Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26 February 2021
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 15 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.643106

Is Reconsolidation a General Property of Memory?

  • 1Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, College of Engineering, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Daejeon, South Korea
  • 2Program of Brain and Cognitive Engineering, College of Engineering, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Daejeon, South Korea

Memory reconsolidation holds great hope for memory modification approaches and clinical treatments of mental disorders associated with maladaptive memories. However, it remains controversial as to whether reconsolidation is a general property of all types of memory. Especially, discrepancies have been reported in research focusing on whether declarative memory undergoes reconsolidation, and whether old memories can be reorganized after retrieval. Here, we discuss how these inconsistent results can be reconciled and what information we need to uncover for the general use of reconsolidation.

Reconsolidation theory has attracted the attention of many researchers because it suggests the possibility that memory can be modified depending on the experience or manipulation given after retrieval within a limited labile time window (Nader et al., 2000; Tronson and Taylor, 2007; Lee et al., 2008; Lee, 2009; Nader and Hardt, 2009; Schiller and Phelps, 2011; Elsey et al., 2018). Especially this theory provides great hope for generating potential therapeutic approaches for treating mental disorders caused by traumatic memories such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Schwabe et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2017).

Since the idea of reconsolidation was initially suggested (Misanin et al., 1968; Schneider and Sherman, 1968), work by Nader et al. (2000) has accelerated reconsolidation research in the field of memory. They showed the impairment of long-term fear memory in rats through an injection of a protein synthesis inhibitor into the amygdala immediately after memory retrieval (Nader et al., 2000). Based on these findings in rodent models, whether such reconsolidation also occurs in human subjects were tested and demonstrated (Schiller and Phelps, 2011; Elsey et al., 2018). Because injection of protein synthesis inhibitors cannot be easily applied to humans, behavioral interference or pharmacological manipulation using propranolol instead of a protein synthesis inhibitor injection was used. Walker et al. (2003) showed that motor memory acquired through training involving a finger-tapping sequence can be disrupted by learning a new finger-tapping sequence if participants retrieved the first learned sequence prior to learning the second sequence. Reconsolidation of Pavlovian fear memory as found in rodents has also been observed in humans. Interfering with reconsolidation by extinction training in the labile window after retrieval impaired consolidated fear memory (Schiller et al., 2010; Oyarzún et al., 2012). Moreover, the reconsolidation interference successfully reduced expression of fear among phobia patients by applying propranolol or extinction training after reactivation (Soeter and Kindt, 2015; Björkstrand et al., 2016), and the effects were sustained over 6 months (Björkstrand et al., 2017). Even symptoms of PTSD patients could be improved by interfering with reconsolidation via the application of propranolol after retrieval (Brunet and Monson, 2014; Kindt and van Emmerik, 2016). Furthermore, human reconsolidation has been reported not only in procedural or aversive memories but also in declarative memories (Hupbach et al., 2007; Chan and LaPaglia, 2013; Sinclair and Barense, 2018).

However, it remains controversial as to whether reconsolidation can be generally applied to every type of memory because there are also reports showing no effect of interfering with reconsolidation on memory performance (Golkar et al., 2012; Hardwicke et al., 2016). This debate appears to be especially more pronounced in the two conditions; declarative memory and old memory. Here, we focus on controversial results associated with the reconsolidation of declarative memory and old memory in human subjects and suggest that the controversial findings may be mainly related to the complexity and range of the cortical circuits involved in a particular memory.

Reconsolidation of Human Declarative Memory

To apply reconsolidation theory to the area of human memory modification, including its application as a clinical treatment for PTSD, it is necessary for both declarative and non-declarative components of traumatic memory to be susceptible to reconsolidation. However, while most reconsolidation studies show consistent results supporting the contention that implicit fear memory undergoes post-retrieval reconsolidation, it remains controversial as to whether reconsolidation can be applied to declarative memory types, including declarative fear memory (Schiller and Phelps, 2011; Soeter and Kindt, 2015). Soeter and Kindt (2015) showed that interference with the systematic administration of propranolol after retrieval induced the erasure of startle fear but did not affect unconditioned stimulus (US) expectancy ratings. Björkstrand et al. (2016) demonstrated that the disruption of reconsolidation could facilitate an approach for individuals with a lifelong fear of spiders to new spider stimuli, and Xue et al. (2012) showed that extinction training 10 min after retrieval could effectively attenuate cue-induced heroin cravings, but these studies did not examine declarative memory components separately. Moreover, there was little effect of interference through a reconsolidation process for disrupting the restabilization of traumatic naturalistic memories in humans (Shiban et al., 2015; Maples-Keller et al., 2017; Telch et al., 2017).

