Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 29 October 2020
Sec. Neurotrauma
This article is part of the Research Topic Tackling Chronic Traumatic Encephalopathy: A Transdisciplinary Series of Original Research Articles to Advance Knowledge on Disease Diagnosis, Risk Factors, and Mechanisms View all 8 articles

Sex Moderates the Relationship That Number of Professional Fights Has With Cognition and Brain Volumes

\nLauren L. Bennett
Lauren L. Bennett1*Steve J. StephenSteve J. Stephen2Charles BernickCharles Bernick2Guogen ShanGuogen Shan3Sarah J. BanksSarah J. Banks4
  • 1Neuropsychologist, Pickup Family Neurosciences Institute, Hoag Memorial Hospital Presbyterian, Newport Beach, CA, United States
  • 2Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health, Las Vegas, NV, United States
  • 3Department of Environmental and Occupational Health, School of Community Health Sciences, University of Nevada Las Vegas, Las Vegas, NV, United States
  • 4Departments of Neurosciences and Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States

Objective: Incidence of concussions and report of symptoms are greater among women across sports. While structural brain changes and cognitive declines are associated with repetitive head impact (RHI), the role of sex is not well-understood. This study aimed to determine if there is a moderating effect of sex on the relationship the number of professional fights has with cognitive functioning and regional brain volumes in a cohort of boxers, mixed martial artists, and martial artists.

Methods: A total of 55 women were matched with 55 men based on age, years of education, ethnicity, and fighting style. Cognition was assessed via the CNS Vital Signs computerized cognitive battery and supplemental measures. Structural brain scans, demographic data, and number of professional fights (NoPF) were also considered. The matched pairs were compared via analysis of covariance, accounting for total brain volume. Within-subject moderation models were utilized to assess the moderating effect of sex on the relationship between NoPF and brain volumes and cognitive performance.

Results: Men were observed to have poorer performance on measures of psychomotor speed when compared to women. On a series of analyses assessing the role of sex as a moderator of the relationship between NoPF and regional brain volumes/cognitive performance, a significant moderation effect was observed across multiple measures of cognitive functioning, such that men had poorer performance. Differences in numerous regional brain volumes were also observed, such that the relationship between NoPF and brain volumes was steeper among men.

Conclusion: Sex was observed to be an important moderator in the relationship between NoPF, aspects of cognitive functioning, and volumes of numerous brain regions, suggesting that sex differences in neuroanatomic and cognitive response to RHI deserve further attention.

Introduction

Multiple epidemiological studies have demonstrated that male athletes have been shown to be at lower risk for sports-related concussion than female athletes within both practice and competition across numerous sports (1). Specifically, women participating in basketball, soccer, lacrosse, softball/baseball, tennis, outdoor track, and cross-country are at an increased risk for sustaining a diagnosed concussion when compared to men (24). Moreover, following head injury, female athletes have been found to endorse a greater severity of concussion-related symptoms, demonstrate greater declines in cognitive functioning, and show slower physiological recovery post-injury. It is important to note prior findings highlighting that female athletes may be more likely to honestly endorse concussion symptoms, while male athletes are more likely to underreport concussive symptoms (57). Interestingly, prior research suggests that there are sex differences in the changes to both structural and metabolic brain functioning following concussions for athletes participating in other sports [i.e., ice hockey, soccer, water polo; (611)]. Very little research has been completed in sex differences in male and female professional fighters (1).

In addition, recent professional fight records comparing fight outcomes across men and women may be an indication of the different rates of repetitive head impact exposure, suggesting a need for further exploration of sex differences. When examining Nevada state fight records from 2015 through 2017, it appears that women are less likely to be knocked out (KO) or sustain a technical knockout (TKO) than male fighters overall [please see Table 1; (12)]. This data is in contrast to the higher rate of concussions observed among female athletes who participate in most other sports. Notably, as fewer women compete in combat sports, it is difficult to determine if the same trend would exist if the samples were to be equally sized. To date, the relationship between sex, regional brain volumes, and cognitive performance among male and female professional fighters has not been examined. This study aimed to investigate if there is a differential effect of sex on the relationship that the number of professional fights (NoPF) has with cognitive functioning and brain region volumes in professional fighters.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. 2015 to 2017 Nevada commission fight records.

