence of artifacts; and (iii) artifacts, generating ripples meeting the described criteria except the last one. All detected ripples and uHFOs were additionally discussed in the team in order to rigorously exclude all false positive events.

For the automatic detection, the data was decomposed into empirical mode functions (two intrinsic mode functions with a maximum of 100 iterations; 42). Events of interest were detected using an RMS detector with a sliding window size of 10 samples and 1-s-sized statistics segments. The properties for events of interest were fixed as follows: minimum duration of ≥12 ms; RMS transition threshold of 2SD and a peak threshold of 3SD. Events separated by <30 ms were combined taken into account a standard deviation square root. The detected events were then classified based on Stockwell's S-transformation (43) for the frequency range of 80–250 Hz, and a Tukey window was applied to segments 1 s around the center of each event of interest. Events of interest were classified as ripples (autoR) based on a maximum power ratio between the trough and the high-frequency peak of 90%, and a minimum high-frequency to low-frequency peak ratio of 20%. The process of automated HFO detection using these methods has been described in detail by Burnos et al. (44).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

The events, detected automatically and visually, were then exported together with the experimental markers and analyzed using MATLAB (release R2019a, The Mathworks, Massachusetts, USA). Rates for all autoR, ripples, uHFO, artifacts, as well as all events detected manually in cumulation were summarized for each encoding and retrieval trial and for each individual patient. Trials were defined as segments starting with stimulus onset and lasting until either patients responded via button press or the next stimulus was presented. In a next step, retrieval trials were paired with their respective encoding trials and grouped into trials with correctly and incorrectly retrieved items (i.e., correct “old” vs. incorrect “new” decisions for previously presented items). Finally, the event rates were related to the respective number of trials and iEEG channels per patient as well as to the lengths of each trial. Thus, we ended up with relative event rates for encoding and retrieval trials corrected for the trial lengths in seconds and for the number of iEEG channels analyzed. For statistical analysis, only events from temporal sites within the hemisphere contralateral to the suspected epileptic zone were considered. In patient 8, we considered the right hemisphere to contain the epileptogenic zone due to the imaging findings, despite seizure onset zones observed in both temporal lobes.

The resulting event matrices were imported into R (45). Statistical analysis aimed at answering three questions to test the general hypothesis. First, we wanted to investigate whether there was a general difference between correct and incorrect trials for the rates of detected events during the retrieval phase. For this purpose, the mean event rates for correct and incorrect trials during retrieval for each patient were entered into a rank-based ANOVA-type test from the package “nparLD” (46) with the two within-subject factors response accuracy (correct vs. incorrect) and event type (all manual events, artifacts, uHFOs, ripples, autoR).

Second, we analyzed whether the rate of events detected during encoding was predictive for the correctness in the subsequent retrieval trials on a trial level. For this purpose, we calculated a generalized linear model with the retrieval trial accuracy as dependent variable and the event rates as predictive factor. Patients were considered as a random factor, in order to take into account variations in baseline events across subjects. Furthermore, we calculated Kendall's correlation between the number of correct trials and the mean event rates per second and estimated a confidence interval using the bias corrected and accelerated bootstrap method with 10,000 bootstrap samples to assess an effect at the group level.

Third, we tested whether the amount of detected events during encoding or retrieval impacted the response time in the retrieval phase. For this we calculated Kendall's correlation between response times and event occurrence rates for each patient individually. We then tested the null hypothesis that the median of these correlations was zero, using a sign test/binomial test: The fact that under the null hypothesis, the number of correlations smaller than zero follows a binomial distribution with probability 0.5 allows for an easy calculation of p-values. Correcting for multiple comparisons (13 statistically significance tests) using the Bonferroni method, the adjusted p-threshold was set at 0.0038, in order to avoid an increased family wise error rate.