Several studies have investigated the reconsolidation of neutral declarative memories based on tasks using lists of words or objects. In these tasks, participants were asked to memorize a word or object list and to learn a second list immediately after the retrieval of the first list (Hupbach et al., 2007, 2009; Dongaonkar et al., 2013; Hupbach, 2015). In subsequent retrieval tests, the participants showed impairment of the original list memory, while the control group who underwent the same procedure except for retrieval just before the second learning showed maintenance of the original list (Hupbach et al., 2007; Dongaonkar et al., 2013; Hupbach, 2015). Additionally, many studies have reported that relearning after the reactivation of the original memory can strengthen memory in both young and old adults, suggesting a memory enhancement through reconsolidation (Forcato et al., 2011, 2013; Tassone et al., 2020). These results suggest that reconsolidation commonly occurs in human declarative memories. However, there are also compelling research results that raise the question of whether the reconsolidation process can be generally applied to declarative memory. In similar tasks based on list learning, the second learning after memory retrieval had no effect on the memory of the original list (Hardwicke et al., 2016; Klingmüller et al., 2017).

These controversial results have also been reported for more naturalistic event memories. Chan and LaPaglia (2013) demonstrated that misinformation following the reactivation of an original memory of a movie clip can interfere with reconsolidation, leading to the impairment of the original episodic memory. Moreover, James et al. (2015) used computer gameplay as a reconsolidation interference tool, showing that traumatic memories were successfully reduced. Sinclair and Barense (2018) also reported that simply watching mismatching movie clips after the reactivation of the original episodic memory can disturb the reconsolidation process, influencing subsequent episodic memory performance outcomes. Additionally, reminders to reactive film memories recovered and prevented a loss of detailed episodic memory (Sekeres et al., 2016), suggesting the strengthening of memory through a reconsolidation process. Moreover, in work by St. Jacques and Schacter (2013) based on a museum tour paradigm, retrieval was shown to enable memories to be both selectively enhanced and distorted via updating. However, there are also inconsistent findings on episodic memory (Rindal et al., 2016; Elsey et al., 2018). Specifically, Rindal et al. (2016) failed to replicate the findings of Chan and LaPaglia (2013). Unlike Chan and LaPaglia (2013), they measured hit and false alarm rates separately. As a result, they found that there was no significant increase in false alarm rate when misinformation was given with retrieval, suggesting no evidence of memory impairment by the integration of misinformation after retrieval. Schwabe and Wolf (2009) showed that while new episodic learning interfered with the reconsolidation of neutral autobiographical memories, the same paradigm was not effective for emotional memories.

With regard to why a substantial number of studies observed a failure of memory reconsolidation, many studies indicate boundary conditions on the induction of reconsolidation (Nader and Einarsson, 2010; Walker and Stickgold, 2016; Elsey and Kindt, 2017; Lee et al., 2017; Monfils and Holmes, 2018). They suggest that stronger memories, usually resulting from repetitive training or exposure to highly attentional conditions such as stressful situations, or older memories are relatively less susceptible to reconsolidation effects (Milekic and Alberini, 2002; Wang et al., 2009; Bustos et al., 2010; Elsey and Kindt, 2017). However, the proposed boundary conditions do not seem to be absolute. Strong fear memories, which are resistant to extinction, were still susceptible to the disruptive effects of propranolol given with memory retrieval (Soeter and Kindt, 2012). Moreover, Fernández et al. (2016a) also showed that strong declarative memory acquired under stress or old memory could still be destabilized and impaired based on a reconsolidation procedure, suggesting that the boundary conditions of reconsolidation are not fixed but depend on memory features and the reminder characteristics.

There is also speculation suggesting that memory reconsolidation depends on the arousal level during the memory processes. These studies claim that the effect of the reconsolidation blockade is limited to specific experimental conditions with a high arousal level, such as in fear-conditioning memory (Lewis, 1976; Squire et al., 1976; Fernández et al., 2016b). However, given the findings related to reconsolidation in non-emotional semantic and episodic memories (Hupbach et al., 2007; Chan and LaPaglia, 2013), a high arousal level may not be indispensable for triggering the reconsolidation process. Other studies also suggest that prediction errors or novel information during memory retrieval would be critical for triggering reconsolidation. It was found that behavioral interference or a protein synthesis block after memory retrieval did not affect the original memory when no new information was given (Sinclair and Barense, 2019). However, it appears that prediction error is necessary for the reconsolidation process but is not sufficient to induce reconsolidation (Sevenster et al., 2014). Therefore, it is necessary to gain a better understanding of the more fundamental neural processes that determine the boundary conditions.

