Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Neurol., 01 March 2021
Sec. Dementia and Neurodegenerative Diseases
This article is part of the Research Topic Dementia in Low and Middle Income Countries View all 37 articles

Association of Leisure Activities With Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Older Adults in Colombia: A SABE-Based Study

  • 1Global Brain Health Institute, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 2Global Brain Health Institute, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 3Escuela de Gobierno, Universidad de los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 4Unidad de Servicios de Salud Occidente de Kennedy, Servicio de Neurología, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Mercer's Institute for Successful Ageing, St. James's Hospital, Dublin, Ireland

Observational and interventional studies suggest that participation in leisure activities may help protect against cognitive decline in older people. This study aimed to examine the association between participation in leisure activities and cognitive impairment in older adults in Colombia. Data for this study were derived from the Colombian National Survey of Aging (SABE 2015), a cross-sectional survey with a sample size of 23,694 older adults representing the total population (mean age, 70.8 years; 57.3% females). Cognitive impairment was classified as cognitive impairment without dementia (CIWD) and dementia, according to the revised version of the Folstein Mini-Mental State Examination and the Lawton and Brody functional scale. Leisure activities were evaluated using six items of a questionnaire. Sex-stratified multinomial regression models were used to analyze the association of leisure activities with CIWD and dementia after adjusting for educational attainment, literacy, and other potential confounders. In adjusted models for men, leisure activities in later life were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (odds ratio [OR], 0.73; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.68–0.78) and dementia (OR, 0,52; 95% CI, 0.48–0.58). For women, leisure activities in later life were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (OR, 0.72; 95% CI, 0.66–0.78) and dementia (OR, 0.48; 95% CI, 0.43–0.53). The findings suggest that greater participation in leisure activities in later life may act as a protective factor against CIWD and dementia among older adults in Colombia, independent of educational attainment and literacy.

Introduction

With a rise in global life expectancy, the prevalence of dementia is rising. A new diagnosis of dementia is reported every 3 s. Currently, the disease affects more than 50 million people globally (1), 50% of those living in middle- and low-income countries. It is expected that this number will rise to 63% in 2030 and 68% in 2050 (2), with the Americas being one of the most affected regions with increasing prevalence and incidence rates of dementia (3).

Several protective factors for dementia have been described in the literature (4). Leisure activities, a term that refers to a range of tasks and activities outside work-related activities is a promising factor for dementia prevention (5). Leisure activities can have a multidimensional profile, manifesting mental, social, and physical involvement (6). These activities are attractive, pleasant, and motivating to the individual and are more likely to be sustained over time (7, 8). Leisure activities usually constitute a relatively large part of daily life post-retirement and may provide mental stimulation, social engagement, and physical activity (6). Systematic reviews have found evidence suggesting that participation in leisure activities might significantly contribute to the prevention of later-life cognitive decline as a risk-preventing factor (5, 9). The most common categories associated with the preventive effect of leisure activities are cognitive, physical, and social activities (5, 1012). Despite these benefits, there is a lack of standard definitions, measures, and methods for studying the role of leisure activities and their protective effects.

The most common explanatory mechanism for the preventive effect of leisure activities is that of the cognitive reserve theory (5, 13). The cognitive reserve theory suggests that innate intelligence or life experiences such as educational or occupational attainments may supply a reserve in the form of skill sets or repertoires that empower the brain to tolerate atrophies and insults and, as a result, allow some people to cope with progressing dementia better than others and delay symptom onset (14, 15). The cognitive reserve cannot be observed or directly measured, and proxy measures such as education, premorbid intelligence (IQ), linguistic ability, and occupational complexity are often used (14). Several studies have proposed that engagement in leisure activities may result in more functionally efficient or resilient cognitive networks or in recruiting alternate networks, providing a cognitive reserve that prevents or delays the onset of dementia (1618). In this regard, it is unclear whether a high cognitive reserve delays symptoms onset, but any noticeable delay, for example, 1 or 2 years, would translate into tremendous public health benefits by reducing the prevalence of dementia in the population (14). While characteristics such as education and intelligence are relatively stable from young adulthood, there is increasing interest in the role of leisure activities in building up a cognitive reserve (14).