3. Results

All event rates per second for both encoding and the corresponding correct or incorrect retrieval trials are presented in Table 2. No events fulfilled all five criteria to being marked as ripples in the channels of interest. We did detect a small number of unclear HFOs in some of the patients that adhered to most criteria, but could potentially be connected to non-cerebral electrophysiological origins. Figure 1 shows such an uHFO, whereas a clear ripple detected on the ipsilateral site of the suspected epileptic focus in patient 6 is depicted in Figure 2. Notably, not all trials could be taken into consideration, as there were some missing responses during retrieval in patients 1, 2, 5, and 8.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Event rates per second and iEEG channel, detected during encoding and retrieval in correct (top rows) and incorrect (bottom rows) trials.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Event of interest defined as unclear HFO by the team as morphology was not regular. Time frequency analyses revealed no clear blob suggesting the potential ripple to be nested in an equally high-frequency noise. The raw signal is depicted at the top with the discrete wavelet power density and the continuous wavelet transformation plotted underneath. At the bottom the signal is filtered between 80 and 250 Hz.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Ripple detected on the epileptogenic site of patient 6. The raw signal is depicted at the top with the discrete wavelet power density, the continuous wavelet transformation, and an empirical mode decomposition (F0) plotted underneath. At the bottom the signal is filtered between 80 and 250 Hz.

Regarding possible differences in event occurrence rates during retrieval, we did not observe a difference between correct and incorrect trials (F1,∞ = 0.108, p = 0.743). There was, however, a main effect for the event type (F1.291,∞ = 81.514, p < 0.001). As can be seen in Figure 3, automatic ripple detection resulted in higher rates across all subjects, regardless of trials being correct or incorrect. Finally, we did not observe an interaction effect between response type and event type (F1.138,∞ = 0.135, p = 0.746). This difference between event types, to some degree, possibly stems from artifactual HFO-like events being marked as ripples. In fact, we have observed plenty of artifacts to mimic HFOs even in the iEEG channels. Especially eye-movements often resulted in such artifactual ripples (see Figure 4 for an example).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean event rates per second during retrieval for all different event types and for correct and incorrect retrieval trials. Each data point corresponds to one patient.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Eye movement-related ripple-like event on the left temporal lobe of patient 1. The raw signal is depicted at the top with the discrete wavelet power density and the continuous wavelet transformation plotted on the side. At the bottom the signal is filtered between 80 and 250 Hz.

A higher event rate for automatically detected events was also observed when looking at event rates during encoding. Considering single encoding trials in relation to performance in the corresponding retrieval trials later on, one does not detect an effect for correct vs. incorrect responses (see Figure 5). As such, analysis revealed no predictive values for any of the event types detected during encoding with regards to the later response: autoR (z = −0.767, p = 0.443), manual events (z = −0.515, p = 0.607), artifacts (z = −0.475, p = 0.635), uHFOs (z = −0.337, p = 0.736). Different baseline rates for patients were taken into account for this analysis, as we expected general differences across patients (see Figure 6). However, in none of the patients the event rate during encoding seemed to affect the response in the respective retrieval trials.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Event rates per second for each encoding trial in relation to the respective response during the corresponding retrieval trial. NA refers to missed responses during retrieval.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Mean event rates per second during encoding trials in relation to the respective response during the corresponding retrieval trial for each individual patient. (A) Shows the results for automatically detected ripples (autoR) and (B) depicts all manually detected events. NA refers to missed responses during retrieval.

Estimated group correlations between event rates and the number of correct trials corrected for the overall number of trials per patient was weak for all event types. Automatically detected event rates revealed no direction of correlation (τ = 0; CI: −0.684–0.563). Ripple-mimicking artifacts were, however, slightly negatively correlated (τ = −0.27; CI: −0.73–0.15), which also is mirrored in the correlation of all manual events (τ = −0.18; CI: −0.537–0.373). In contrast, uHFOs did not reveal a negative correlation with the number of correct trials (τ = 0.083; CI: −0.462–0.632).