Effect of Age of Memory

Another controversial issue related to the general application of reconsolidation is whether reconsolidation can be triggered regardless of how old the memory is. Despite numerous studies suggest the possibility that reconsolidation can be used to modify long-term memories, it has also been reported that older memory is less susceptible to reconsolidation. Thus, as mentioned above, the age of memory has been considered as one of the major boundary conditions of the induction of reconsolidation. Prior animal studies showed that while pharmacological interference after retrieval impaired recent memory (1 day or 1 week), remote memory (more than 2 weeks or 1 month) was less influenced (Milekic and Alberini, 2002; Eisenberg and Dudai, 2004; Suzuki et al., 2004; Frankland et al., 2006). Some studies also suggest that this difference may reflect the gradual decrease of hippocampal involvement with increasing memory age (Dudai, 2004; McKenzie and Eichenbaum, 2011; Squire et al., 2015). However, there is also evidence indicating that remote memories undergo reconsolidation after retrieval, suggesting ACC as neural substrates underlying the reconsolidation of remote memory (Frankland et al., 2004; Einarsson and Nader, 2012). Reengagement of the hippocampus in remote memory reconsolidation was also considered (Debiec et al., 2002; Myers and Davis, 2002).

The dependency of memory reconsolidation on the age of the memories has also been examined in human research. Wichert et al. (2011) compared memory impairment of 1-, 7-, and 28-day-old memories after reactivation followed by behavioral interference, finding that 1- and 7-day-old memories but not 28-day-old memories were impaired by post-reactivation interference. Steinfurth et al. (2014) suggested no effect of memory age, but they examined only 1 and 7-day-old memories based on the Pavlovian fear memory paradigm. However, it is hasty to conclude that older memories are less susceptible to reconsolidation because there is also evidence supporting the modification of human remote memories (Cohen and Squire, 1981).

Involvement of Cortical Circuits in Reconsolidation

To utilize reconsolidation as a tool to modify human memory, including as a clinical treatment, reconsolidation must be a general property of memories. However, as reviewed above, controversy remains in particular with regard to whether declarative memory undergoes reconsolidation and whether even old memories can be reorganized after retrieval. We suggest the possibility that these two issues are ultimately related to how different and complex cortical circuits are involved in a given memory.

While implicit fear memory induced by Pavlovian conditioning involves relatively simple neural circuits centered at the amygdala, declarative memories appear to involve broadly distributed neural circuits across different brain regions. The medial temporal lobe (MTL) regions, including the hippocampus, are well-known as a key system for the encoding, consolidation and retrieval of declarative memories (Gluck and Myers, 1997; Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998; Teyler and Rudy, 2007; Moscovitch et al., 2016). Not only the MTL regions but also diverse cortical regions are involved in declarative memory processing. Prior studies suggest that specific types of sensory information are stored in the sensory cortical regions that are specialized for the processing of those types of information (Gandhi, 2001; Hofstetter et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2019). The association cortex is also involved in linking identical or different modality information in declarative memory processes (Svoboda et al., 2006; Cabeza and St Jacques, 2007; Rugg and Vilberg, 2013). Furthermore, the frontal cortical regions are considered to be engaged in the processing of higher-level conceptual information and executive control of memory processes (Tomita et al., 1999; Badre and Wagner, 2002; Preston and Eichenbaum, 2013). Specifically, Sekeres et al., showed that memory retrieval activates the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) followed by the hippocampus (Sekeres et al., 2021).

Although episodic and semantic memories are inextricably intertwined and show considerable overlap in their neural substrates, they nonetheless retain a measure of distinctiveness (Renoult et al., 2019). In particular, while the detailed contextual information of episodic memory is considered to involve the hippocampus (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997; Eldridge et al., 2000; Eichenbaum et al., 2007), gist information or semantic memory is thought mainly to depend on the cortical regions (Patterson et al., 2007; Binder and Desai, 2011; Moscovitch et al., 2016). Thus, even within declarative memory, the extent to which cortical circuits are involved may depend on the contents of the memory. The time-dependent loss of the peripheral details of episodic memories compared to the lower levels of the loss of the gist of events may reflect this different degree of involvement of the cortical and hippocampal circuits (Sekeres et al., 2016).