Although maintaining or building a cognitive reserve is a possible mechanism for dementia prevention (5, 9, 14), culture, poverty, and inequality are key obstacles to, and drivers of, the need for changes concerning the cognitive reserve. Sections of the society that are most deprived of basic needs, like low and middle income countries (LMIC), require these changes and will greatly benefit from them (4). It has been suggested that a low socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with less access to physical (19) and cognitive stimulating activities (20). In Latin America, low levels of education, high rates of brain injury, poor diet, a sedentary lifestyle, and a high risk of cardiovascular diseases are among the main risk factors behind the rapid growth in the number of people with dementia (21); however, there is a high potential for dementia prevention in the region (4, 22). Thus, there is an urgent need for dementia prevention policies in Latin America to reduce the burden and economic costs of dementia in the region (2325).

In this study, we detail the association between participation in leisure activities and the risk of developing cognitive impairment without dementia (CIWD), or dementia, using the data from the Colombian National Survey of Aging (SABE 2015) (26). There are few papers focusing on cognitive outcomes from Colombian SABE, the effect of education in early life and the probability of cognitive impairment in later life in Colombia has been recently published (27) as well as the role of gait speed in dementia (28) and the mediating effect of physical fitness on cognitive functioning (29). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to analyze this topic in Colombia using this dataset.

Materials and Methods

We conducted a secondary data analysis of a cross-sectional study using data from SABE Colombia 2015. SABE Colombia 2015 is the first study in Colombia representative of the national population of those aged at least 60 years. Individuals for SABE Colombia were selected following a multistage area probability sample design with a total sample size of 23,694 participants from 244 municipalities (urban and rural). Data collection took place between April and September 2015 (26). This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Universidad de los Andes, code ID 1114/2019.

The dependent variable was cognitive impairment, as measured in SABE 2015. This survey used the revised version of the Folstein Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), a validated international scale translated to Spanish (30). A cut-off point of 12 or less indicated cognitive impairment, while a score of 13 or above was normal (26). For dementia, functional impairment was evaluated using four items of the Lawton and Brody functional scale (31): phone use, transport use, handling medicines, and management of money. The lack of functionality for doing at least two of the four activities was defined as dependence and indication of dementia (32). If functional impairment was not detected, participants were classified as CIWD.

Independent variables were educational attainment, literacy (reading and writing), and participation in leisure activities. Educational attainment was established based on the 11 categories reported by SABE 2015. We classified participants into three groups in accordance with their educational qualifications: those who had an educational qualification lower than completion of primary school, those who had completed primary school, and those who had sought further qualifications after completion of high school. Literacy was reported using a yes/no question. Using an 18-item questionnaire reported by SABE 2015, we constructed a 6-item continuous variable for participation in leisure activities, among which five items were considered cognitively challenging (reading, solving math problems, solving puzzles, tabletop games, attending classes or courses) and one item that included physical activity.

Confounding variables included were age in years, area of residence (urban, rural), marital status (married/with a partner, separated/widower, single), living alone (yes/no), health insurance (subsidized, contribute, no affiliation), antecedent of forced displacement during life (yes/no), country region (residence in one of the six geographic areas of Colombia: Atlantic, Oriental, Orinoquia and Amazonia, Bogotá, Central, Pacific), skin color (light, medium, dark), pension (yes/no), lifetime occupation (manual or dependent worker, boss or independent worker, none), salary (<1 minimum wage, between 1 and 2 minimum wages, more than 2 minimum wages), and physical capital tercile (13).

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The present study included the total SABE survey sample: participants aged 60 years and above, non-institutionalized, capable of communicating with the research team and able to provide written informed consent. If the MMSE was below 13 a proxy interview was developed (26).