Methods

Study Design

The participants were drawn from an ongoing longitudinal observational study of professional combat sport athletes, the Professional Fighters Brain Health Study [PFBHS; (13)]. As part of the PFBHS, the participants were assessed at baseline and at subsequent 1-year intervals. For the purposes of this study, baseline imaging, demographic, number of professional fights, and cognitive functioning data were considered. Given the small number of women with longitudinal data, we restricted the current analysis to the baseline data; thus, we report cross-sectional analyses here. While collected, self-reports of the number of amateur fights and concussions were excluded from analyses as these data reflect high levels of variability and are uncorroborated. Since the NoPF could be validated, it was used as a measure of fight exposure with consideration of training exposure, in addition to exposure during the actual match.

The study was approved by the local institutional review board (#10-944), and written informed consent was obtained from all study participants. Study visits for all participants included in these analyses were performed at the Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health in Las Vegas, Nevada between 03/30/2011 and 10/18/2017 [for a detailed explanation of the study methods, see Bernick et al. (13)].

Participants

The study cohort consisted of all 55 women fighters enrolled in the PFBHS to date and 55 male fighters who were matched with the female fighters on the following variables: age, years of education, ethnicity, and type of competitive fighting, including boxing, mixed martial arts (MMAs), and martial arts (MAs; e.g., kickboxing, Muay Thai, judo). Both retired and active professional fighters were included in the analyses for this study. The females were matched as closely as possible on the following variables in order of priority: (1) age, (2) years of education, (3) race and ethnicity, and (5) fighting style (e.g., boxing, mixed martial arts, and martial arts). In the majority of cases, the male and female fighter matches were exact, but in a minority of matches, age and/or years of education was matched ±2 years secondary. Additionally, in a few cases, fighting style was not matched exactly (i.e., a purely MMA female fighter was matched with a male fighter who endorsed/practiced MMA 70% or greater of his time). There were no significant differences between male and female fighters with regard to age, years of education, ethnicity, and type of fighting (e.g., boxer, MMAs, MAs; all p > 0.2). The participants were only seen for their baseline or follow-up visits after at least 45 days following their most recent fight.

Cognitive and Psychological Assessment

The participants completed a short battery of computerized cognitive and motor tests, including symbol digit coding, finger-tapping, and Stroop-like tasks from the CNS Vital Signs program (14). Performance across CNS Vital Sign subtests produced composite scores of the following domains: verbal memory (word list recognition), processing speed (the number of correctly completed items on symbol digit coding while accounting for incorrect responses), psychomotor speed (bilateral finger-tapping speed and number correct on a digit–symbol–digit coding task), and reaction time (response time on Stroop-like tasks). Prior research has found that men perform significantly better on right-sided finger-tapping, but no other significant sex-based performance differences were observed (15).

Beyond the CNS Vital Signs battery, the fighters completed six additional measures of cognitive functioning during assessment, including supplemental measures of processing speed [a timed reading passage and a computerized version of Trails A via the iCOMET battery (speeded connection of numeric dots)], language [semantic fluency (ability to name items belonging to given a semantic category), a word task (ability to pronounce words with irregular, non-phonetic spellings)], and executive functioning [letter fluency (ability to generate words beginning with a given letter) and a computerized version of Trails B via the iCOMET battery (speeded, alternating connection of numeric and alphabetic dots)].

Imaging

Brain MRI scans were conducted on a MAGNETOM Verio 3-tesla scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, AG, Erlangen, Germany) with volumetric values derived from T1-weighted images via FreeSurfer, version 6 (16).

Volumetric segmentation was performed on the MPRAGE sequence using the Freesurfer, version 6.0, image analysis suite (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). The procedures for measuring cortical thickness have been validated against histological analysis and manual measurements. Freesurfer morphometric procedures have demonstrated good test–retest reliability across scanner manufacturers and across field strengths.