The involvement of diverse and complex cortical circuits may be more prominent in older memories. Memory models suggest that locally consolidated memory traces undergo system-level consolidation, which involves the reorganization of memory traces across different neural circuits and regions (Dudai, 2004; Barry and Maguire, 2019). This system-level consolidation process occurs over weeks, months, or even years after new information is learned, while synaptic consolidation is thought to occur within hours or days (Dudai, 2004). According to standard consolidation theory, declarative memory is initially encoded in a hippocampal-cortical trace but, over time, is stored in progressively strengthened cortico-cortical connections while the hippocampal trace fades (Squire et al., 2015). Multiple trace theory or recent scene construction theory also suggests the progressive development of a cortico-cortical trace over time but argues a perpetual role of the hippocampus in the retrieval of memory representations (Nadel et al., 2007; Moscovitch et al., 2016; Barry and Maguire, 2019). Although these views on system-level consolidation suggest different roles of the hippocampus in memory consolidation, they all suggest that stabilization of a memory progressively engages more cortico-cortical connections over time. Therefore, more cortical circuits may be involved in older memories, even for simple types of memory.

The fact that memory involves more diverse and broadly distributed cortical circuits indicates that it is more difficult to modify the memory via a reconsolidation process in two aspects. First, due to the diversity and complexity of neural circuits, there is a possibility that interference signals after memory retrieval may not sufficiently reach the core circuits underlying the target memory trace. If only partial circuits are affected, it is possible that other unaffected circuits compensate for the interfering effect. Of course, whether interfering partial circuits are effective may depend on how much the core circuits are affected. Therefore, it will be important to clarify how we can effectively interfere with the core circuits underlying the target memory in future reconsolidation research. The experiments with non-invasive brain stimulation methods such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) or direct current stimulation (tDCS) can contribute to uncover the core circuits in humans. Recent brain stimulation studies have shown that stimulating or disturbing the neural activity of a specific region after memory retrieval can modify the target episodic memory (Table 1). For example, anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on the lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) after memory reactivation enhanced episodic memory recognition in healthy young and old adults (Javadi and Cheng, 2013; Sandrini et al., 2014) as well as old adults with subjective memory complaints (SMC) (Manenti et al., 2017) or amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) (Manenti et al., 2020). Moreover, repeated TMS to the medial PFC of PTSD patients after brief exposure to a traumatic event with an imagery procedure provided a significant therapeutic effect in these patients (Isserles et al., 2013). On the other hand, over the left posterior parietal cortex, no effect of tDCS during the reconsolidation of word memory was reported (Crossman et al., 2019). Based on these results, we can consider the prefrontal cortex as a candidate region that includes the core circuits of declarative memory processes.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Recent human reconsolidation studies of declarative memory using brain stimulation as an intervention tool.

The second aspect of difficulty when attempting to modify memory via a reconsolidation process can arise from the property of cortical synapses. Because cortical synapses are known to have relatively slow plasticity compared to the property of the hippocampal synapses (Nakazawa et al., 2003; Dudai, 2004; Horner and Doeller, 2017), reconsolidation of a memory trace that involves more cortical circuits may need stronger and longer-lasting interfering signals after retrieval.

Thus, the controversial results on the reconsolidation of declarative memory or old memory may be due to the different degrees of the involvement of the cortical circuits and the different intensity levels of the interfering signals affecting them. Given that there are reports showing that reconsolidation can be triggered by interfering with or facilitating the activity of particular cortical circuits (Javadi and Cheng, 2013; Sandrini et al., 2013; Censor et al., 2014; Manenti et al., 2017; Borgomaneri et al., 2020) and that the same synapse that undergoes simple memory consolidation can be also disrupted and reconstructed after retrieval (Lee et al., 2012), it is still hopeful that reconsolidation is a general property of memory. If interfering signals with sufficient intensity levels and ranges are given, even declarative memories or old memories may be reconsolidated or modified. Further studies need to be done to elucidate such conditions for the induction of reconsolidation of consolidated memories. In line with that, it will be important to uncover neural circuits that are core to any type of memory trace and to reveal how the hippocampal circuits, which show the property of fast plasticity, interact with cortical circuits. These studies may critically contribute to the formulation and general application of reconsolidation in the development of effective interventions as part of a treatment regime for mental disorders related to maladaptive memories.

Author Contributions

GK, MK, WK, and S-HL wrote the manuscript. GK and S-HL conceptualized the manuscript. S-HL supervised and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Brain Research Program (NRF-2017M3C7A1031333), the Basic Science Research Program (NRF-2020R1A2C2007770), and the Neurological Disorder Research Program (NRF-2020M3E5D9079913) through the National Research Foundation (NRF) of Korea.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank to members of the Memory and Cognition Laboratory, KAIST for discussion.