Finally, we analyzed a relationship between response times and event rates during retrieval trials as well as during the respective encoding trials. The median correlation coefficients for each patient are depicted in Figure 7. Probability testing suggested a trend for retrieval trials with more artifacts and respectively more overall manual events to be longer (both with Md τ = 0.094, p = 0.039). Other than that, no relationships between event rates and response times for any of the event occurrence rates more extreme than the random binomial probability of 0.5 have been found.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Individual correlations for each patients' event occurrence rates per second during encoding (A) and retrieval (B) with the response times during the respective retrieval trials.

4. Discussion

In the present study we aimed at assessing a relationship between the occurrence of stimulus-induced ripples and performance in a visual memory task in order to evaluate two detection approaches for HFOs. We incorporated both, automatic and manual ripple detection and analyzed the iEEG during encoding and retrieval periods of a task, that had previously been reported to induce meaningful HFOs in the resting period between the task phases (20).

Importantly, manual detection did not reveal any events to occur in the iEEG channels contralateral to the suspected epileptogenic zone fulfilling all strict criteria defined for ripples. In contrast, automatic detection revealed significantly higher numbers of events detected in the chosen segments. This discrepancy seems to be caused by a high number of artifacts falsely detected as ripples. In any case, statistical analysis, did not reveal a relationship between task performance and event occurrence rates derived from either detection approach. There was no significant difference between correct and incorrect trials, and also event occurrence during encoding was not predictive of the accuracy in the respective retrieval trials. Furthermore, analyses did not suggest an association between event rates and the time needed to respond during retrieval, either.

In the first part of this section we will elaborate on the incorporated detection strategies, and discuss discrepancies in the detected ripple rates, taking into account important sources of falsely detected events. In the second part, we will briefly discuss physiologic explanations for our findings, especially the lack of manually detected ripple events. Finally, we will consider some limitations to this investigation before drawing an overall conclusion.

4.1. Manual vs. Automatic Detection of HFOs

Ever since the first examinations of HFOs, the exact way of detection has left room for debate. The gold standard of visual data inspection and manual marking by one or more raters is highly uneconomic in terms of time and resources needed (12). Furthermore, detecting events that are defined as clearly discernible from the background EEG is subjective, introducing a bias that can be well-appreciated when considering the high variability in events detected by different raters on the same data (29).

Several automatic detection algorithms have been developed to overcome these problems (25, 3034), making it easier than ever to conduct HFO analyses. However, automatic detection algorithms are not without flaws in their own respect. First of all, algorithms are usually developed and trained on specific data sets, leading to them offering good results in optimal conditions, i.e., a high signal-to-noise ratio and relatively clean data (24, 40). Furthermore, for each algorithm there are numerous settings, that can be altered, making it difficult to compare findings derived from the use of different algorithms and settings.

Second, and more importantly, automatic detection is prone to false-positive detections, resulting from artifacts and sharp transients, that can mimic HFOs after filtering (24, 36, 37), as wells as from a high-frequency noise in the data (24, 35, 38, 39). Even in invasive EEG recordings, which are considered to seldom contain biological artifacts, automatic HFO detection seems to produce a (comparably) high number of false positives.

There have been reports of muscle contractions, body movements and ocular artifacts to corrupt EEG data recorded from deep in the brain (39, 47). Furthermore, eye movements have also been shown to elicit artifacts in brain regions close to extra-ocular muscles (35, 48), appearing as HFO-like events. In line with these reports, we also found ripple-like events to coincide with eye-movements and, when filtered, EOG revealed similar HFO-like derivates as iEEG channels, suggesting eye movement-related ripples to also appear in the iEEG. Taking into account additional channels, such as EOG and EMG, highly increased the number of events defined as artifact-derived HFOs in our data.

Comparing both detection approaches, manual detection led to only few events being considered as possible ripples in our data. Taking into account additional channels, such as EOG and EMG, highly increased the number of events defined as artifact-derived HFOs. Considering these additional channels may be crucial when opting for HFO detection, even in intracranial EEG data. While the strict visual detection led to a high specificity, automatic detection appeared to produce a very high number of false positives. These findings underline the pitfalls of automated HFO detection. Preprocessing the data with special emphasis on reducing artifacts or training algorithms to acknowledge artificial HFOs might prove helpful to increase the specificity of detection algorithms (49, 50).