Conventional sagittal 3D magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient echo (MPRAGE) T1 [voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1.2 mm; flip angle/repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE)/inversion time (TI) = 9/2300/2.98/900 ms; scan time = 9:14] and axial turbo spin-echo T2 (voxel size = 0.8 × 0.8 × 4 mm; TR/TE =5,000/84 ms; 38 slices; scan time = 0:57).

Analyses

Guided by prior findings from the PFBHS, as well as other studies highlighting structural and metabolic changes following repetitive head impact, the following brain regions were chosen a priori for examination: the putamina, hippocampi, amygdalae, caudates, and thalami (8, 1720). When applicable, both the left and the right sides of the brain regions were evaluated. Additionally, total brain volume was controlled for in all imaging analyses. Moderation analyses were conducted in SAS, version 9. All other analyses were conducted in SPSS, version 23.

Analysis of variance tests were run to examine group differences between female and male fighters on the four CNS Vital Signs composite scores (verbal memory, processing speed, psychomotor speed, reaction time), as well as supplemental measures of processing speed (timed reading task, computerized version of Trails A), language (semantic fluency), and executive functioning (letter fluency, computerized version of Trails B). Analysis of covariance tests were run to examine group differences across regional brain volumes between male and female fighters while accounting for total brain volume. To assess the moderating role of sex in the relationship between fight exposure and regional brain volumes and cognitive performance, a series of within-subject moderation models was computed.

Twenty models were computed, one for each of the five bilateral brain regions identified a priori and each of the 10 cognitive outcome measures. In order to account for multiple comparisons, alpha was set to 0.005 per the Bonferroni method. Sex as a moderator within models was considered at p < 0.05. Exploratory analyses to characterize models were considered at p < .05. For fighters whose NoPF was more than two standard deviations above the mean NoPF (n = 5), imaging and cognitive performance data from additional study visits were reviewed to ensure that measurements of regional brain volumes and cognitive performance were similar across study visits in an attempt to rule out a measurement error or spurious findings. As baseline measurements and cognitive performance did not significantly differ from measurements taken at later annual study visits, the data were thought to be reflective of the true impact of NoPF on the variables of interest, warranting inclusion. Beyond ruling out a measurement error, longitudinal data were not considered as part of the current study as these were available for only 26 female fighters.

Results

Demographic Data

The fighters ranged in age from 19 to 55 years, with a mean age of 30.70 (SD = 6.7). Of the 110 fighters, 101 were active fighters consisting of 52 men and 49 women. Of the nine retired fighters, three were men and six were women. The participants were encouraged to select all race categories that applied to them. Self-defined race was 69% (n = 76) white, 13% (n = 14) black, 8% (n = 9) other, 6% (n = 6) Pacific Islander, and 3% (n = 3) Asian. Four participants (4%) did not provide information on their race. The majority of fighters identified themselves as MMAs (n = 64). The mean number of years of education completed was 14.46 (SD = 2.0). As the participants were matched, there were no significant differences between male and female fighters with regard to age, years of education, ethnicity, and type of fighting (e.g., boxer, MMA, MAs; all p > 0.2). The fighters had a mean of 12.8 professional fights (SD = 15.8). The female and the male fighters did not significantly differ in the number of professional fights fought (p = 0.764). Additional demographic data are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participants' demographic data.

Sex Differences in Regional Brain Volumes

ANCOVAs were conducted to assess differences in regional brain volumes while controlling for total brain volume. Female fighters (mean = 1,624.18 mm3) differed significantly from male fighters (mean = 1,669.05 mm3) in the left amygdala [F(1, 106) = 8.417; p = 0.005] when accounting for total brain volume. In contrast, the right amygdala, bilateral hippocampi, thalamus, and caudate volumes were not significantly different as a function of sex (all p > 0.05). These results suggest that female fighters have a larger left amygdala, when accounting for total brain volume, than their male fighter counterparts.

