Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 23 December 2021
Sec. Neurotrauma
This article is part of the Research Topic Long-Term Consequences of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury View all 12 articles

A Preliminary DTI Tractography Study of Developmental Neuroplasticity 5–15 Years After Early Childhood Traumatic Brain Injury

\nElisabeth A. Wilde,,,
Elisabeth A. Wilde1,2,3,4*Ilirjana Hyseni,Ilirjana Hyseni2,5Hannah M. Lindsey,Hannah M. Lindsey1,6Jessica FaberJessica Faber7James M. McHenryJames M. McHenry8Erin D. Bigler,Erin D. Bigler1,6Brian D. BiekmanBrian D. Biekman2Laura L. HollowellLaura L. Hollowell2Stephen R. McCauley,,Stephen R. McCauley2,3,5Jill V. Hunter,,Jill V. Hunter2,4,9Linda Ewing-CobbsLinda Ewing-Cobbs8Mary E. AitkenMary E. Aitken10Marianne MacLeodMarianne MacLeod2Zili D. Chu,Zili D. Chu4,9Linda J. Noble-HaeussleinLinda J. Noble-Haeusslein11Harvey S. Levin,,Harvey S. Levin2,3,5
  • 1Department of Neurology, Traumatic Brain Injury and Concussion Center, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
  • 2H. Ben Taub Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, United States
  • 3Department of Neurology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, United States
  • 4Department of Radiology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, United States
  • 5Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, United States
  • 6Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, United States
  • 7Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 8Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, TX, United States
  • 9Department of Pediatric Radiology, Texas Children's Hospital, Houston, TX, United States
  • 10Department of Pediatrics, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, AR, United States
  • 11Departments of Psychology and Neurology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States

Plasticity is often implicated as a reparative mechanism when addressing structural and functional brain development in young children following traumatic brain injury (TBI); however, conventional imaging methods may not capture the complexities of post-trauma development. The present study examined the cingulum bundles and perforant pathways using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) in 21 children and adolescents (ages 10–18 years) 5–15 years after sustaining early childhood TBI in comparison with 19 demographically-matched typically-developing children. Verbal memory and executive functioning were also evaluated and analyzed in relation to DTI metrics. Beyond the expected direction of quantitative DTI metrics in the TBI group, we also found qualitative differences in the streamline density of both pathways generated from DTI tractography in over half of those with early TBI. These children exhibited hypertrophic cingulum bundles relative to the comparison group, and the number of tract streamlines negatively correlated with age at injury, particularly in the late-developing anterior regions of the cingulum; however, streamline density did not relate to executive functioning. Although streamline density of the perforant pathway was not related to age at injury, streamline density of the left perforant pathway was significantly and positively related to verbal memory scores in those with TBI, and a moderate effect size was found in the right hemisphere. DTI tractography may provide insight into developmental plasticity in children post-injury. While traditional DTI metrics demonstrate expected relations to cognitive performance in group-based analyses, altered growth is reflected in the white matter structures themselves in some children several years post-injury. Whether this plasticity is adaptive or maladaptive, and whether the alterations are structure-specific, warrants further investigation.

Introduction

The concepts of increased vulnerability and neuroplasticity have been used to understand recovery from early traumatic brain injury (TBI), yet the interaction of the timing of brain insult with developmental factors that influence recovery remains unclear (1, 2). Some have postulated early childhood TBI may critically disrupt subsequent synaptic organization and modify neural network formation, whereas later TBI may have more localized effects (35). In contrast, some children with acquired injury early in life have exhibited remarkable resiliency, suggesting a capacity for reorganization may also be present (6).

Diffuse axonal injury (DAI) is a consequence of wide spread deformation to white matter (WM) fiber systems that often results from the biomechanics of TBI (7, 8). DAI may affect WM integrity by disrupting neural networks (9), and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), which is more sensitive to structural alteration in pediatric TBI than conventional MRI (10), is ideally suited to explore potential WM changes related to TBI and age at the time of injury. DTI measures the diffusion of water molecules in brain tissue to interrogate WM integrity or organization via several quantitative diffusion metrics, including fractional anisotropy (FA), a measure of the extent to which diffusion is restricted, and mean diffusivity (MD), a measure of the average rate of diffusion within a voxel (10). Axial (AD) and radial diffusivity (RD) are directional measures of the rate of diffusion that may be sensitive to changes in axonal integrity (11) and myelination (12), respectively. DTI tractography is a quantitative post-processing method by which discrete tract streamlines that model WM pathways are generated (13).

There is limited research into the effects of early TBI on WM integrity several years post-injury; however, a large study of children and adolescents with moderate-to-severe TBI found disruptions to the integrity of various WM tracts at the acute/subacute, postacute, and chronic (6–26 months) post-injury periods (14). Specifically, significantly lower FA and higher MD was observed across tracts in those with TBI, relative to controls, at all three post-injury periods. Post-hoc analysis revealed higher RD at each post-injury period, while AD was observed to be lower acutely but higher at later post-injury periods across the majority of pathways. This study also found that, while injury severity was a significant contributor to the extent of WM alterations early on, the amount of variance in diffusion metrics explained by injury severity decreased over time.