Participants with missing data for leisure activities participation were excluded from this study.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive analyses of data were conducted using frequencies and percentages for qualitative variables and means and standard deviations for quantitative variables. Unadjusted odds ratio (OR) was calculated using multinomial regression logistic analyses for dependent and independent variables. Multicollinearity was test using variance inflation factor (VIF). Finally, sex-stratified multinomial regression models were used to examine the association of leisure activities with CI and dementia after adjusting for educational attainment, literacy, and other potential confounders. Results are presented as OR with 95% confidence intervals. All analyses were performed using STATA release 16® (StataCorp LP, College Station, USA).

Results

The sample size of the survey was 23,694 participants, 31 participants with missing data for leisure activities were excluded from this study, the final sample size was 23.663 with a mean age of 70.82 years (standard deviation [SD] 8.20); 57.3% were women. Most participants had an educational qualification lower than completion of primary school (62.6%), followed by those who had completed primary school (26.8%) and sought further qualifications after completion of high school (10.5%). Literacy was reported as 78.2 and 77.1% for reading and writing, respectively. The prevalence of CIWD was 8.9 and 10.8% for dementia (Table 1). The mean number of leisure activities participated in was 2.02 (SD 1.59) by those with normal cognition, 0.93 (SD 1.17) by those with CIWD, and 0.51 (SD 0.86) by those with dementia (Figure 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive analyses.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean leisure activities participation. CIWD, Cognitive impairment without dementia.

Unadjusted OR estimation showed that people who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school were more likely to develop CIWD (OR 10.18; CI 7.33–14.15) and dementia (OR 8.54; CI 6.52–11.17) than those who had sought further educational qualifications after high school. Reading and writing illiteracy were associated with a higher likelihood of having either CIWD (OR 4.70, CI 4.28–5.17; OR 4.57, CI 4.16–5.02, respectively), or dementia (OR 6.43, CI 5.90–7.02; OR 6.48, CI 5.95–7.07, respectively). Participation in leisure activities was a protective factor against CIWD and dementia (OR 0.56, CI 0.54–0.59; OR 0.36, CI 0.34–0.38, respectively) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Non-adjusted odds ratio for independent variables.

Analysis of multicollinearity showed high correlation between reading and writing literacy, the former was removed from the multinomial regression models. Table 3 shows the results for sex-stratified multinomial regression models adjusted for potential confounders. In model 1, those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 7.27, CI 4.19–12.61) as well as those who had completed primary school (OR 3.02, CI 1.73–5.30) were significantly more likely to have CIWD than their counterparts with higher educational qualifications. Similar results were found for the association between those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 3.30, CI 2.06–5.28) and dementia in comparison with those who had the highest level of education. Model 2 for CIWD persisted with the association for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 3.70, CI 2.12–6.47) and those who had completed primary school (OR 2.65, CI 1.51–4.64). Leisure activities were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (OR 0.73, CI 0.68–0.78). For dementia, in model 2, the effect of educational attainment disappeared, and inability to read was significantly associated with greater risk (OR 2.77, CI 2.29–3.34). Participation in leisure activities decreased the risk of dementia (OR 0.52, CI 0.48– 0.58).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multinomial sex-stratified regression models.

In adjusted models for men, model 1 for CIWD showed significant results for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 4.57, CI 2.71–7.70) and those who had completed primary school (OR 2.47, CI 1.45–4.21). For dementia, results were significant for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 3.46, CI 1.97–6.07). Model 2 for CIWD persisted with the association for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 2.42, CI 1.41–4.14) and those who had completed primary school (OR 2.20, CI 1.28–3.77) and was significant for the inability to read (OR 1.94, CI 1.57–2.41). Leisure activities were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (OR 0.72, CI 0.66–0.78). For dementia, in model 2, the effect of educational attainment disappeared; results for reading literacy were significant (OR 1.68, CI 1.34–2.11). Leisure activities decreased the risk of dementia (OR 0.48, CI 0.43–0.53).