Sex Differences in Cognitive Performance

Male fighters had poorer performance on CNS Vital Signs psychomotor speed measures [F(1, 106) = 8.32; p = 0.005]. There were no significant differences on CNS Vital Signs composite scores of verbal memory, processing speed, and reaction time (all p > 0.05). Similarly, there were no significant differences on the supplement cognitive functioning measures of processing speed (Trails A and passage reading time), language (semantic fluency and word reading), and executive functioning (Trails B and letter fluency) performance as a function of sex (all p > 0.05). All analyses accounted for education.

Sex as a Moderator of the Relationship Between Fight Exposure and Regional Brain Volumes

In order to explore the role of sex as a moderator of the relationship between fight exposure and regional brain volumes, a series of within-subject moderation analyses was computed according to the above-stated parameters. A significant moderation effect of sex was observed on the right hippocampus, right thalamus, left putamen, left amygdala, and right amygdala (please see Figures 1AE and Table 3). While the smaller right hippocampus, right thalamus, left putamen, and bilateral amygdala were associated with greater fight exposure in both men and women, the relationship between the number of professional fights and volumes was much steeper among men than the relationship between the number of professional fights and volumes among women.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Relationship between the number of professional fights and the subcortical regional brain volumes (mm3) by sex: (A) right hippocampus, (B) right thalamus, (C) left putamen, (D) right putamen, (E) left amygdala, and (F) right amygdala.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Moderation effect of sex on subcortical regional brain volumes.

While a significant moderation effect of sex was also observed on the right putamen, the relationship among women and men differed. Among male fighters, a higher number of professional fights was associated with a smaller right putamen volume. In contrast, greater right putamen volume was associated with a higher number of professional fights among female fighters (please see Figure 1F and Table 3). The left hippocampus, left thalamus, and bilateral caudate were not found to be significant moderators in the relationship between the number of professional fights and regional brain volumes (p > 0.05).

Sex as a Moderator of the Relationship Between Fight Exposure and Cognitive Performance

A significant moderation effect of sex was also observed on CNS Vital Signs verbal memory and reaction time performance. While lower verbal memory performance was associated with a higher number of professional fights in male fighters, verbal memory performance was positively associated with the number of professional fights among female fighters (please see Figure 2A and Table 4). Looking at CNS Vital Signs reaction time performance, which is reverse-scored with lower scores equating to a faster, better performance, while the reaction time performance of both male and female fighters was found to be better with a greater number of professional fights, the relationship between the number of professional fights and reaction time performance was much steeper among men than the relationship between the number of professional fights and reaction time performance among women (please see Figure 2B and Table 4).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Relationship between the number of professional fights and CNS Vital Signs performance by sex: (A) verbal memory subject score (raw number correct) and (B) reaction time subject score (reverse-scored; milliseconds). As CNS Vital Signs reaction time is reverse-scored, the graph depicts maximum score—raw score for each participant, which is reported in milliseconds.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Moderation effect of sex on subcortical regional brain volumes.

Sex was not found to be a significant moderator in the relationship between the number of professional fights and performance on measures of processing speed (CNS Vital Signs processing speed, Trails A, timed reading task), psychomotor speed (CNS Vital Signs psychomotor speed), language (semantic fluency, word reading task), or executive functioning (letter fluency, Trails B; all p > 0.05).

Discussion

The current findings reveal key insights into sex-based differences among professional fighters for the relationship between NoPF and both regional brain volumes and cognitive performance. When accounting for whole brain volume, women were found to have a larger left amygdala than men. While prior research is equivocal, a recent meta-analysis suggests that the amygdalae are not sexually dimorphic (21). Additional sex-based differences emerged when exploring the role of sex as a moderator of the relationship between fight exposure and regional brain volumes or cognitive performance.

With regard to regional brain volumes, relevant subcortical smaller volumes were associated with a greater number of professional fights among both male and female fighters. Notably, the relationship between the number of professional fights and regional brain volumes was observed to be much steeper in men. Interestingly, while a significant moderation effect of sex was observed on the right putamen, such that lower volumes in men were associated with a higher number of professional fights, an inverse relationship was observed among women. Notably, a level of noise exists in the measurement of regional brain volumes via MRI. Prior research has found that the test–retest differences of structures is up to 5%, so the association between the number of professional fights and larger volumes in the right putamen may reflect the noise of the measure itself (22). Alternatively, as the putamen is a highly connected brain region, it may be that as the volumes of other surrounding regions are decreasing, the volume of the putamen increases in an effort to compensate. In addition, prior research suggests that the basal ganglia may be one of the earliest brain regions to manifest inflammation in some disease processes [e.g., HIV; (23)]. It may be that the observed slight “growth” in the right putamen is actually a reflection of inflammation in response to exposure to repetitive head impact.

