Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Virtual Real., 07 August 2020
Sec. Virtual Reality and Human Behaviour
Volume 1 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frvir.2020.00005

Flexible Recruitment of Balance Mechanisms to Environmental Constraints During Walking

  • 1Department of Kinesiology and Applied Physiology, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States
  • 2Department of Applied Kinesiology and Physiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 3Bertec Corporation, Columbus, OH, United States

Background: Humans need to actively control their upright posture during walking to avoid loss of balance. We do not have a comprehensive theory for how humans regulate balance during walking, especially in complex environments. The nervous system must process many aspects of the environment to produce an appropriate motor output in order to maintain balance on two legs. We have previously identified three balance mechanisms that young healthy adults use to maintain balance while walking: (1) The ankle roll mechanism, a modulation of ankle inversion/eversion; (2) The foot placement mechanism, a shift of the swing foot placement; and (3) The push-off mechanism, a modulation of the ankle plantarflexion angle during double stance. We know that these mechanisms are interdependent and can be influenced by internal factors such as the phase of the gait cycle and walking cadence. Here we seek to determine whether there are changes in neural control of balance when walking in the presence of environmental constraints.

Methods: Subjects walked on a self-paced treadmill while immersed in a virtual environment that provides three different colored pathways. Subjects were instructed not to step in the No-Step Zone, which appeared either on the right or left side of the subject. While walking, subjects received balance perturbations in the form of galvanic vestibular stimulation, providing the sensation of falling sideways, either toward the No-Step zone or toward the Neutral zone on the other side.

Results: The results indicate that the use of the balance mechanisms are altered depending on whether the perceived fall is toward the No-Step or the Neutral zone. Participants increased the use of the lateral ankle and foot placement mechanisms for No-Step stimuli resulting in a larger shift of the center of mass (CoM) compared to the Neutral stimuli.

Conclusion: This experiment provides further evidence that the balance control system during walking is extremely flexible, recruiting multiple mechanisms at different times in the gait cycle to adapt to environmental constraints.

Introduction

Human balance control has been extensively studied in standing (Winter et al., 1998; Peterka, 2002; Kiemel et al., 2008), but a comprehensive theory for how it is accomplished during walking is still not developed. Compared to standing, the gait cycle adds a layer of complexity to bipedal systems during locomotion. Balance while walking is further complicated in a dynamic and variable environment in walking compared to standing. Balance must be maintained in a variety of contexts, such as crowded city side-walks, rocky nature trails, walks on the beach, or fast-paced sporting events. How do environmental constraints contribute to the required motor output in order to maintain bipedal balance? Our interest here is to investigate a paradigm that systematically alters the use of the balance mechanisms during walking in response to constraints in the environment.

To our knowledge it is not understood how balance is achieved in the presence of environmental constraints. The majority of literature surrounding environmental constraints focuses on stepping (Rietdyk and Rhea, 2011) or steering around (Patla et al., 1991) an obstacle. These paradigms are valid representations of environmental constraints, as slipping and tripping combined account for an estimated 46% of falls among older adults (Leavy et al., 2015). If environmental constraints are imposed, we must adapt, and decide if falling in a certain direction has increased consequences or costs. Obviously any fall is undesirable, but certain types of falls may be more harmful, such as falling off a curb or a flight of steps.

Imagine a real-world situation that may alter how balance mechanisms are recruited, such as entering an environment that restricts stepping to one side. The extreme case being walking along a narrow mountain foot-path with a sheer cliff on one side. A sudden gust of wind or a trip may shift the center of mass to the restricted side. Does the nervous system change its response compared to unrestricted stepping? Here we investigate this very case in a virtual environment to simulate the cost of stepping in a particular direction with asymmetric constraints.

We know that the healthy human nervous system, without any environmental constraints, typically uses three major balance mechanisms (Reimann et al., 2018): (1) The ankle roll mechanism, a modulation of ankle inversion/eversion that shifts the center of pressure under the stance foot toward the perceived fall, pulling the body in the opposite direction; (2) The foot placement mechanism, a shift of the swing foot placement toward the perceived fall that shifts the center of pressure toward the perceived fall after heel-strike; and (3) The push-off mechanism, a modulation of the ankle plantarflexion/dorsiflexion angle of the trailing leg, accelerating the CoM not only in the anterior-posterior direction, but also in the medial-lateral direction to correct the perceived fall. The nervous system combines these mechanisms in different ways to maintain balance during walking, as the use of each mechanism varies on any given step (Fettrow T.D. et al., 2019), reflecting typical step-to-step variability. Furthermore, the relative use of the mechanisms is altered based on internal constraints, such as the phase of the gait cycle (Reimann et al., 2019), or the stepping cadence (Fettrow T. et al., 2019). This illustrates a highly flexible system that maintains balance during locomotion as it confronts changing task demands and environmental challenges.