We acquired DTI data 5–15 years after complicated mild, moderate, or severe TBI sustained during young childhood and utilized quantitative tractography to interrogate two pathways that are implicated in the cognitive consequences of TBI (15). The late-developing cingulum bundle (CB; Figure 1A) connects the prefrontal, posterior, and limbic regions and has been associated with cognitive control and executive functioning. The perforant pathway (PP; Figure 1B) projects to the entorhinal cortex and hippocampal formation and is linked to memory functioning (16). The spatial relationship of the CB relative to the PP is demonstrated in Figure 1C. Furthermore, the CB and PP are particularly vulnerable to TBI, as they connect frontal and temporal regions of the brain that are susceptible to damage due to their situation within the anterior and cranial fossa of the skull (17). Consistent with our focus on the CB and PP, we selected tests of episodic memory and executive function that are well-validated in TBI populations (1820) for this study. Episodic memory was assessed using the California Verbal Learning Test [CVLT (21, 22)], which is a measure of list learning and immediate and delayed verbal recall. Executive functioning was assessed using the Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System [D-KEFS; (23)] Color-Word Interference Test (CWIT), which is used to measure inhibition of over-learned responses through performance across color naming (CN), word reading (WR), inhibition (IN), and inhibition/switching (IS) conditions. In this exploratory investigation, we investigated whether early TBI would adversely affect later structural development and functionality of these pathways.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Tractography of the cingulum bundle (CB) and perforant pathway (PP). Directional color maps of the right CB (A) and PP (B) are shown on the sagittal (left) and axial (right) planes, overlaid by a 3D cortical isosurface. The colors represent the 3D fiber rendering direction; red, left-to-right (along the x-axis); green, anterior-to-posterior (along the y-axis); and blue, superior-to-inferior (along the z-axis). The bottom panel (C) demonstrates the spatial relationship of the CB (orange) and PP (blue), which are overlaid on the subject's T1-weighted image. The right side of the brain is depicted in the left side of the screen in the axial views.

Materials and Methods

The data used in the present study were part of a research program on pediatric TBI that was conducted at Baylor College of Medicine and the University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at all participating sites. Written informed consent was obtained from all guardians of participants, as well as verbal assent from child participants, prior to enrollment, prior the acquisition of MRI data, and prior to the neuropsychological assessment.

Participants

The present study included 21 patients (29% female) aged 10–18 years at the time of the evaluation, who sustained a TBI between the ages of 1–8 years (mean = 4.10 ± 2.02). Although we had designed the study to focus on children injured at age 5 years or younger, difficulty in recruiting children who met the eligibility criteria (see below) necessitated that we expand the upper age at injury range to 8 years. Capability of undergoing unsedated DTI was also a consideration in specifying the youngest current age of children selected for this follow-up study, which was 10 years in the TBI group (Table 1). Patients were enrolled in the study between 5.8 and 14.7 years (mean = 9.51 ± 2.73) post-injury. Based on initial Glasgow Coma Scale [GCS (24)] scores obtained at the scene by first responders, TBI severity was classified as mild (GCS = 13–15) complicated by intracranial findings on initial CT, moderate (GCS = 9–12), and severe (GCS = 3–8). Initial GCS scores of the present sample ranged from 3 to 15 (mean = 7.86 ± 3.82), where complicated mild, moderate, and severe injuries were sustained by 14%, 19%, and 67% of TBI patients, respectively. In addition to other injury characteristics, Table 2 lists the pathology identified by acute clinical imaging for each TBI patient; the type of brain injury was heterogeneous, including 11 cases of hemorrhage or hematoma, two patients with contusions, and a single case diagnosed as DAI. The most common mechanisms of injury were falls (33%) and motor vehicle accidents (24% passenger, 10% pedestrian), although other injury mechanisms included blunt-force trauma (14%), recreational vehicle accidents (9.5%), and sport- or play-related injuries (9.5%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the present sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Injury details for each child and adolescent with traumatic brain injury.

The comparison group included 19 typically-developing children (TDC; 32% female), aged 9–17 years (mean = 13.58 ± 2.34). The groups were demographically-comparable on age at evaluation, sex, ethnicity, handedness, and socioeconomic status (SES; Table 1). SES was determined using the Socioeconomic Composite Index (SCI), according to the guidelines described by Yeates et al. (25). Exclusion criteria for both groups included contradictions to MRI, history of child abuse, and prior diagnosis of psychotic or neurologic disorders. Exclusion criteria specific to the TDC group included prior head injury that required hospitalization, whereas children were excluded from the TBI group if any head injury that required hospitalization occurred prior to or following the index injury.

Neuroimaging Protocol

Participants underwent MRI without sedation on Philips 3T Achieva scanners (Philips, Cleveland, OH) at Texas Children's Hospital in Houston (12 TBI, 11 TDC) or at the University of Arkansas in Little Rock (9 TBI, 8 TDC) using comparable platforms and software. Regular quality assurance testing was performed on both scanners, and both were consistently found to be within specification throughout the duration of the study. No systematic differences in DTI metrics derived between sites for TBI or comparison participants were observed (Supplementary Table 1). Additionally, there was no relationship between study site and the number of TBI or TDC participants recruited, χ2(1) = 0.002, p = 0.962.

DTI acquisition involved transverse multi-slice spin echo, single shot, echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequences (TR/TE = 7,305/51 ms, slice thickness/gap = 2/2 mm, matrix = 112 × 110 mm, voxel size = 1.75 × 1.75 × 2.0 mm), with diffusivities measured along 32 directions using a low/high b-value of 0/1,000 s/mm2. To achieve higher signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and improve the reproducibility of the generated tract streamlines, two DTI acquisitions were acquired and later averaged using the Philips diffusion affine registration program (26). During the averaging process, any eddy current distortion and head motion artifact were corrected. Philips fiber tracking 4.1V3 Beta 2 software [Philips, Best, The Netherlands (27)] was used to quantify and extract FA, MD, AD, and RD maps and perform the fiber tractography using a deterministic algorithm based on the fiber assignment by continuous tracking (FACT) method (28) with Runge-Kutta interpolation. Tracking was terminated in voxels when FA fell below 0.2 or if the angular threshold of 7° was met. Quantitative tractography was performed manually using multiple regions of interest (ROIs) for the CB and PP in each hemisphere, utilizing protocols previously published by our group and others (15, 16, 2931), which yield a point score for each identifiable streamline within a ROI. Details of the ROI selection and tract isolation procedure are provided in Figure 2. It is important to note that the number of streamline points (i.e., streamline density) does not reflect the true underlying fiber density but is dependent on the fiber tracking algorithm used (32).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Delination of regions-of-interest for the CB and PP on the FA color map. Fiber tracts of cingulum bundle (CB) and perforant pathway (PP) are isolated via regions of interest (ROIs). Two ROIs of the CB are selected on the coronal plane. The first ROI of the CB (A1) is selected at the level of the fornix body four slices posterior to the second ROI (A2), which is selected at the level of the fornix column. Three ROIs are selected on the coronal plane for the PP. The first ROI is selected on the most anterior slice of the splenium of the corpus callosum (B1), the second ROI of PP is selected three slices anterior to the first (B2), and the third ROI is selected six slices anterior to the first ROI (B3). The right side of the brain is depicted in the left side of the screen. The colors represent the 3D fiber rendering direction; red, left-to-right (along the x-axis); green, anterior-to-posterior (along the y-axis); and blue, superior-to-inferior (along the z-axis). FA, fractional anisotropy.