References

Badre, D., and Wagner, A. D. (2002). Semantic retrieval, mnemonic control, and prefrontal cortex. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 1, 206–218. doi: 10.1177/1534582302001003002.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, D. N., and Maguire, E. A. (2019). Consolidating the case for transient hippocampal memory traces. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 635–636. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.008.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., and Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 527–536. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkstrand, J., Agren, T., Åhs, F., Frick, A., Larsson, E. M., Hjorth, O., et al. (2016). Disrupting reconsolidation attenuates long-term fear memory in the human amygdala and facilitates approach behavior. Curr. Biol. 26, 2690–2695. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.022.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkstrand, J., Agren, T., Åhs, F., Frick, A., Larsson, E. M., Hjorth, O., et al. (2017). Think twice, it’s all right: Long lasting effects of disrupted reconsolidation on brain and behavior in human long-term fear. Behav. Brain Res. 324, 125–129. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.02.016.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgomaneri, S., Battaglia, S., Garofalo, S., Tortora, F., Avenanti, A., and di Pellegrino, G. (2020). State-dependent TMS over prefrontal cortex disrupts fear-memory reconsolidation and prevents the return of fear. Curr. Biol. 30, 3672-3679. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.091.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunet, A., and Monson, E. (2014). Suicide risk among active and retired canadian soldiers: the role of posttraumatic stress disorder. Can. J. Psychiatry 59, 457–459. doi: 10.1177/070674371405900901.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bustos, S. G., Giachero, M., Maldonado, H., and Molina, V. A. (2010). Previous stress attenuates the susceptibility to midazolam’s disruptive effect on fear memory reconsolidation: influence of pre-reactivation D-cycloserine administration. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 1097–1108. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.215.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., and St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 219–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.005.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Censor, N., Dayan, E., and Cohen, L. G. (2014). Cortico-subcortical neuronal circuitry associated with reconsolidation of human procedural memories. Cortex 58, 281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.013.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. C. K., and LaPaglia, J. A. (2013). Impairing existing declarative memory in humans by disrupting reconsolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 9309–9313. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1218472110.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, N. J., and Squire, L. R. (1981). Retrograde amnesia and remote memory impairment. Neuropsychologia 19, 337–356. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90064-6.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossman, M., Bartl, G., Soerum, R., and Sandrini, M. (2019). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation over the posterior parietal cortex on episodic memory reconsolidation. Cortex 121, 78–88. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.08.009.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debiec, J., LeDoux, J. E., and Nader, K. (2002). Cellular and systems reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron 36, 527–538. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01001-2.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dongaonkar, B., Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., and Nadel, L. (2013). Effects of psychosocial stress on episodic memory updating. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 226, 769–779. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-2998-8.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y. (2004). The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 51–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einarsson, E. Ö., and Nader, K. (2012). Involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation of recent and remote contextual fear memory. Learn. Mem. 19, 449–452. doi: 10.1101/lm.027227.112.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, M., and Dudai, Y. (2004). Reconsolidation of fresh, remote, and extinguished fear memory in medaka: Old fears don’t die. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 3397–3403. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03818.x.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Engel, S. A. (2000). Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1149–1152.