Given the lack of visually detected clear ripples, and the extreme discrepancy between the detection approaches, further point to a need for a more precise definition of what truly constitutes an HFO. While a very strict definition, as applied in our manual detection, leads to very few or even no clear HFOs to be detected, it may serve as a basis to align detection strategies between different rater, research groups, and different detection algorithms. Besides, the methodological and technical interpretation of our findings, there are also some physiologic explanations for the lack of manually detected ripples in our data.

4.2. Memory Task-Related HFO Occurrence

Neither of the two incorporated HFO detection approaches yielded event rates, that could be linked to performance during encoding or retrieval in the visual memory task. It should be noted, however, that we correlated event rates across all analyzed channels with memory performance and did not subselect specific channels. Furthermore, we were unable to manually detect clear ripples in the data. This finding is notable, given the numerous notions of spontaneous HFO occurrence in memory-related brain areas (3, 14, 15, 20). One explanation for the incompatible findings could be that our manual detection criteria were extremely strict (maybe too strict) and missed physiologic ripples that did not conform to the ideal pattern. Another explanation could lie in the fact, that these studies all investigated HFOs during periods of rest and sleep.

Sleep has been suggested to offer a unique window into memory consolidation via hippocampal reactivation (18, 5153), and thus might offer an increased probability to record memory-related HFOs. Especially hippocampal ripples being nested in sleep spindles have been suggested to be crucial for long-term potentiation and memory consolidation (22, 54, 55). Furthermore, resting and sleep EEG may provide data with a higher signal-to-noise ratio. Especially, high background noise and artifacts, that might have also been induced by the task, can lead to a number of false-positives for automatic detectors (24, 56). This would explain the discrepancy between the automatic and manual detection, as visual inspection would not have considered events embedded within a noisy background.

On another note, continuous high frequency activity in the background EEG has been suggested to reflect physiological activity distinctive for certain brain regions (57). This is in line with reports of high gamma band activity (including frequencies that fall into the ripple band) being related to memory (58, 59). These studies further point to a weakness in detecting single HFO events, as ripple band activity might be not only easier to detect during memory tasks, but also reveal important links to memory processes. Thus, a shift in focus from single oscillatory events to frequency band characteristics when studying cognition may be promising. Distinguishing HFOs from high frequency activity in this context may have the further benefit of ruling out epilepsy-related HFOs confounding the events of interest (60, 61).

4.3. Limitations

There are some limitations to the study at hand, some of which have already been outlined in the discussion. First, performing a manual detection with one rater only may result in very stable event detections across recordings, however multiple raters might have increased the sensitivity of visual detection. Since all unclear events and marked ripples were discussed in the team, specificity would not have changed with multiple raters, though. Second, differences between the two detection strategies have to be interpreted with caution, bearing in mind that we chose two very extreme approaches. The visual detection was performed strictly, with events of interest only being marked as ripples in case of no doubt. In contrast, the automatic detection algorithm's settings were chosen to increase sensitivity in order to make the differences between both detection strategies as visible as possible.

Third, the external reference used (linked mastoids) may have contributed to the artifact contamination of our iEEG data. The impact of the reference electrodes have already been described, and to this end a bipolar montage might have resulted in less artifactual events (24, 35, 62), which would have impacted the data for both detection strategies, however. Finally, numbers of trials between patients differed, especially with respect to correct and incorrect trials. Thus, the statistical sample was small for some analyses. This fact in connection with the small number of events for some types likely led to a low statistical power, which even carefully selected statistical tests may not have been able to compensate. Regardless of these limitations, there are some conclusions that can be drawn from the obtained results.