To ensure inter-operator reliability of DTI protocols, two experienced raters independently analyzed a subset of the study data (6 TBI and 6 TDC), and all measures were analyzed twice by the same rater. Shrout-Fleiss reliability statistics were used to provide intra-class correlation coefficients (ICCs) as a measure to establish intra- and inter-rater reliability; ICCs for each measurement were above 0.97. All DTI analysts were masked to the participants' performance on neuropsychological testing.

Cognitive Assessment

The following assessments were administered as part of a larger battery of neuropsychological tests. All participants completed the cognitive assessment on the same day as they underwent neuroimaging. All of the test administrators were masked to the results of DTI analyses.

Verbal learning and memory was assessed using the standard five-trial version of the CVLT–Children's version [CVLT-C (21)] for children aged 5–16 years, and the CVLT–Second edition [CVLT-II (22)] for participants aged 17–18 years. List recall performance across the five learning trials (Trials 1–5), short-delay free recall (SDFR)–requiring recall of the word list following a brief delay after the initial five learning trials–and long-delay free (LDFR)–requiring recall of the word list after a 20-min delay–were analyzed. Raw scores from the CVLT were standardized using demographically-corrected normative data provided in test administration manual, and the resulting standardized scores [T-scores (mean = 50 ± 10) for Trials 1–5; z-scores (mean = 0.0 ± 1.0) for SDFR and LDFR] were used in all statistical analyses.

The D-KEFS CWIT was used to assess executive function, where the first two conditions (CN, WR) assess baseline competency in identifying the color of patches and reading color words in black ink. The third condition (IN) requires the examinee to name the color of ink that color words are printed in while inhibiting naming the color word itself. The fourth condition (IS) measures cognitive flexibility and the ability to inhibit an overlearned response (33). Specifically, this condition requires the examinee to switch between naming the color of ink that color words are printed in and reading color words, which are printed in ink of a different color (e.g., “red” printed in green ink). Raw scores from the CWIT were standardized using demographically-corrected normative data provided in test administration manual, and the resulting scaled scores (mean = 10 ± 3) were used in all statistical analyses.

Statistical Analysis

All continuous demographic variables were examined for normality and homogeneity of variance prior to between-group comparisons. Independent t-tests were used to assess group differences in age at evaluation and SES (i.e., SCI z-score). Distributions of sex, handedness, and ethnicity between groups were compared using Pearson's chi-square or Fisher's exact tests, appropriately. Within-group correlational analyses were also conducted to determine if relationships existed between sex (point-biserial r), age at evaluation, and SES (Pearson's r) with all cognitive and DTI variables.

Due to violations of homogeneity of variance for a number of the cognitive testing and DTI variables, age at evaluation, sex, and SES were not included as covariates in our models; rather, two-tailed Welch's t-tests (34) were used to compare performance on cognitive measures (CVLT: Trials 1–5, SDFR, and LDFR; CWIT: CN, WR, IN, and IS) and DTI parameters (FA, MD, and number of streamline points) of the CB and PP between TBI and TDC groups. All comparisons included the calculation of 95% confidence intervals (CIs) and Hedges's g as a measure of effect size (35), where |g| ≥ 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 are considered small, moderate, and large effects, respectively (36).

Partial correlation analyses were conducted within the TBI group to explore the relation between DTI parameters (streamline points, FA, MD) extracted from the CB and PP and cognitive functioning. Partial correlations were also used to explore the relationship between integrity of the CB and PP and age at injury. The effects of age at evaluation, sex, and SES were statistically controlled for in all correlational analyses, and squared semipartial correlations are included as estimates of effect size, where rsp2 ≥ 0.01, 0.09, and 0.25 are considered small, moderate, and large effects, respectively (36).

Supplementary analyses were conducted to explore differences in measures of directional diffusivity (AD, RD) extracted from the CB and PP between groups using Welch's t-tests, as well as relationships between AD and RD with cognitive functioning and age at injury using partial correlations. As with the primary analyses, the effects of age at evaluation, sex, and SES were statistically controlled for in all correlational analyses, and Hedges's g and squared semipartial correlations are included as estimates of effect size for between-group comparisons and partial correlations, respectively.

Given the exploratory nature of the study, formal correction for multiple comparisons was not performed for the primary or supplementary analyses; exact p-values are reported for direct evaluation of statistical significance (33). All statistical analyses were performed using Stata version 16.0 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX).

Results

Group-Level Comparisons

No significant group differences were observed for demographic variables (Table 1). Within the TDC group, sex was correlated with FA in the right PP (rpb = −0.47, p = 0.042) and with the number of streamline points in the right CB (rpb = −0.51, p = 0.027); however, there were no correlations between sex and DTI metrics for any ROI in the TBI group. No relationship was found for sex with any cognitive variable in the TDC group; however, sex-related differences were apparent in the TBI group for performance on the CN (rpb = 0.46, p = 0.037), WR (rpb = 0.46, p = 0.038), and IN (rpb = 0.47, p = 0.030) conditions of the CWIT. Age at evaluation was related to WR performance within the TBI group (r = −0.44, p = 0.047); however, no other relationships were found for age at evaluation with any cognitive or DTI variables in either group.