Google Scholar

Elsey, J. W. B., and Kindt, M. (2017). Breaking boundaries: optimizing reconsolidation-based interventions for strong and old memories. Learn. Mem. 24, 472–479. doi: 10.1101/lm.044156.116.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsey, J. W. B., Van Ast, V. A., and Kindt, M. (2018). Human memory reconsolidation: a guiding framework and critical review of the evidence. Psychol. Bull. 144, 797–848. doi: 10.1037/bul0000152.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, R. S., Bavassi, L., Forcato, C., and Pedreira, M. E. (2016a). The dynamic nature of the reconsolidation process and its boundary conditions: evidence based on human tests. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 130, 202–212. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.03.001.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, R. S., Bavassi, L., Kaczer, L., Forcato, C., and Pedreira, M. E. (2016b). Interference conditions of the reconsolidation process in humans: the role of valence and different memory systems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:641. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00641.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forcato, C., Fernandez, R. S., and Pedreira, M. E. (2013). The role and dynamic of strengthening in the reconsolidation process in a human declarative memory: what decides the fate of recent and older memories? PLoS One 8:e61688. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061688.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forcato, C., Rodríguez, M. L. C., and Pedreira, M. E. (2011). Repeated labilization-reconsolidation processes strengthen declarative memory in humans. PLoS One 6:e23305. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023305.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., Bontempi, B., Talton, L. E., Kaczmarek, L., and Silva, A. J. (2004). The involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in remote contextual fear memory. Science 304, 881–883. doi: 10.1126/science.1094804.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., Ding, H. K., Takahashi, E., Suzuki, A., Kida, S., and Silva, A. J. (2006). Stability of recent and remote contextual fear memory. Learn. Mem. 13, 451–457. doi: 10.1101/lm.183406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandhi, S. P. (2001). Memory retrieval: reactivating sensory cortex. Curr. Biol. 11, R32-R34. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00040-3.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluck, M. A., and Myers, C. E. (1997). Psychobiological models of hippocampal function in learning and memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 481–514. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.481.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golkar, A., Bellander, M., Olsson, A., and Öhman, A. (2012). Are fear memories erasable? -reconsolidation of learned fear with fear relevant and fearirrelevant stimuli. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 6:80. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00080.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardwicke, T. E., Taqi, M., and Shanks, D. R. (2016). Postretrieval new learning does not reliably induce human memory updating via reconsolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 5206–5211. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601440113.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstetter, C., Achaibou, A., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Reactivation of visual cortex during memory retrieval: content specificity and emotional modulation. Neuroimage 60, 1734–1745. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.110.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horner, A. J., and Doeller, C. F. (2017). Plasticity of hippocampal memories in humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 43, 102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.02.004.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hupbach, A. (2015). Retrieval practice does not safeguard memories from interference-based forgetting. Learn. Motiv. 49, 23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2015.01.004.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., and Nadel, L. (2007). Reconsolidation of episodic memories: a subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learn. Mem. 14, 47–53. doi: 10.1101/lm.365707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., and Nadel, L. (2009). Episodic memory reconsolidation: updating or source confusion? Memory 17, 502–510. doi: 10.1080/09658210902882399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isserles, M., Shalev, A. Y., Roth, Y., Peri, T., Kutz, I., Zlotnick, E., et al. (2013). Effectiveness of deep transcranial magnetic stimulation combined with a brief exposure procedure in post-traumatic stress disorder-a pilot study. Brain Stimul. 6, 377–383. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, E. L., Bonsall, M. B., Hoppitt, L., Tunbridge, E. M., Geddes, J. R., Milton, A. L., et al. (2015). Computer game play reduces intrusive memories of experimental trauma via reconsolidation-update mechanisms. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1201–1215. doi: 10.1177/0956797615583071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Javadi, A. H., and Cheng, P. (2013). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) enhances reconsolidation of long-term memory. Brain Stimul. 6, 668–674. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kindt, M., and van Emmerik, A. (2016). New avenues for treating emotional memory disorders: towards a reconsolidation intervention for posttraumatic stress disorder. Ther. Adv. Psychopharmacol. 6, 283–295. doi: 10.1177/2045125316644541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klingmüller, A., Caplan, J. B., and Sommer, T. (2017). Intrusions in episodic memory: reconsolidation or interference? Learn. Mem. 24, 216–224. doi: 10.1101/lm.045047.117.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroes, M. C. W., Tendolkar, I., van Wingen, G. A., van Waarde, J. A., Strange, B. A., and Fernández, G. (2014). An electroconvulsive therapy procedure impairs reconsolidation of episodic memories in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 204–206. doi: 10.1038/nn.3609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. L. C. (2009). Reconsolidation: maintaining memory relevance. Trends Neurosci. 32, 413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. L. C., Nader, K., and Schiller, D. (2017). An update on memory reconsolidation updating. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 531–545. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. H., Choi, J. H., Lee, N., Lee, H. R., Kim, J. I., Yu, N. K., et al. (2008). Synaptic protein degradation underlies destabilization of retrieved fear memory. Science 319, 1253-1256. doi: 10.1126/science.1150541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. H., Kravitz, D. J., and Baker, C. I. (2019). Differential representations of perceived and retrieved visual information in hippocampus and cortex. Cereb. Cortex 29, 4452–4461. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. H., Kwak, C., Shim, J., Kim, J. E., Choi, S. L., Kim, H. F., et al. (2012). A cellular model of memory reconsolidation involves reactivation-induced destabilization and restabilization at the sensorimotor synapse in aplysia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 14200–14205. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211997109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, D. J. (1976). A cognitive approach to experimental amnesia. Am. J. Psychol. 