Sex also moderated the relationship between the number of professional fights and two aspects of cognitive functioning, verbal memory and reaction time. Notably, a greater number of professional fights was associated with poorer verbal memory performance among male fighters, while an inverse relationship was observed among female fighters. Recent research has demonstrated that factors impacting hormones (e.g., use of hormonal contraceptives) in women younger than the age of 65 can have a profound impact on cognitive functioning, with the strongest association between verbal memory and hormonal contraceptive use (24). As age was observed to be highly collinear with the number of professional fights, it may be that as NoPF and age increased, so too does the potential for lifetime hormonal contraceptive use among female fighters. Although data on hormonal contraceptive use in our population of female fighters were not collected as part of PFBHS, it is assumed that, among female fighters, the duration of hormonal contraceptive use increases with continued use over time and thus is also likely highly collinear with age and NoPF. Another study has shown how pre-menopausal women (mean age = 24.2) taking oral contraceptives had improved verbal memory compared to women not taking oral contraceptives (mean age = 25.69), which may also contribute to the inverse relationship discovered in our female cohort [mean age = 31; (25)]. In contrast, the poorer reaction time performance among both male and female fighters was associated with a higher number of professional fights. Notably, the relationship between reaction time performance and the number of professional fights was much steeper among men.

While smaller regional brain volumes and poorer cognitive performance were largely associated with a higher number of professional fights in both men and women, female fighters were consistently less negatively impacted than male fighters. This may reflect lower velocity punches, greater resilience, or a combination of multiple factors. For example, Kimm and Thiel (26) found that while the velocity of punches increases with experience (i.e., more fights) for all fighters, regardless of gender, women have lower velocity punches than men at all levels of experience. These findings also suggest that as NoPF rise, so too does the velocity of punches and, subsequently, the risk of head injury to the fighter's opponent. Another recent study examined backward arm cranking power output as a proxy for punch power output, demonstrating that men's “muscle performance for protracting the arm to propel the first forward” was significantly greater than their female counterparts' performance (27). The described discrepancy in punch velocity may also explain why female fighters appear to show less deleterious cognitive and regional brain volume impact.

The current findings are further supported by prior PFBHS research outcomes that demonstrate volumetric reduction and negative impacts on cognitive performance with increased NoPF. One study using NoPF and years of fighting as a proxy for head injury exposure found that increased exposure was associated with decreased thalamic and caudate volumes as well as poorer processing speed performance (18). Similarly, when comparing boxers, mixed martial artists, and martial artists, fighting style was observed to moderate the relationship between NoPF, cognitive performance, and regional brain volumes (28).

Though the PFBHS is highly unique in that the data reflect a large cohort of professional fighters, various limitations of the current study must be considered. Less than 10% of the entire PFBHS sample is female. As the cohort of female fighters is significantly smaller than the cohort of male fighters, only baseline data could be considered in an effort to retain as many participants as possible and preserve statistical power. Notably, as the cohort of martial artists practice a wide variety of fighting styles (e.g., kickboxing, Muay Thai, taekwondo, and jiu-jitsu), the martial arts cohort may reflect a wide range of exposure to repetitive head impact. Similarly, NoPF only accounts for professional matches. As such, the impact of training for and participating in amateur matches is not considered. Additionally, rather than considering KO/TKO from professional matches alone, NoPF was utilized as a proxy for head impact exposure (inclusive of subconcussive and concussive impacts) across the period of training for and participating in professional level matches. Moreover, the fighters' weight classes were not considered but may impact the incidence and the severity of repetitive head impact. As approximately 40 analyses (some including covariates and multiple variables) were completed, an increased potential for false positives is acknowledged. One counterintuitive finding (i.e., larger putamen volumes among women with more exposure) was observed, indicating that additional research is required. Finally, additional unconsidered factors (i.e., drug use, socioeconomic status, genetic predisposition) may contribute to declines in cognitive functioning and regional brain volumes rather than exposure to repetitive head impact alone.