Significant differences were found between the TBI and TDC groups for several cognitive variables (Table 3). The TBI group performed more poorly than the TDC group on WR, IN, and IS conditions of the CWIT, and a moderate effect size was found for poorer CN performance in the TBI group. Similarly, the TBI group scored lower on Trials 1–5 and SDFR of the CVLT, and a moderate effect size was found for poorer LDFR performance in the TBI group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Between-group differences in cognitive performance and diffusion metrics.

With the exception of increased MD in the left CB of the TBI group, relative to the TDC group, no significant differences in the number of streamline points, FA, or MD were found between groups for either ROI (Table 3). However, moderate to large effect sizes were found for decreased FA in the right PP, increased MD in the right CB, and increased MD in the right PP of the TBI group, relative to the TDC group.

Supplementary analyses revealed significantly higher RD in the left CB and a moderate effect size for higher RD in the right CB of those with TBI relative to those in the TDC group (Supplementary Table 2). No group differences were found in AD of either pathway.

Relation of DTI Parameters to Cognitive Performance and Age at Injury in TBI

Within the TBI group, increased FA of the left PP was related to better performance on the IN condition of the CWIT. No other significant relationships were seen between streamline points, FA, or MD of the CB or PP with performance on CWIT (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Partial correlations between performance on the Color-Word Interference Test (CWIT) and diffusion parameters.

Supplementary analysis of associations between CWIT performance and measures of directional diffusivity within the TBI group (Supplementary Table 3) revealed a significant correlation, with a moderate effect size, between increased RD in the left PP and poorer performance on the WR condition.

As for associations with performance on the CVLT within the TBI group (Table 5), an increased number of streamline points derived from the left PP was associated with better performance on Trials 1–5, SDFR, and LDFR, accounting for a large proportion of variance in performance on these measures. Similarly, DTI-derived FA of the left PP was increased in relation to better performance on Trials 1–5, SDFR, and LDFR of the CVLT, and decreased MD of the right and left PP was associated with better SDFR and LDFR performance; FA and MD accounted for a large proportion of variance in CVLT performance in each of these cases. No relation was found between MD of the left PP and performance on Trials 1–5 of the CVLT; however, decreased MD of the right PP was associated with better performance on this measure and accounted for a moderate proportion of its variance.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Partial correlations between performance on the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) and diffusion parameters.

No associations between CVLT performance and the number of streamline points of the CB were found (Table 5); however, increased FA of the left CB was related to better LDFR performance. Additionally, decreased MD of the bilateral CB was associated with better performance on CVLT SDFR and LDFR.

Comparisons in CWIT and CVLT performance between select children from the TBI group and demographically-matched children from the TDC group are shown along with tractographic renderings of the CB and PP in Figures 3, 4, respectively. Additionally, graphical representations of the significant correlations reported between DTI metrics and performance on the CWIT and CVLT can be found in the Supplementary Figures.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Executive functioning and CB tractography in typically-developing children and children with traumatic brain injury. Executive functioning is reflected by scaled scores (mean = 10 ± 3) on the color naming (CN), word reading (WR), inhibition (IN), and interference/switching (IS) trials of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Color-Word Interference Test (CWIT). CB, cingulum bundle; GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale score.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Verbal memory and PP tractography in typically-developing children and children with traumatic brain injury. Verbal memory performance is measured by T-scores (mean = 50 ± 10) on Trials 1–5 (T1–5) and z-scores (mean = 0.0 ± 1.0) on short-delay free recall (SDFR) and long-delay free recall (LDFR) trials of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT). PP, perforant pathway; GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale score.

Supplementary analyses of associations between measures of directional diffusivity and CVLT performance (Supplementary Table 4) revealed significant associations between increased AD of the right PP and poorer SDFR and LDFR performance within the TBI group. Significant associations were also present between increased RD of the left PP and poorer performance on Trials 1–5 of the CVLT, as well as between increased RD of both the PP and CB bilaterally and poorer performance on CVLT SDFR and LDFR. In each of these relationships, fluctuations in directional diffusivity accounted for a large proportion of the variance in CVLT performance.

Over half of the children in the TBI group exhibited hypertrophic appearance in the anterior region of the CB (Figure 3). Greater hypertrophy was reflected by an increased number of streamline points in the bilateral CB, and this was significantly associated with younger age at injury, accounting for a large proportion of variance (Table 6). Increased FA in the right CB was also associated with younger age at injury (Figure 5); however, MD of the CB was not associated with age at injury. Age at injury was not related to the number of streamline points, FA, or MD of the PP.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Partial correlations between diffusion metrics and age at injury.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Relation between white matter integrity of the CB and age at brain injury. In the top panel, younger age at injury is associated with increased fractional anisotropy (FA) of the cingulum bundle (CB), but not the perforant pathway (PP; not shown). Younger age at injury is also associated with an increased number of streamline points for the right CB (middle panel) and the left CB (bottom panel) but not the PP (not shown).

Supplementary analyses further revealed that younger age at injury was significantly associated with higher AD in the left CB (Supplementary Table 5), and a moderate effect size was present for the right CB. A moderate effect size was also present for the relationship between younger age at injury and increased RD in the right CB. No relationships were present between age at injury and AD or RD of the PP.

Discussion

We explored the effects of early TBI on later structural development and functionality of the CB and PP. We observed anticipated decreases in FA and increases in MD in both pathways, which had moderate-to-large effect sizes, along with poorer cognitive performance on measures of executive functioning and verbal memory in the participants who experienced a childhood TBI when compared to those in the TDC group. Our results also suggest that in those with early childhood TBI, cognitive performance and age at the time of injury are differentially related to changes in streamline density and tract integrity of the CB versus the PP.