89, 51–80. doi: 10.2307/1421753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manenti, R., Sandrini, M., Gobbi, E., Binetti, G., and Cotelli, M. (2020). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on episodic memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 75, 1403–1413. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manenti, R., Sandrini, M., Gobbi, E., Cobelli, C., Brambilla, M., Binetti, G., et al. (2017). Strengthening of existing episodic memories through non-invasive stimulation of prefrontal cortex in older adults with subjective memory complaints. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:401. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maples-Keller, J. L., Price, M., Jovanovic, T., Norrholm, S. D., Odenat, L., Post, L., et al. (2017). Targeting memory reconsolidation to prevent the return of fear in patients with fear of flying. Depress. Anxiety 34, 610–620. doi: 10.1002/da.22626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenzie, S., and Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Consolidation and reconsolidation: two lives of memories? Neuron 71, 224–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milekic, M. H., and Alberini, C. M. (2002). Temporally graded requirement for protein synthesis following memory reactivation. Neuron 36, 521–525. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00976-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R., and Lewis, D. J. (1968). Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 160, 554–555. doi: 10.1126/science.160.3827.554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monfils, M. H., and Holmes, E. A. (2018). Memory boundaries: opening a window inspired by reconsolidation to treat anxiety, trauma-related, and addiction disorders. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 1032–1042. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30270-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovitch, M., Cabeza, R., Winocur, G., and Nadel, L. (2016). Episodic memory and beyond: the hippocampus and neocortex in transformation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 105–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, K. M., and Davis, M. (2002). Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron 36, 567–584. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01064-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadel, L., Winocur, G., Ryan, L., and Moscovitch, M. (2007). Systems consolidation and hippocampus: two views. Debates Neurosci. 1, 55–66. doi: 10.1007/s11559-007-9003-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, K., and Einarsson, E. Ö. (2010). Memory reconsolidation: an update. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 27–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05443.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, K., and Hardt, O. (2009). A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 224–234. doi: 10.1038/nrn2590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., and Le Doux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406, 722–726. doi: 10.1038/35021052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakazawa, K., Sun, L. D., Quirk, M. C., Rondi-Reig, L., Wilson, M. A., and Tonegawa, S. (2003). Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucial for memory acquisition of one-time experience. Neuron 38, 305–315. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00165-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oyarzún, J. P., Lopez-Barroso, D., Fuentemilla, L., Cucurell, D., Pedraza, C., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., et al. (2012). Updating fearful memories with extinction training during reconsolidation: a human study using auditory aversive stimuli. PLoS One 7:e38849. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., and Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 976–987. doi: 10.1038/nrn2277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preston, A. R., and Eichenbaum, H. (2013). Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr. Biol. 23, R764-R773. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renoult, L., Irish, M., Moscovitch, M., and Rugg, M. D. (2019). From knowing to remembering: the semantic–episodic distinction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 1041–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rindal, E. J., DeFranco, R. M., Rich, P. R., and Zaragoza, M. S. (2016). Does reactivating a witnessed memory increase its susceptibility to impairment by subsequent misinformation? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 42, 1544–1558. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rugg, M. D., and Vilberg, K. L. (2013). Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandrini, M., Brambilla, M., Manenti, R., Rosini, S., Cohen, L. G., and Cotelli, M. (2014). Noninvasive stimulation of prefrontal cortex strengthens existing episodic memories and reduces forgetting in the elderly. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:289. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandrini, M., Censor, N., Mishoe, J., and Cohen, L. G. (2013). Causal role of prefrontal cortex in strengthening of episodic memories through reconsolidation. Curr. Biol. 23, 2181–2184. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, D., Monfils, M. H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, D. C., Ledoux, J. E., and Phelps, E. A. (2010). Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature 463, 49–53. doi: 10.1038/nature08637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, D., and Phelps, E. A. (2011). Does reconsolidation occur in humans? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 5:24. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, A. M., and Sherman, W. (1968). Amnesia: A function of the temporal relation of footshock to electroconvulsive shock. Science 159, 219–221. doi: 10.1126/science.159.3811.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, L., Nader, K., and Pruessner, J. C. (2014). Reconsolidation of human memory: Brain mechanisms and clinical relevance. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 274–280. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, L., and Wolf, O. T. (2009). New episodic learning interferes with the reconsolidation of autobiographical memories. PLoS One 4:e7519. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekeres, M. J., Bonasia, K., St-Laurent, M., Pishdadian, S., Winocur, G., Grady, C., et al. (2016). Recovering and preventing loss of detailed memory: differential rates of forgetting for detail types in episodic memory. Learn. Mem. 23, 72–82. doi: 10.1101/lm.039057.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekeres, M. J., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Pishdadian, S., Nichol, D., and Grady, C. L. (2021). Reminders activate the prefrontal-medial temporal cortex and attenuate forgetting of event memory. Hippocampus 31, 28–45. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., and Kindt, M. (2014). Prediction error demarcates the transition from retrieval, to reconsolidation, to new learning. Learn. Mem. 21, 580–584. doi: 10.1101/lm.035493.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiban, Y., Brütting, J., Pauli, P., and Mühlberger, A. (2015). Fear reactivation prior to exposure therapy: Does it facilitate the effects of VR exposure in a randomized clinical sample? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 46, 133–140. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.09.009