Discussion

This study's main objective was to investigate the effect of participation in leisure activities on reducing the risk of developing CIWD or dementia in Colombian older adults. We found that for CIWD, participation in leisure activities is linked with educational attainment as well as the ability to read. For dementia, participation in leisure activities in later life might impart a protective effect independent of educational attainment for men and women.

In this sample, more than two-thirds of the participants had low educational attainment. Low educational attainment has been associated with a higher risk of dementia and CIWD (33, 34). Our results suggest that when adjusted for leisure activities, the effect of education is significant for CIWD but not for dementia. Education attainment is an essential factor for cognitive reserve building (35); nonetheless, cognitive reserve is not only determined by education but also by other lifestyle factors in which leisure activities are included (4, 36). In individuals with CIWD, educational attainment has been shown to affect intellectual development during the entire adult life span. However, it was not associated with the cognitive decline rate, in contrast with mid/late-life cognitive activities, which was beneficial and delayed the onset of dementia (37). Previous studies had shown the protective effect of cognitive reserve factors such as education and literacy in LMIC (38). Our study contributes with evidence from this region on the potential role of leisure activities on cognitive reserve building, but further research is needed to clarify this association.

Illiteracy was associated as an independent risk factor in non-adjusted analysis for both CIWD and dementia in previous findings (39). Our study included illiteracy while evaluating writing and reading abilities separately; even though the effect of this difference is not known, it is presumed that acquiring one aspect of literacy over the other could lead to differential dementia risks (39). The ability to read provides the means to acquire and structure new knowledge for language skills and reinforces working memory, visual memory, visuospatial processing, and visuomotor skills (4042). Our results indicate that an inability to read was associated with a higher risk of dementia independent of educational attainment for women; contradictory results have been found in the literature (39, 43).

The study findings suggest that greater participation in leisure activities in later life may be a protective factor against CIWD and dementia among older adults in Colombia after adjusting for education attainment, illiteracy, and other socioeconomic factors covariates. Previous studies have shown the protective effect of participation in leisure activities with a reduced risk of CIWD and dementia (9) and that the relationship between leisure activities and cognition is not driven by educational attainment (44) or SES (45). In contrast to these findings, a recent study based on longitudinal follow-up failed to support that leisure activity participation can lower the risk of dementia but instead suggested that reduction in activity participation indicates possible prodromal dementia (46).

This study has several limitations. First, the study was based on a yes/no self-report questionnaire and frequency, intensity, or quality of the activities was measured. Thus, it is not possible to elucidate the underlying mechanisms for the protective effect described here (12). A study found that participation in several activities with varying cognitive complexity levels was a better predictor of cognitive impairment than increased activity frequency (47). We need to know the amount and frequency that generates a protective effect to be able to design a suitable intervention. Second, although gender, demographic, and SES covariates were adjusted, other known risk factors for dementia such as hypertension, diabetes, stroke, and depression were not included (48). In a cohort of Swedish older adults, moderate to high engagement levels in mental, social, and physical leisure activities were associated with a dramatically decreased risk of dementia in people with diabetes (49). Third, education was measured by years of school completed, and there was no measure of the quality of education. Higher literacy may be a more sensitive marker of cognitive reserve than higher education (50); thus we cannot rule out the possibility that leisure activities' protective effect is dependent on education. Further research is needed to clarify the association between education, leisure activities, and cognitive impairment. Furthermore, data from this study comes from a cross-sectional survey; long term, population-based or representative cohort studies are needed to estimate with more precision the role of lifestyle choices like leisure activities might play in reducing dementia risk (51).

Despite these limitations, this is the first study that demonstrates the protective effect of leisure activities using a survey representative of the Colombian population to the best of our knowledge. The public health implications from this finding are related to the importance of increasing late-life participation in leisure activities, their potential role in late-life cognition, and its benefits in people with and without cognitive impairment (52). Nevertheless, further research is needed to determine the amount of exposure, intervention period, and frequency for designing an effective intervention (53). The available research evidence suggests that it is not too late to increase physical and cognitive activity in old age (14) and policymaking for dementia primary prevention needs evidence for non-pharmacological interventions aiming to increase cognitive reserve (54). More research is required to stablish more reliable conclusions for dementia preventive factors and potential interventions.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the user agreement does not permit sharing the data directly. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the Colombian Ministry of Health (repositorio@minsalud.gov.co).