These results are consistent with previous studies reporting impaired cognitive outcome due to injuries sustained to the CB and PP (15, 16, 31). Projections from the CB to the prefrontal cortex (also late developing), posterior cortex, and limbic regions surrounding the corpus callosum promote communication between these regions (37). The function of the CB is linked to cognitive control, working memory, and executive functioning, and these functions are compromised when the CB is damaged (15, 38); hence, our inclusion of the CWIT, which is a sensitive measure of components of executive functioning, including cognitive inhibition and response flexibility (23). The PP has been linked to verbal episodic memory (16), and previous studies have reported that performance on the CVLT, a sensitive measure of verbal learning and memory, is impaired by injuries sustained to the PP (3942); our results are consistent with these findings.

Prior DTI studies in moderate-to-severe TBI generally report overall decreases in FA and increases in MD, which are reflected by diminished tractographic renderings of WM tracts in the affected areas (15, 16). However, despite these anticipated group differences, careful visual inspection of our data revealed unexpected, qualitative differences in the tractography-derived pathways of children who sustained early injuries. Using quantitative diffusion tractography, which evaluates the number of distinct streamlines that pass through an ROI, we observed elaborated, hypertrophic pathways that were more prominent in children who experienced TBI at a younger age. These elaborations were particularly evident in the CB and, to a lesser extent, the PP; however, the relation of the DTI-derived parameters extracted from these tracts demonstrated a disparate relation to age at injury and performance on cognitive testing. An increased number of tract streamlines in the bilateral CB was strongly associated with younger age at injury, particularly in late-developing frontal regions, though the number of streamlines was not related to executive functioning, as measured by the CWIT (Figure 3). In contrast, the streamline density of the PP was not related to age at injury; rather, a greater number of streamline points, higher FA, and lower MD in the left PP was associated with better verbal memory performance on the CVLT. In the right PP, lower MD, without change in streamline density or FA, was also associated with improved verbal recall ability. The positive relationship observed between the number of PP streamlines and verbal memory may illustrate adaptive neuroplasticity that occurs following damage to an early-developing tract, which was relatively mature at the time of injury. In contrast, the absence of such a relationship in the CB may reflect the long-term, adverse effects of an injury sustained to this late-developing tract and its support of both executive functioning and memory.

There are several potential explanations for changes in WM integrity following pediatric TBI in children with differing ages at the time of injury. These findings may merely represent the heterogeneity associated with TBI, where injury severity influences the extent of axonal injury, with partial injury permitting the myelin reconstitution associated with recovery (10, 43). Furthermore, apparent axon regeneration has been reported in those with late recovery from a minimally conscious state (44). In support of this, our supplementary analysis of measures of directional diffusivity revealed that the higher FA and lower MD associated with verbal memory performance in the TBI group are likely driven by decreased RD, largely without change in AD, which suggests that some extent of remyelination of these pathways may have occurred. Remyelination by surviving oligodendrocytes has been shown to occur after trauma (45, 46), particularly to the developing brain, and it has been suggested that this process may have a neuroprotective effect against axonal damage and neurodegeneration (47).

Another possible mechanism underlying these observations in tractography is that potential dynamic changes in pediatric TBI relate to potential injury-induced proliferation of neural progenitor cells (NPCs). Increased expression of NPC markers has been described in human tissue surrounding focal lesions following injury (48), presumably as a restorative response. Two primary hippocampal pathways have been implicated in the generation, migration, and integration of new neurons into local circuitry (4951): the subgranular zone (SGZ), which neighbors aspects of the PP, and the subventricular zone (SVZ), which borders aspects of the ventricles, striatum, and CB (52, 53). Interestingly, it is these two regions that also reflect the altered tractography observed in our sample.

We found a striking relation between age at injury and the number of CB streamlines generated through tractography. Specifically, we observed that children injured as toddlers generally demonstrated the most elaborated structures, whereas this relation with streamline density of the CB was less apparent in children who were injured at an older age (Figure 3). Age-dependent increases in post-traumatic neurogenic response to injury have been found to be more robust in the immature brain as opposed to the adult or aged brain in experimental models (54), specifically within the SVZ (55). While this response has generally been presumed to underlie greater functional recovery following TBI (49), it has also been shown to result in altered cell migration patterns, which may contribute to long-term cognitive deficits and the phenomenon of growing into an injury after childhood TBI (55). However, the role that neurogenesis and cell migration have in repair following early TBI and whether these responses are beneficial have yet to be determined. Additionally, this response may also differ regionally; although the generation and migration of neurons remains most apparent in early childhood (49, 55), levels of neural proliferation may remain substantially higher in the SVZ (which borders the CB) as compared to the SGZ (which borders the PP) as the individual matures. This is consistent with our observation that the elaboration of the CB in children with early TBI appeared more age-dependent than the PP. The CB is one of the latest-developing tracts within the human brain (56), which may render it more prone to developmental alteration and myelination following TBI. On the contrary, the PP is an earlier-developing tract that projects from the entorhinal cortex to all fields of the hippocampal formation (57, 58).