Google Scholar

Sinclair, A. H., and Barense, M. D. (2018). Surprise and destabilize: prediction error triggers episodic memory updating. Learn. Mem. 25, 369–381. doi: 10.1101/lm.046912.117.25

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, A. H., and Barense, M. D. (2019). Prediction error and memory reactivation: how incomplete reminders drive reconsolidation. Trends Neurosci. 42, 727–739. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soeter, M., and Kindt, M. (2012). Stimulation of the noradrenergic system during memory formation impairs extinction learning but not the disruption of reconsolidation. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 1204–1215. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soeter, M., and Kindt, M. (2015). An abrupt transformation of phobic behavior after a post-retrieval amnesic agent. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 880–886. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., Genzel, L., Wixted, J. T., and Morris, R. G. (2015). Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:a021766. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., Slater, P. C., and Chace, P. M. (1976). Reactivation of recent or remote memory before electroconvulsive therapy does not produce retrograde amnesia. Behav. Biol. 18, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/S0091-6773(76)92295-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St. Jacques, P. L. S., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Modifying memory: selectively enhancing and updating personal memories for a museum tour by reactivating them. Psychol. Sci. 24, 537–543. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457377.Modifying

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinfurth, E. C. K., Kanen, J. W., Raio, C. M., Clem, R. L., Huganir, R. L., and Phelps, E. A. (2014). Young and old Pavlovian fear memories can be modified with extinction training during. Learn. Mem. 21, 338–341. doi: 10.1101/lm.033589.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, A., Josselyn, S. A., Frankland, P. W., Masushige, S., Silva, A. J., and Kida, S. (2004). Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. J. Neurosci. 24, 4787–4795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5491-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., and Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2189–2208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tassone, L. M., Urreta Benítez, F. A., Rochon, D., Martínez, P. B., Bonilla, M., Leon, C. S., et al. (2020). Memory reconsolidation as a tool to endure encoding deficits in elderly. PLoS One 15:0237361. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Telch, M. J., York, J., Lancaster, C. L., and Monfils, M. H. (2017). Use of a brief fear memory reactivation procedure for enhancing exposure therapy. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 5, 367–378. doi: 10.1177/2167702617690151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teyler, T. J., and Rudy, J. W. (2007). The hippocampal indexing theory and episodic memory: Updating the index. Hippocampus 17, 1158–1169. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomita, H., Ohbayashi, M., Nakahara, K., Hasegawa, I., and Miyashita, Y. (1999). Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval. Nature, 401, 699–703. doi: 10.1038/44372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronson, N. C., and Taylor, J. R. (2007). Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 262–275. doi: 10.1038/nrn2090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 198–204.

Google Scholar

Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., and Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 277, 376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5324.376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., and Stickgold, R. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425, 616–620. doi: 10.1038/nature01930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, M. P., and Stickgold, R. (2016). Understanding the boundary conditions of memory reconsolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, E3991-E3992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607964113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S. H., De Oliveira Alvares, L., and Nader, K. (2009). Cellular and systems mechanisms of memory strength as a constraint on auditory fear reconsolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 905–912. doi: 10.1038/nn.2350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichert, S., Wolf, O. T., and Schwabe, L. (2011). Reactivation, interference, and reconsolidation: Are recent and remote memories likewise susceptible? Behav. Neurosci. 125, 699–704. doi: 10.1037/a0025235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, Y. X., Luo, Y. X., Wu, P., Shi, H. S., Xue, L. F., Chen, C., et al. (2012). A memory retrieval-extinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science 336, 241–245. doi: 10.1126/science.1215070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reconsolidation, memory retrieval, declarative memory, cortical circuit, memory age

Citation: Kim G, Kwon M, Kang W and Lee S-H (2021) Is Reconsolidation a General Property of Memory? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:643106. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.643106

Received: 17 December 2020; Accepted: 09 February 2021;
Published: 26 February 2021.

Edited by:

Andy C. H. Lee, University of Toronto, Canada

Reviewed by:

Marco Sandrini, University of Roehampton London, United Kingdom
Melanie Sekeres, University of Ottawa, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Kim, Kwon, Kang and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sue-Hyun Lee, suelee@kaist.ac.kr

Download