4.4. Conclusion

Our findings suggest grave differences between automatically and manually detected events. Our analysis suggests automatic detection to be highly affected by false ripples derived not only from technical but also from physiologic artifacts. Recording additional facial EMG as well as EOG channels seems beneficial for the identification of false ripples even in iEEG data. Future automated detection algorithms should implement artifact matching in these additional channels, in order to improve specificity. Also developing a preprocessing pipeline in order to clean the data of artifacts before automatic algorithms detect HFOs could be a potential aim for future studies. Until then, guidelines for a more strict and careful visual inspection are needed to ensure comparable results, especially when dealing with conditions that seldom offer ideal data, for instance when performing cognitive paradigms.

Finally, we were not able to visually detect clear ripples, and other event types, including automatically detected ripples, could not be related to memory processes. Therefor, it remains questionable whether HFOs as single events can be exclusively identified as physiologic biomarkers. For now high frequency activity rather than single high frequency events may present a more suitable surrogate marker for cognition. Being also less affected by epileptogenity as well as artifacts, it is also less time-consuming to investigate high frequency band activity, thus offering another promising approach for future studies.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by local ethics committee: Ethikkommission an der Medizinischen Fakultät der Rheinischen Friedrich-Wilhelms-Universität Bonn, Venusberg Campus 1 (Geb. 02), 53127 Bonn. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

AT wrote the initial manuscript and performed the automatic event detection. NG manually detected the events and developed the marking strategy in correspondence with AT and YH. PL performed the marker statistics supervised by AB. JF prepared the EEG data for analysis and conducted the initial study. ET and YH supervised the event detection strategies and general data analysis. All authors read and critically revised the manuscript before submission.

Funding

The presented research was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF): KLI657-B31 and T 798-B27, and by the Research Fund of the Paracelsus Medical University Salzburg (PMU-FFF): A-16/02/021-HÖL and A-18/01/029-HÖL.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the staff at the Department of Epileptology of the University Hospital Bonn, who took part in conducting this study, as well as Hui Zhang and Nikolai Axmacher from the Dept. of Psychology of the University of Bochum.