Ethics Statement

This study was a secondary data analysis approved by the Institutional Review Board of Universidad de los Andes, code ID 1114/2019. And it was consider an investigation without risk according to Colombian Ministry of Health laws.

Author Contributions

AG and DL were responsible for the study design and data analyses. AG was responsible for drafting the manuscript and interpretation of the findings. DL and BL provided critical feedback on drafts, and approved the final manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Alzheimer's Association and Alzheimer's Society (GBHI ALZ UK-20-640663). AG is an Atlantic Fellow for Equity in Brain Health and received fellowships from the Global Brain Health Institute.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Alzheimer's Disease International. From Plan to Impact: Progress Towards Targets of the Global Action Plan on Dementia. London: Alzheimer's Disease International (2018).

Google Scholar

2. Alzheimer's Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2015- The Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends. London: Alzheimer's Disease International (2015).

Google Scholar

3. Organizacion Panamericana de la Salud ADI. Demencia: Una Prioridad de Salud Publica. Washington: Organizacion Panamericana de la Salud (2013).

Google Scholar

4. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. (2020) 396:413–46. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Fallahpour M, Borell L, Luborsky M, Nygård L. Leisure-activity participation to prevent later-life cognitive decline: a systematic review. Scand J Occup Ther. (2015) 23:162–97. doi: 10.3109/11038128.2015.1102320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Karp A, Paillard-Borg S, Wang HX, Silverstein M, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2006) 21:65–73. doi: 10.1159/000089919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Wa Lam LC, Cheng ST. Maintaining long-term adherence to lifestyle interventions for cognitive health in late life. Int Psychogeriatr. (2013) 25:171–3. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212001603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Cheng ST, Chow PK, Yu ECS, Chan ACM. Leisure activities alleviate depressive symptoms in nursing home residents with very mild or mild dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2012) 20:904–8. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182423988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Yates LA, Ziser S, Spector A, Orrell M. Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. (2016) 28:1791–806. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216001137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Tolppanen AM, Solomon A, Kulmala J, Kåreholt I, Ngandu T, Rusanen M, et al. Leisure-time physical activity from mid- to late life, body mass index, and risk of dementia. Alzheimers Dement. (2015) 11:434–43.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Akbaraly TN, Portet F, Fustinoni S, Dartigues JF, Artero S, Rouaud O, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: results from the three-city study. Neurology. (2009) 73:854–61. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b7849b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sajeev G, Weuve J, Jackson JW, Vanderweele TJ, Bennett DA, Grodstein F, et al. Late-life cognitive activity and dementia. Epidemiology. (2016) 27:732–42. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2003) 25:625–33. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Cheng ST. Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: the role of physical and cognitive activities. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2016) 18:85. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0721-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2006) 20:112–7. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000213815.20177.19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang M-X, Manly J, Stern Y. Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology. (2001) 57:2236–42. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.12.2236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Crowe M, Andel R, Pedersen NL, Johansson B, Gatz M. Does participation in leisure activities lead to reduced risk of Alzheimer's disease? A prospective study of Swedish twins. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2003) 58:249–55. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.5.P249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth MEJ. Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med. (2003) 56:785–92. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00075-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Cerin E, Leslie E. How socio-economic status contributes to participation in leisure-time physical activity. Soc Sci Med. (2008) 66:2596–609. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cassarino M, Setti A. Environment as ‘Brain Training': a review of geographical and physical environmental influences on cognitive ageing. Ageing Res Rev. (2015) 23:167–82. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gonzalez FJ, Gaona C, Quintero M, Chavez CA, Selga J, Maestre GE. Building capacity for dementia care in Latin America and the Caribbean Francisco. Dement e Neuropsychol. (2014) 8:310–6. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642014DN84000002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntley J, Livingston G. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. Lancet Glob Heal. (2019) 7:e596–603. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30074-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Custodio N, Wheelock A, Thumala D, Slachevsky A. Dementia in Latin America: epidemiological evidence and implications for public policy. Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:221. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Baez S, Ibáñez A. Dementia in Latin America: an emergent silent tsunami. Front Aging Neurosci. (2016) 8:253. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Parra MA, Baez S, Allegri R, Nitrini R, Lopera F, Slachevsky A, et al. Dementia in Latin America Assessing the present and envisioning the future. Neurology. (2018) 90:222–31. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gomez F, Corchuelo J, Curcio CL, Calzada MT, Mendez F. SABE Colombia: survey on health, well-being, and aging in Colombia - study design and protocol. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. (2016) 2016:7910205. doi: 10.1155/2016/7910205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Donovan GO, Hamer M, Sarmiento OL, Hessel P. Education in early life markedly reduces the probability of cognitive impairment in later life in Colombia. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:17685. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74822-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Garcia-Cifuentes E, Márquez I, Vasquez D, Aguillon D, Borda MG, Lopera F, et al. The role of gait speed in dementia: a secondary analysis from the SABE Colombia study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2020) 1–8. doi: 10.1159/000510494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Pérez-Sousa MÁ, del Pozo-Cruz J, Olivares PR, Cano-Gutiérrez CA, Izquierdo M, Ramírez-Vélez R. Role for physical fitness in the association between age and cognitive function in older adults: a mediation analysis of the SABE Colombia study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:751. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18020751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Icaza MG, Albala C. Minimental State Examinations (MMSE) del Estudio de Demencia en Chile: Analisis Estadistico. Washington, DC: OPS Investig en Salud Pública Doc Técnicos. (1999) p. 7.