As the collection of data is ongoing, further analyses exploring changes over time when accounting for sex differences may provide insight into longer-term discrepancies in regional brain volumes and cognitive functioning. These baseline results do not necessarily imply differences in the long-term impacts of repetitive head impact as a function of sex, but longitudinal data over time comparing male and female fighters may have predictive value. Comparing male and female retired fighters may also provide some information about long-term concussive impact differences.

In summary, this study adds to our understanding that sex-based cognitive and volumetric differences in response to repetitive head impact exist among fighters. The main finding is that sex is an important moderator in the relationship between the number of professional fights, aspects of cognitive functioning, and brain volumes of numerous regions. While smaller regional brain volumes and poorer cognitive performance were generally associated with a greater number of professional fights among men and women, female fighters were consistently less negatively impacted than male fighters.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Cleveland Clinic Institutional Review Board (#10-944). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CB and SB designed the PFBHS. LB, SS, CB, and SB contributed to the design and execution of this project. LB, SS, CB, GS, and SB contributed to the analysis of the results and to the writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Belator, Ultimate Fighting Competition (UFC), the August Rapone Family Foundation, Top Rank, Haymon Boxing, and an Institutional Development Award (SB, IDeA NIGMS; P20GM109025).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Merritt VC, Padgett CR, Jak AJ. A systematic review of sex differences in concussion outcome: what do we know? Clin Neuropsychol. (2019) 33:1016–43. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2018.1508616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Lincoln AE, Caswell SV, Almquist JL, Dunn RE, Norris JB, Hinton RY. Trends in concussion incidence in high school sports: a prospective 11-year study. Am J Sports Med. (2011) 39:958–63. doi: 10.1177/0363546510392326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Marar M, McIlvain NM, Fields SK, Comstock RD. Epidemiology of concussions among United States high school athletes in 20 sports. Am J Sports Med. (2012) 40:747–55. doi: 10.1177/0363546511435626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Zuckerman SL, Kerr ZY, Yengo-Kahn A, Wasserman E, Covassin T, Solomon GS. Epidemiology of sports-related concussion in NCAA athletes from 2009-2010 to 2013-2014: incidence, recurrence, and mechanisms. Am J Sports Med. (2015) 43:2654–62. doi: 10.1177/0363546515599634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Dick RW. Is there a gender difference in concussion incidence and outcomes? Br J Sports Med. (2009) (43 Suppl. 1):i46–50. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2009.058172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Frommer LJ, Gurka KK, Cross KM, Ingersoll CD, Comstock RD, Saliba SA. Sex differences in concussion symptoms of high school athletes. J Athl Train. (2011) 46:76–84. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-46.1.76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Covassin T, Moran R, Elbin RJ. Sex differences in reported concussion injury rates and time loss from participation: an update of the national collegiate athletic association injury surveillance program from 2004–2005 through 2008–2009. J Athl Train. (2016) 51:189–94. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-51.3.05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Chamard E, Lefebvre G, Lassonde M, Theoret H. Long-term abnormalities in the corpus callosum of female concussed athletes. J Neurotrauma. (2016) 33:1220–6. doi: 10.1089/neu.2015.3948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Covassin T, Elbin RJ, Bleecker A, Lipchik A, Kontos AP. Are there differences in neurocognitive function and symptoms between male and female soccer players after concussions? Am J Sports Med. (2013) 41:2890–5. doi: 10.1177/0363546513509962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fakhran S, Yaeger K, Collins M, Alhilali L. Sex differences in white matter abnormalities after mild traumatic brain injury: localization and correlation with outcome. Radiology. (2014) 272:815–23. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14132512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sollmann N, Echlin PS, Schultz V, Viher PV, Lyall AE, Tripodis Y, et al. Sex differences in white matter alterations following repetitive subconcussive head impacts in collegiate ice hockey players. Neuroimage Clin. (2018) 17:642–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Nevada State Athletic Commission. Nevada Department of Business and Industry. Results. (2015–2017). Retrieved from: http://boxing.nv.gov/