Limitations and Future Directions

Despite the significant findings and moderate-to-large effect sizes, a larger sample is needed to replicate and confirm our observations, especially in view of the heterogeneous pathophysiology in moderate-to-severe TBI in this age range and the long-term effects on neurodevelopment. Although the exclusion of inflicted injury limited recruitment of children who sustained a TBI during infancy, this approach enabled us to focus on children who sustained a single TBI that was well-specified for age at injury without confounding by repetitive head trauma. We also acknowledge the wide range of the time-since-injury interval and age at follow-up in this preliminary study; however, one of the strengths of the study is the close matching of demographic characteristics between the TBI and TDC groups, which mitigated the number of confounding variables that could influence the results of our study. Another limitation of the study is the heterogeneity in mechanism of injury across our sample, as well as the location and severity of focal injury (Table 2). Although we examined structures considered to be important in verbal memory and executive functioning, as well as those in proximity to NPC migration, there may be additional sites of injury that contribute to cognitive deficit and structural change. Longitudinal DTI analysis would further elucidate the integrity of WM and its development throughout the injury-to-imaging time interval and inform the evolution of long-term structural and cognitive outcomes within the TBI group. We also appreciate that individual outcome after early TBI is affected by unique environmental factors, such as stress (2), as well as accessibility and quality of therapeutic intervention, genetic factors, and pre-existing psychological and behavioral disorders that were not assessed in this study. Additionally, single tensor-based methods, such as DTI, are neither able to account for complex architecture or crossing fibers within a voxel nor to determine the accurate origin and destination of fibers (5961). Furthermore, DTI-derived metrics may be affected by several factors related to image acquisition and preprocessing (60, 6266). More advanced diffusion-weighted imaging approaches have more recently emerged that demonstrate improvements over the shortcomings of DTI (32), such as high-angular resolution diffusion imaging (67, 68), diffusion spectrum imaging (69), and generalized q-sampling imaging (70); thus, future studies should take advantage of these techniques whenever possible. Finally, the present findings are limited by the fact that streamline density is largely dependent on the fiber tracking algorithm used, thus it is not a true reflection of the underlying fiber density (32); however, steps were taken to standardize the fiber tracking algorithm and control for any influence of data quality. For these reasons, we do not believe that the importance of the present findings is undermined. We strongly urge, however, that caution is taken when generalizing the present findings to other samples, particularly when other fiber tracking algorithms are used for analysis.

Conclusion

The aim of this study was to investigate neuroplasticity and its implications for WM recovery and cognitive outcome after early childhood TBI. As expected, we found evidence that pediatric TBI disrupts WM structure and function, though age at injury may play a significant role in the post-injury development of brain structure. DTI analyses did not fit the conventional tenet of static pathology following TBI, rather these analyses support the possibility of fundamentally altered development in some children who are injured at a very young age. The present findings also challenge the concept that post-trauma indices of brain structure directly relate to functional outcome in an expected pattern.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Baylor College of Medicine Institutional Review Board and the University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

EW, IH, EB, and SM conceptualized and designed the study. EW, BB, LE-C, MA, and MM assisted with data collection and recruitment procedures. MM also acted as the project and clinical data manager. JH was responsible for reviewing raw imaging data for incidental findings. ZC was responsible for the development and quality assurance of the neuroimaging protocol, and analysis of the neuroimaging data was performed by EW, IH, JF, BB, and LH. MA and LE-C were the site principal investigators. HSL and LN-H were the principal investigators for the entire study. EW, LN-H, and HSL performed project oversite and supervision. EW, IH, HML, JF, JM, SM, JH, LE-C, and MA drafted the manuscript. EB, LN-H, and HSL critically reviewed the manuscript. HML and EW finalized the manuscript. HML performed statistical analyses. HML and IH prepared the figures. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The study was funded by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) grant R21-NS065937 granted to LN-H and HSL.

Author Disclaimer

The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Health.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the participants of this study. They also would like to thank JoAnn Petrie, Ph.D. (University of Utah, Department of Neurology) and Elizabeth Hovenden (University of Utah, Department of Neurology) for their editorial assistance with the manuscript. Additionally, they would like to thank Tracy J. Abildskov (University of Utah, Department of Neurology) for his assistance with the preparation of the Figures.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2021.734055/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Dennis M, Spiegler BJ, Simic N, Sinopoli KJ, Wilkinson A, Yeates KO, et al. Functional plasticity in childhood brain disorders: when, what, how, and whom to assess. Neuropsychol Rev. (2014) 24:389–408. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9261-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Lindsey HM, Wilde EA, Caeyenberghs K, Dennis EL. Longitudinal neuroimaging in pediatric traumatic brain injury: current state and consideration of factors that influence recovery. Front Neurol. (2019) 10:1296. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Anderson V, Spencer-Smith M, Wood A. Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioural plasticity after early brain insult. Brain. (2011) 134(Pt 8):2197–221. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Johnson MH. Sensitive periods in functional brain development: problems and prospects. Dev Psychobiol. (2005) 46:287–92. doi: 10.1002/dev.20057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2005) 28:377–401. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Yu TS, Washington PM, Kernie SG. Injury-induced neurogenesis: mechanisms and relevance. Neuroscientist. (2016) 22:61–71. doi: 10.1177/1073858414563616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bigler ED, Maxwell WL. Neuropathology of mild traumatic brain injury: relationship to neuroimaging findings. Brain Imaging Behav. (2012) 6:108–36. doi: 10.1007/s11682-011-9145-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Palacios EM, Fernandez-Espejo D, Junque C, Sanchez-Carrion R, Roig T, Tormos JM, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging differences relate to memory deficits in diffuse traumatic brain injury. BMC Neurol. (2011) 11:24. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-11-24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hayes JP, Bigler ED, Verfaellie M. Traumatic brain injury as a disorder of brain connectivity. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2016) 22:120–37. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Dennis EL, Babikian T, Giza CC, Thompson PM, Asarnow RF. Neuroimaging of the injured pediatric brain: methods and new lessons. Neuroscientist. (2018) 24:652–70. doi: 10.1177/1073858418759489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Budde MD, Xie M, Cross AH, Song SK. Axial diffusivity is the primary correlate of axonal injury in the experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis spinal cord: a quantitative pixelwise analysis. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:2805–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Song SK, Sun SW, Ramsbottom MJ, Chang C, Russell J, Cross AH. Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage. (2002) 17:1429–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. O'Donnell LJ, Westin CF. An introduction to diffusion tensor image analysis. Neurosurg Clin N Am. (2011) 22:185–96. doi: 10.1016/j.nec.2010.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Dennis EL, Caeyenberghs K, Hoskinson KR, Merkley TL, Suskauer SJ, Asarnow RF, et al. White matter disruption in pediatric traumatic brain injury: results from enigma pediatric moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neurology (2021) 97:e298–e309. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wilde EA, Ramos MA, Yallampalli R, Bigler ED, McCauley SR, Chu Z, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of the cingulum bundle in children after traumatic brain injury. Dev Neuropsychol. (2010) 35:333–51. doi: 10.1080/87565641003696940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Christidi F, Bigler ED, McCauley SR, Schnelle KP, Merkley TL, Mors MB, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of the perforant pathway zone and its relation to memory function in patients with severe traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2011) 28:711–25. doi: 10.1089/neu.2010.1644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bigler ED. Anterior and middle cranial fossa in traumatic brain injury: relevant neuroanatomy and neuropathology in the study of neuropsychological outcome. Neuropsychology. (2007) 21:515–31. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.5.515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Anderson LB, Jaroh R, Smith H, Strong CH, Donders J. Criterion validity of the D-KEFS color-word and verbal fluency switching paradigms following traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2017) 39:890–9. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1277513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. DeJong J, Donders J. A confirmatory factor analysis of the California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition (CVLT-II) in a traumatic brain injury sample. Assessment. (2009) 16:328–36. doi: 10.1177/1073191109336989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Jacobs ML, Donders J. Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2007) 22:143–9. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test-Children's Version (CVLT-C). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (1994).