Google Scholar

31. Luck T, Luppa M, Wiese B, Maier W, Den Bussche H Van, Eisele M, et al. Prediction of incident dementia: Impact of impairment in instrumental activities of daily living and mild cognitive impairment-results from the german study on ageing, cognition, and dementia in primary care patients. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2012) 20:943–54. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31825c09bc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ministerio de Salud y Protección Social. Oficina de Protección Social-MINSALUD, Departamento Administrativo de ciencia tecnología e innovación-C-. SABE Colombia 2015: Estudio Nacional de Salud, Bienestar y Envejecimiento. Bogotá: Minsalud. (2015).

Google Scholar

33. Caamaño-Isorna F, Corral M, Montes-Martínez A, Takkouche B. Education and dementia: a meta-analytic study. Neuroepidemiology. (2006) 26:226–32. doi: 10.1159/000093378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sharp ES, Gatz M. Relationship between education and dementia an updated systematic review. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2011) 25:289–304. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318211c83c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Whalley LJ, Deary IJ, Appleton CL, Starr JM. Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Res Rev. (2004) 3:369–82. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Polidori MC, Nelles G, Pientka L. Prevention of dementia: focus on lifestyle. Int J Alzheimers Dis. (2010) 2010:393579. doi: 10.4061/2010/393579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, Machulda M, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, et al. Association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with cognitive decline in the older population. JAMA Neurol. (2014) 71:1017–24. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Prince M, Acosta D, Ferri CP, Guerra M, Huang Y, Rodriguez JJL, et al. Dementia incidence and mortality in middle-income countries, and associations with indicators of cognitive reserve: a 10/66 Dementia Research Group population-based cohort study. Lancet. (2012) 380:50–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60399-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Arce Rentería M, Vonk JMJ, Felix G, Avila JF, Zahodne LB, Dalchand E, et al. Illiteracy, dementia risk, and cognitive trajectories among older adults with low education. Neurology. (2019) 93:E2247–56. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000008587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bramão I, Mendonça A, Faísca L, Ingvar M, Peterson KM, Reis A. The impact of reading and writing skills on a visuo-motor integration task: a comparison between illiterate and literate subjects. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2007) 13:359–64. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Petersson KM, Reis A, Ingvar M. Cognitive processing in literate and illiterate subjects: a review of some recent behavioral and functional neuroimaging data. Scand J Psychol. (2001) 42:251–67. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Kosmidis MH, Zafiri M, Politimou N. Literacy versus formal schooling: influence. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2011) 26:575–82. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Zhang M, Katzman R, Salmon D, Jin H, Cai G, Wang Z, et al. The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in Shanghai, China: impact of age, gender, and education. Ann Neurol. (1990) 27:428–37. doi: 10.1002/ana.410270412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Peterson RL, Gilsanz P, George KM, Ackley S, Glymour MM, Mungas DM, et al. Differences in association of leisure time activities and cognition in a racially/ethnically diverse cohort of older adults: findings from the KHANDLE study. Alzheimers Dement Transl Res Clin Interv. (2020) 6:1–9. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Singh-Manoux A, Richards M, Marmot M. Leisure activities and cognitive function in middle age: Evidence from the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. (2003) 57:907–13. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.11.907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Sommerlad A, Sabia S, Livingston G, Kivimäki M, Lewis G, Singh-Manoux A. Leisure activity participation and risk of dementia: 18 year follow-up of the Whitehall II Study. Neurology. (2020) 95:e2803–15. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Carlson MC, Parisi JM, Xia J, Xue QL, Rebok GW, Bandeen-Roche K, et al. Lifestyle activities and memory: variety may be the spice of life the women's health and aging study II. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2012) 18:286–94. doi: 10.1017/S135561771100169X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Frankish H, Horton R. Prevention and management of dementia: a priority for public health. Lancet. (2017) 390:2614–5. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31756-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Marseglia A, Wang HX, Rizzuto D, Fratiglioni L, Xu W. Participating in mental, social, and physical leisure activities and having a rich social network reduce the incidence of diabetes-related dementia in a cohort of Swedish older adults. Diabetes Care. (2019) 42:232–9. doi: 10.2337/dc18-1428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kaup AR, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Satterfield S, Metti AL, Ayonayon HN, et al. Older adults with limited literacy are at increased risk for likely dementia. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2014) 69:900–6. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Henderson VW, Elias MF. Leisure activity for dementia prevention: more work to be done. Neurology. (2020) 95:895–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Wells RE, Kerr C, Dossett ML, Danhauer SC, Sohl SJ, Sachs BC, et al. Can adults with mild cognitive impairment build cognitive reserve and learn mindfulness meditation? Qualitative theme analyses from a small pilot study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2019) 70:825–42. doi: 10.3233/JAD-190191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, Kobayashi-Cuya KE, Kobayashi M, Takebayashi T, et al. Can cognitive leisure activity prevent cognitive decline in older adults? A systematic review of intervention studies. Geriatr Gerontol Int. (2019) 19:469–82. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Wu YT, Fratiglioni L, Matthews FE, Lobo A, Breteler MMB, Skoog I, et al. Dementia in western Europe: epidemiological evidence and implications for policy making. Lancet Neurol. (2016) 15:116–24. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00092-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: prevention, cognitive reserve, leisure activities, dementia, cognitive impairment

Citation: Guerrero Barragán A, Lucumí D and Lawlor B (2021) Association of Leisure Activities With Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Older Adults in Colombia: A SABE-Based Study. Front. Neurol. 12:629251. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.629251

Received: 24 November 2020; Accepted: 26 January 2021;
Published: 01 March 2021.

Edited by:

Huali Wang, Peking University Sixth Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Andreas Ihle, Université de Genève, Switzerland
Robinson Ramírez-Vélez, Public University of Navarre, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Guerrero Barragán, Lucumí and Lawlor. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alejandra Guerrero Barragán, alejandra.guerrero@gbhi.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.