References

1. Jacobs J, Staba R, Asano E, Otsubo H, Wu J, Zijlmans M, et al. High-frequency oscillations (HFOs) in clinical epilepsy. Prog Neurobiol. (2012) 98:302–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bragin A, Engel J Jr., Wilson CL, Fried I, Buzsáki G. High-frequency oscillations in human brain. Hippocampus. (1999) 9:137–42. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:2<137::AID-HIPO5>3.0.CO;2-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bragin A, Engel J, Wilson CL, Fried I, Mathern GW. Hippocampal and entorhinal cortex high-frequency oscillations (100–500 Hz) in human epileptic brain and in kainic acid-treated rats with chronic seizures. Epilepsia. (1999) 40:127–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1999.tb02065.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bragin A, Wilson CL, Staba RJ, Reddick M, Fried I, Engel J Jr. Interictal high-frequency oscillations (80–500 Hz) in the human epileptic brain: entorhinal cortex. Ann Neurol. (2002) 52:407–15. doi: 10.1002/ana.10291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Staba RJ, Wilson CL, Bragin A, Jhung D, Fried I, Engel J Jr. High-frequency oscillations recorded in human medial temporal lobe during sleep. Ann Neurol. (2004) 56:108–15. doi: 10.1002/ana.20164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Jirsch J, Urrestarazu E, LeVan P, Olivier A, Dubeau F, Gotman J. High-frequency oscillations during human focal seizures. Brain. (2006) 129:1593–608. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Urrestarazu E, Jirsch JD, LeVan P, Hall J. High-frequency intracerebral EEG activity (100–500 Hz) following interictal spikes. Epilepsia. (2006) 47:1465–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00618.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Allen P, Fish D, Smith S. Very high-frequency rhythmic activity during sEEG suppression in frontal lobe epilepsy. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1992) 82:155–9. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(92)90160-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fisher RS, Webber W, Lesser RP, Arroyo S, Uematsu S. High-frequency EEG activity at the start of seizures. J Clin Neurophysiol. (1992) 9:441–8. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199207010-00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Urrestarazu E, Chander R, Dubeau F, Gotman J. Interictal high-frequency oscillations (100–500 Hz) in the intracerebral EEG of epileptic patients. Brain. (2007) 130:2354–66. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Wang S, Wang IZ, Bulacio JC, Mosher JC, Gonzalez-Martinez J, Alexopoulos AV, et al. Ripple classification helps to localize the seizure-onset zone in neocortical epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2013) 54:370–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03721.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Frauscher B, Bartolomei F, Kobayashi K, Cimbalnik J, Klooster MA, Rampp S, et al. High-frequency oscillations: the state of clinical research. Epilepsia. (2017) 58:1316–29. doi: 10.1111/epi.13829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Buzsáki G, da Silva FL. High frequency oscillations in the intact brain. Prog Neurobiol. (2012) 98:241–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Worrell GA, Gardner AB, Stead SM, Hu S, Goerss S, Cascino GJ, et al. High-frequency oscillations in human temporal lobe: simultaneous microwire and clinical macroelectrode recordings. Brain. (2008) 131:928–37. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Staba RJ, Wilson CL, Bragin A, Fried I, Engel J Jr. Quantitative analysis of high-frequency oscillations (80–500 Hz) recorded in human epileptic hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. J Neurophysiol. (2002) 88:1743–52. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.4.1743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Frauscher B, von Ellenrieder N, Zelmann R, Doležalová I, Minotti L, Olivier A, et al. Atlas of the normal intracranial electroencephalogram: neurophysiological awake activity in different cortical areas. Brain. (2018) 141:1130–44. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Frauscher B, von Ellenrieder N, Zelmann R, Rogers C, Nguyen DK, Kahane P, et al. High-frequency oscillations in the normal human brain. Ann Neurol. (2018) 84:374–85. doi: 10.1002/ana.25304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. (1994) 265:676–9. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Chrobak JJ, Buzsáki G. High-frequency oscillations in the output networks of the hippocampal-entorhinal axis of the freely behaving rat. J Neurosci. (1996) 16:3056–66. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-03056.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Axmacher N, Elger CE, Fell J. Ripples in the medial temporal lobe are relevant for human memory consolidation. Brain. (2008) 131:1806–17. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Clemens Z, Mölle M, Erőss L, Barsi P, Halász P, Born J. Temporal coupling of parahippocampal ripples, sleep spindles and slow oscillations in humans. Brain. (2007) 130:2868–78. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Siapas AG, Wilson MA. Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron. (1998) 21:1123–8. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D, Buzsáki G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2003) 100:2065–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0437938100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Worrell GA, Jerbi K, Kobayashi K, Lina JM, Zelmann R, Le Van Quyen M. Recording and analysis techniques for high-frequency oscillations. Prog Neurobiol. (2012) 98:265–78. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Navarrete M, Pyrzowski J, Corlier J, Valderrama M, Le Van Quyen M. Automated detection of high-frequency oscillations in electrophysiological signals: methodological advances. J Physiol. (2016) 110:316–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2017.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zijlmans M, Worrell GA, Dümpelmann M, Stieglitz T, Barborica A, Heers M, et al. How to record high-frequency oscillations in epilepsy: a practical guideline. Epilepsia. (2017) 58:1305–15. doi: 10.1111/epi.13814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Zelmann R, Zijlmans M, Jacobs J, Châtillon CE, Gotman J. Improving the identification of high frequency oscillations. Clin Neurophysiol. (2009) 120:1457–64. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.05.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Roehri N, Pizzo F, Bartolomei F, Wendling F, Bénar CG. What are the assets and weaknesses of HFO detectors? A benchmark framework based on realistic simulations. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0174702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Spring AM, Pittman DJ, Aghakhani Y, Jirsch J, Pillay N, Bello-Espinosa LE, et al. Interrater reliability of visually evaluated high frequency oscillations. Clin Neurophysiol. (2017) 128:433–41. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Liu S, Sha Z, Sencer A, Aydoseli A, Bebek N, Abosch A, et al. Exploring the time-frequency content of high frequency oscillations for automated identification of seizure onset zone in epilepsy. J Neural Eng. (2016) 13:026026. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/13/2/026026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Migliorelli C, Alonso JF, Romero S, Nowak R, Russi A, Mañanas MA. Automated detection of epileptic ripples in MEG using beamformer-based virtual sensors. J Neural Eng. (2017) 14:046013. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aa684c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Quitadamo LR, Foley E, Mai R, De Palma L, Specchio N, Seri S. EPINETLAB: a software for seizure-onset zone identification from intracranial EEG signal in epilepsy. Front Neuroinform. (2018) 12:45. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Ren GP, Yan JQ, Yu ZX, Wang D, Li XN, Mei SS, et al. Automated detector of high frequency oscillations in epilepsy based on maximum distributed peak points. Int J Neural Syst. (2018) 28:1750029. doi: 10.1142/S0129065717500290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Shimamoto S, Waldman ZJ, Orosz I, Song I, Bragin A, Fried I, et al. Utilization of independent component analysis for accurate pathological ripple detection in intracranial EEG recordings recorded extra-and intra-operatively. Clin Neurophysiol. (2018) 129:296–307. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jerbi K, Freyermuth S, Dalal S, Kahane P, Bertrand O, Berthoz A, et al. Saccade related gamma-band activity in intracerebral EEG: dissociating neural from ocular muscle activity. Brain Topogr. (2009) 22:18–23. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0078-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Amiri M, Lina JM, Pizzo F, Gotman J. High frequency oscillations and spikes: separating real HFOs from false oscillations. Clin Neurophysiol. (2016) 127:187–96. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.04.290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Bénar CG, Chauviére L, Bartolomei F, Wendling F. Pitfalls of high-pass filtering for detecting epileptic oscillations: a technical note on ‘false’ ripples. Clin Neurophysiol. (2010) 121:301–10. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kovach CK, Tsuchiya N, Kawasaki H, Oya H, Howard MA III, Adolphs R. Manifestation of ocular-muscle EMG contamination in human intracranial recordings. Neuroimage. (2011) 54:213–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Otsubo H, Ochi A, Imai K, Akiyama T, Fujimoto A, Go C, et al. High-frequency oscillations of ictal muscle activity and epileptogenic discharges on intracranial EEG in a temporal lobe epilepsy patient. Clin Neurophysiol. (2008) 119:862–8. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zelmann R, Mari F, Jacobs J, Zijlmans M, Dubeau F, Gotman J. A comparison between detectors of high frequency oscillations. Clin Neurophysiol. (2012) 123:106–16. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Höller P, Trinka E, Höller Y. MEEGIPS-a modular EEG investigation and processing system for visual and automated detection of high frequency oscillations. Front Neuroinform. (2019) 13:20. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2019.00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Flandrin P, Rilling G, Goncalves P. Empirical mode decomposition as a filter bank. IEEE Signal Process Lett. (2004) 11:112–4. doi: 10.1109/LSP.2003.821662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Stockwell RG, Mansinha L, Lowe R. Localization of the complex spectrum: the S transform. IEEE Trans Signal Process. (1996) 44:998–1001. doi: 10.1109/78.492555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Burnos S, Hilfiker P, Sürücü O, Scholkmann F, Krayenbühl N, Grunwald T, et al. Human intracranial high frequency oscillations (HFOs) detected by automatic time-frequency analysis. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e94381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna (2018). Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/