PubMed Abstract

22. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition (CVLT-II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2000).

Google Scholar

23. Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS): Examiner's Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2001). doi: 10.1037/t15082-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment and prognosis of coma after head injury. Acta Neurochir. (1976) 34:45–55. doi: 10.1007/BF01405862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Yeates KO, Taylor HG, Drotar D, Wade SL, Klein S, Stancin T, et al. Preinjury family environment as a determinant of recovery from traumatic brain injuries in school-age children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (1997) 3:617–30. doi: 10.1017/S1355617797006176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Netsch T, van Muiswinkel A. Quantitative evaluation of image-based distortion correction in diffusion tensor imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (2004) 23:789–98. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2004.827479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hoogenraad F. Multi-Center Evaluation of In-vivo Fiber Tracking. Best: Philips Medical Systems (2002).

28. Mori S, Crain BJ, Chacko VP, van Zifl PC. Three-dimensional tracking of axonal projections in the brain by magnetic resonance imaging. Ann Neurol. (1999) 45:265–9. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199902)45:2>265::AID-ANA21>3.0.CO;2-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bigler ED, McCauley SR, Wu TC, Yallampalli R, Shah S, MacLeod M, et al. The temporal stem in traumatic brain injury: preliminary findings. Brain Imaging Behav. (2010) 4:270–82. doi: 10.1007/s11682-010-9105-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex. (2008) 44:1105–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wu TC, Wilde EA, Bigler ED, Yallampalli R, McCauley SR, Troyanskaya M, et al. Evaluating the relationship between memory functioning and cingulum bundles in acute mild traumatic brain injury using diffusion tensor imaging. J Neurotrauma. (2010) 27:303–7. doi: 10.1089/neu.2009.1110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Jones DK, Knosche TR, Turner R. White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: the do's and don'ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage. (2013) 73:239–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 3 ed. Sherman EMS, editor. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2006).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Welch BL. The generalization ofstudent's' problem when several different population variances are involved. Biometrika. (1947) 34:28–35. doi: 10.1093/biomet/34.1-2.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hedges LV. Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat. (1981) 6:107–28. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (1988).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