13. Bernick C, Banks S, Phillips M, Lowe M, Shin W, Obuchowski N, et al. Professional fighters brain health study: rationale and methods. Am J Epidemiol. (2013) 178:280–6. doi: 10.1093/aje/kws456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS vital signs. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2006) 21:623–43. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Iverson GL, Brooks BL, Ashton Rennison VL. Minimal gender differences on the CNS vital signs computerized neurocognitive battery. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. (2014) 21:36–42. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2012.721149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. (2012) 62:774–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bernick C, Banks S. What boxing tells us about repetitive head trauma and the brain. Alzheimer's Res Ther. (2013) 5:23. doi: 10.1186/alzrt177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bernick C, Banks SJ, Shin W, Obuchowski N, Butler S, Noback M, et al. Repeated head trauma is associated with smaller thalamic volumes and slower processing speed: the professional fighters' brain health study. Br J Sports Med. (2015) 49:1007–11. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2014-093877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lee B, Bennett LL, Bernick C, Shan G, Banks SJ. The relations among depression, cognition, and brain volume in professional boxers: a preliminary examination using brief clinical measures. J Head Trauma Rehabil. (2019) 34:E29–E39. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Schultz V, Stern RA, Tripodis Y, Stamm J, Wrobel P, Lepage C, et al. Age at first exposure to repetitive head impacts is associated with smaller thalamic volumes in former professional American football players. J Neurotrauma. (2018) 35:278–85. doi: 10.1089/neu.2017.5145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Marwha D, Halari M, Eliot L. Meta-analysis reveals a lack of sexual dimorphism in human amygdala volume. Neuroimage. (2017) 147:282–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Iscan Z, Jin TB, Kendrick A, Szeglin B, Lu H, Trivedi M, et al. Test–retest reliability of FreeSurfer measurements within and between sites: effects of visual approval process. Hum Brain Mapp. (2015) 36:3472–85. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wright PW, Pyakurel A, Vaida FF, Price RW, Lee E, Peterson J, et al. Putamen volume and its clinical and neurological correlates in primary HIV infection. AIDS. (2016) 30:1789–94. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0000000000001103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Maki PM, Sundermann E. Hormone therapy and cognitive function. Hum Reprod Update. (2009) 15:667–81. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dmp022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Mordecai KL, Rubin LH, Maki PM. Effects of menstrual cycle phase and oral contraceptive use on verbal memory. Horm Behav. (2008) 54:286–93. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kimm D, Thiel DV. Hand speed measurements in boxing. Procedia Eng. (2015) 112:502–6. doi: 10.1016/j.proeng.2015.07.232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Morris JS, Link J, Martin JC, Carrier DR. Sexual dimorphism in human arm power and force: implications for sexual selection on fighting ability. J Exp Biol. (2019) 223:jeb212365. doi: 10.1242/jeb.212365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Stephen SJ, Shan G, Banks SJ, Bernick C, Bennett LL. The relationship between fighting style, cognition, and regional brain volume in professional combatants: a preliminary examination using brief neurocognitive measures. J Head Trauma Rehabil. (2019) 35:E280–7. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sports-related concussion, repetitive head impacts, boxing, sex differences, mixed martial arts, martial arts

Citation: Bennett LL, Stephen SJ, Bernick C, Shan G and Banks SJ (2020) Sex Moderates the Relationship That Number of Professional Fights Has With Cognition and Brain Volumes. Front. Neurol. 11:574458. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.574458

Received: 22 June 2020; Accepted: 25 September 2020;
Published: 29 October 2020.

Edited by:

Michael E. Buckland, The University of Sydney, Australia

Reviewed by:

Christine M. Baugh, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, United States
Sarah C. Hellewell, The University of Sydney, Australia

Copyright © 2020 Bennett, Stephen, Bernick, Shan and Banks. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lauren L. Bennett, Lauren.Bennett@Hoag.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.