Google Scholar

46. Noguchi K, Gel YR, Brunner E, Konietschke F. nparLD: an R software package for the nonparametric analysis of longitudinal data in factorial experiments. J Stat Softw. (2012) 50:1–23. doi: 10.18637/jss.v050.i12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Worrell G. High-frequency oscillations recorded on scalp EEG. Epilepsy Curr. (2012) 12:57–8. doi: 10.5698/1535-7511-12.2.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Yuval-Greenberg S, Tomer O, Keren AS, Nelken I, Deouell LY. Transient induced gamma-band response in EEG as a manifestation of miniature saccades. Neuron. (2008) 58:429–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Jiang C, Li X, Yan J, Yu T, Wang X, Ren Z, et al. Determining the quantitative threshold of high-frequency oscillation distribution to delineate the epileptogenic zone by automated detection. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:889. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ren S, Gliske SV, Brang D, Stacey WC. Redaction of false high frequency oscillations due to muscle artifact improves specificity to epileptic tissue. Clin Neurophysiol. (2019) 130:976–85. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Davidson TJ, Kloosterman F, Wilson MA. Hippocampal replay of extended experience. Neuron. (2009) 63:497–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gais S, Albouy G, Boly M, Dang-Vu TT, Darsaud A, Desseilles M, et al. Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2007) 104:18778–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705454104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Ji D, Wilson MA. Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci. (2007) 10:100–7. doi: 10.1038/nn1825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Buzsáki G. Long-term changes of hippocampal sharp-waves following high frequency afferent activation. Brain Res. (1984) 300:179–82. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)91356-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Staresina BP, Bergmann TO, Bonnefond M, Van Der Meij R, Jensen O, Deuker L, et al. Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:1679. doi: 10.1038/nn.4119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. von Ellenrieder N, Andrade-Valença LP, Dubeau F, Gotman J. Automatic detection of fast oscillations (40–200 Hz) in scalp EEG recordings. Clin Neurophysiol. (2012) 123:670–80. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.07.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Melani F, Zelmann R, Dubeau F, Gotman J. Occurrence of scalp-fast oscillations among patients with different spiking rate and their role as epileptogenicity marker. Epilepsy Res. (2013) 106:345–56. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Kunii N, Kawai K, Kamada K, Ota T, Saito N. The significance of parahippocampal high gamma activity for memory preservation in surgical treatment of atypical temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2014) 55:1594–601. doi: 10.1111/epi.12764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Pu Y, Cornwell BR, Cheyne D, Johnson BW. High-gamma activity in the human hippocampus and parahippocampus during inter-trial rest periods of a virtual navigation task. Neuroimage. (2018) 178:92–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Liu S, Parvizi J. Cognitive refractory state caused by spontaneous epileptic high-frequency oscillations in the human brain. Sci Transl Med. (2019) 11:eaax7830. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aax7830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Waldman ZJ, Camarillo-Rodriguez L, Chervenova I, Berry B, Shimamoto S, Elahian B, et al. Ripple oscillations in the left temporal neocortex are associated with impaired verbal episodic memory encoding. Epilepsy Behav. (2018) 88:33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Nagasawa T, Matsuzaki N, Juhász C, Hanazawa A, Shah A, Mittal S, et al. Occipital gamma-oscillations modulated during eye movement tasks: simultaneous eye tracking and electrocorticography recording in epileptic patients. Neuroimage. (2011) 58:1101–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: high-frequency oscillations, visual memory, invasive EEG, electroencephalography, epilepsy

Citation: Thomschewski A, Gerner N, Langthaler PB, Trinka E, Bathke AC, Fell J and Höller Y (2020) Automatic vs. Manual Detection of High Frequency Oscillations in Intracranial Recordings From the Human Temporal Lobe. Front. Neurol. 11:563577. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.563577

Received: 22 May 2020; Accepted: 26 August 2020;
Published: 19 October 2020.

Edited by:

Julia Jacobs, University of Freiburg Medical Center, Germany

Reviewed by:

Giovanni Pellegrino, McGill University, Canada
Xiaofeng Yang, Beijing Institute for Brain Disorders, China

Copyright © 2020 Thomschewski, Gerner, Langthaler, Trinka, Bathke, Fell and Höller. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aljoscha Thomschewski, a.thomschewski@salk.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.