37. Rudrauf D, Mehta S, Grabowski TJ. Disconnection's renaissance takes shape: formal incorporation in group-level lesion studies. Cortex. (2008) 44:1084–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bunge SA, Ochsner KN, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. Brain. (2001) 124:2074–86. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.10.2074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Donders J, Hoffman NM. Gender differences in learning and memory after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. (2002) 16:491–9. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lindsey HM, Lalani SJ, Mietchen J, Gale SD, Wilde EA, Faber J, et al. Acute pediatric traumatic brain injury severity predicts long-term verbal memory performance through suppression by white matter integrity on diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Imaging Behav. (2019) 14:1626–37. doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00093-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Roman MJ, Delis DC, Willerman L, Magulac M, Demadura TL, de la Pena JL, et al. Impact of pediatric traumatic brain injury on components of verbal memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1998) 20:245–58. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.2.245.1168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Yeates KO, Blumenstein E, Patterson CM, Delis DC. Verbal learning and memory following pediatric closed-head injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (1995) 1:78–87. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700000138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Dennis EL, Babikian T, Giza CC, Thompson PM, Asarnow RF. Diffusion MRI in pediatric brain injury. Childs Nerv Syst. (2017) 33:1683–92. doi: 10.1007/s00381-017-3522-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Voss HU, Ulug AM, Dyke JP, Watts R, Kobylarz EJ, McCandliss BD, et al. Possible axonal regrowth in late recovery from the minimally conscious state. J Clin Invest. (2006) 116:2005–11. doi: 10.1172/JCI27021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kutzelnigg A, Lassmann H. Pathology of multiple sclerosis and related inflammatory demyelinating diseases. Handb Clin Neurol. (2014) 122:15–58. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52001-2.00002-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Plemel JR, Liu WQ, Yong VW. Remyelination therapies: a new direction and challenge in multiple sclerosis. Nat Rev Drug Discov. (2017) 16:617–34. doi: 10.1038/nrd.2017.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Villoslada P, Martinez-Lapiscina EH. Remyelination: a good neuroprotective strategy for preventing axonal degeneration? Brain. (2019) 142:233–6. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Zheng W, ZhuGe Q, Zhong M, Chen G, Shao B, Wang H, et al. Neurogenesis in adult human brain after traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2013) 30:1872–80. doi: 10.1089/neu.2010.1579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Curtis MA, Low VF, Faull RL. Neurogenesis and progenitor cells in the adult human brain: a comparison between hippocampal and subventricular progenitor proliferation. Dev Neurobiol. (2012) 72:990–1005. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Emery DL, Fulp CT, Saatman KE, Schutz C, Neugebauer E, McIntosh TK. Newly born granule cells in the dentate gyrus rapidly extend axons into the hippocampal CA3 region following experimental brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2005) 22:978–88. doi: 10.1089/neu.2005.22.978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sun D, McGinn MJ, Zhou Z, Harvey HB, Bullock MR, Colello RJ. Anatomical integration of newly generated dentate granule neurons following traumatic brain injury in adult rats and its association to cognitive recovery. Exp Neurol. (2007) 204:264–72. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2006.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Clausi MG, Kumari E, Levison SW. Unmasking the responses of the stem cells and progenitors in the subventricular zone after neonatal and pediatric brain injuries. Neural Regen Res. (2016) 11:45–8. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.175041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Niimi Y, Levison SW. Pediatric brain repair from endogenous neural stem cells of the subventricular zone. Pediatr Res. (2018) 83:385–96. doi: 10.1038/pr.2017.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Sun D, Colello RJ, Daugherty WP, Kwon TH, McGinn MJ, Harvey HB, et al. Cell proliferation and neuronal differentiation in the dentate gyrus in juvenile and adult rats following traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2005) 22:95–105. doi: 10.1089/neu.2005.22.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Costine BA, Missios S, Taylor SR, McGuone D, Smith CM, Dodge CP, et al. The subventricular zone in the immature piglet brain: anatomy and exodus of neuroblasts into white matter after traumatic brain injury. Dev Neurosci. (2015) 37:115–30. doi: 10.1159/000369091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Lebel C, Gee M, Camicioli R, Wieler M, Martin W, Beaulieu C. Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan. Neuroimage. (2012) 60:340–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hevner RF, Kinney HC. Reciprocal entorhinal-hippocampal connections established by human fetal midgestation. J Comp Neurol. (1996) 372:384–94. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960826)372:3<384::AID-CNE4>3.0.CO;2-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Witter M. Hippocampus. In: Watson C, Paxinos G, Puelles L, editors. The Mouse Nervous System. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (2012). p. 112–39. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-369497-3.10005-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Abhinav K, Yeh FC, Pathak S, Suski V, Lacomis D, Friedlander RM, et al. Advanced diffusion MRI fiber tracking in neurosurgical and neurodegenerative disorders and neuroanatomical studies: a review. Biochim Biophys Acta. (2014) 1842:2286–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2014.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Alexander AL, Hasan KM, Lazar M, Tsuruda JS, Parker DL. Analysis of partial volume effects in diffusion-tensor MRI. Magn Reson Med. (2001) 45:770–80. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Tuch DS, Reese TG, Wiegell MR, Wedeen VJ. Diffusion MRI of complex neural architecture. Neuron. (2003) 40:885–95. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00758-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Hui ES, Cheung MM, Chan KC, Wu EX. B-value dependence of DTI quantitation and sensitivity in detecting neural tissue changes. Neuroimage. (2010) 49:2366–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Huisman TA, Loenneker T, Barta G, Bellemann ME, Hennig J, Fischer JE, et al. Quantitative diffusion tensor MR imaging of the brain: field strength related variance of apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC) and fractional anisotropy (FA) scalars. Eur Radiol. (2006) 16:1651–8. doi: 10.1007/s00330-006-0175-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Ni H, Kavcic V, Zhu T, Ekholm S, Zhong J. Effects of number of diffusion gradient directions on derived diffusion tensor imaging indices in human brain. Am J Neuroradiol. (2006) 27:1776–81.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

65. Oouchi H, Yamada K, Sakai K, Kizu O, Kubota T, Ito H, et al. Diffusion anisotropy measurement of brain white matter is affected by voxel size: underestimation occurs in areas with crossing fibers. Am J Neuroradiol. (2007) 28:1102–6. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A0488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Yendiki A, Koldewyn K, Kakunoori S, Kanwisher N, Fischl B. Spurious group differences due to head motion in a diffusion MRI study. Neuroimage. (2014) 88:79–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Jansons KM, Alexander DC. Persistent Angular Structure: new insights from diffusion MRI data. Dummy version. Inf Process Med Imaging. (2003) 18:672–83. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-45087-0_56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Tuch DS, Reese TG, Wiegell MR, Makris N, Belliveau JW, Wedeen VJ. High angular resolution diffusion imaging reveals intravoxel white matter fiber heterogeneity. Magn Reson Med. (2002) 48:577–82. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wedeen VJ, Hagmann P, Tseng WY, Reese TG, Weisskoff RM. Mapping complex tissue architecture with diffusion spectrum magnetic resonance imaging. Magn Reson Med. (2005) 54:1377–86. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Yeh FC, Wedeen VJ, Tseng WY. Generalized q-sampling imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (2010) 29:1626–35. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2010.2045126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pediatric traumatic brain injury, diffusion tensor imaging, tractography, brain development, neuroplasticity, structural neuroimaging

Citation: Wilde EA, Hyseni I, Lindsey HM, Faber J, McHenry JM, Bigler ED, Biekman BD, Hollowell LL, McCauley SR, Hunter JV, Ewing-Cobbs L, Aitken ME, MacLeod M, Chu ZD, Noble-Haeusslein LJ and Levin HS (2021) A Preliminary DTI Tractography Study of Developmental Neuroplasticity 5–15 Years After Early Childhood Traumatic Brain Injury. Front. Neurol. 12:734055. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.734055

Received: 30 June 2021; Accepted: 06 December 2021;
Published: 23 December 2021.

Edited by:

Mårten Risling, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Sarah C. Hellewell, Curtin University, Australia
Bridgette D. Semple, Monash University, Australia

Copyright © 2021 Wilde, Hyseni, Lindsey, Faber, McHenry, Bigler, Biekman, Hollowell, McCauley, Hunter, Ewing-Cobbs, Aitken, MacLeod, Chu, Noble-Haeusslein and Levin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elisabeth A. Wilde, elisabeth.wilde@hsc.utah.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.