Here we test whether the recruitment of the mechanisms depends on the perceived availability of places to step in the environment. It is known that vision is used to assign constraints to the environment in which the person is navigating (Patla et al., 1991; Patla and Greig, 2006; Jansen et al., 2011) and for planning of future step location (Matthis and Fajen, 2014). We created a paradigm that attempts to recreate the cliff trail example, without the factor of fear. The literature has focused on the use of the foot placement mechanism in order to maintain balance (Townsend, 1985; Bruijn and Van Dieën, 2018). We know the foot placement is actively modulated in response to a perceived fall (Hof and Duysens, 2013; Rankin et al., 2014; Reimann et al., 2017), and during normal walking (Wang and Srinivasan, 2014). If the foot placement mechanism is constrained, other mechanisms may change in order to respond to a perceived threat to balance. We attempted to achieve this by placing “No Step Zones” lateral to the subject's walking path, and periodically providing sensory perturbations to balance, providing the sensation of falling to the side, either toward the No-Step zone or the Neutral zone. We hypothesize that the foot placement mechanism will be reduced when the balance perturbation produces a perceived fall toward the No-Step zone.

Methods

Twenty healthy young subjects (13 female, 23.6 ± 4.48 years, 1.68 ± 0.096 m, 70.14 ± 10.48 kg) volunteered for the study. Subjects provided informed verbal and written consent to participate. Subjects did not have a history of neurological disorders or surgical procedures involving the legs, spine, or head. The experiment was approved by the University of Delaware Institutional Review Board (IRB ID: 1339047-4).

Experimental Design

Figure 1 shows the experimental setup, with subjects walking on a split-belt, instrumented treadmill (Bertec, Columbus, OH, USA, Figure 1A) within a virtual environment displayed via an Oculus Rift (Facebook Inc., Menlo Park, CA, USA, Figure 1B). The treadmill was self-paced, using a nonlinear PD-controller in Labview (National instruments Inc., Austin, TX, USA) to keep the middle of the posterior superior iliac spine markers on the mid-line of the treadmill. The head position in the virtual world was linked to the midpoint between the two markers on top of the Oculus headset, in which optic flow (forward motion) was defined by the treadmill speed. Rotation in the virtual world was updated via the IMU embedded in the Oculus. We adjusted yaw drift of the IMU through use of real-time rigid body 6DOF tracking of the head via Qualisys (Gothenburg, Sweden) motion capture software, according to the equation

δ=α(β-γ),    (1)

where β is the yaw angle of the head-mounted display rigid body from motion capture, γ the internal yaw angle of the Oculus IMU and α a gain factor. This equation corrected the internal yaw angle of the Oculus system to the motion capture value by a small factor each time step, effectively tethering the Oculus perspective to the motion capture to remove long-term drift from gyroscope integration. The gain parameter was set to α = 0.05.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Subjects walked on a self-paced treadmill, wearing a head-mounted display, while harnessed to a body-weight support system (not shown). The front handrail was used to assist in starting and stopping the self-paced treadmill comfortably. (B) The virtual reality scene displayed in the Oculus Rift. Subjects were instructed to walk on the green pathway and avoid stepping in the red No-Step zones. If the subject needed to, they were able to step in the gray Neutral zones.

The virtual environment was designed and implemented in Unity3d (Unity Technologies, San Francisco, CA, USA) and consisted of a tiled marble floor with floating cubes randomly distributed in a volume 0–10 m above the floor, 2–17 m to each side from the midline, and infinitely into the distance providing peripheral visual depth and texture. On top of the marble floor was a pathway with three colored lanes of equal width. These three virtual lanes were aligned with the treadmill belts and, in combination, fully covered the width of the treadmill (1.10 m, see Figure 1B). The middle lane was colored green. The left and right lanes alternated between gray and red, infinitely into the distance, each block with a length of 10 m. Instructions to the subjects were “Walk on the green path. Please do not step onto the red zones. It is ok to step onto the gray zones if you have to.” We will refer to the red areas as “No-Step” and the gray areas as “Neutral” zones.

After explaining the experiment, obtaining consent and placing markers and EMG sensors, subjects first walked for 15 min to adapt to the self-paced treadmill with the Oculus Rift. The instructions for starting the treadmill were to hold the handrail until the treadmill reached a comfortable pace, then let go and walk normally. During the adaptation we told the subjects that we would now perturb their sense of balance by use of the galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS), and asked them to cope with this perturbation “normally” and keep walking forward. Data collection blocks consisted of alternating phases of metronome and stimulus. During metronome phases, lasting 30 s, we provided an auditory metronome at 100 bpm and asked subjects to use this as an “approximate guideline” for their footsteps, both during metronome and stimulus phases. During stimulus phases, lasting 120 s, we turned the metronome off, and subjects received intermittent balance perturbations (details below). Data were collected during stimulus phases. Each subject performed four blocks of walking, each block consisting of five metronome and five perturbed phases, always starting with metronome phases, for a total of 12.5 min per block. After each block, the treadmill was turned off and subjects were offered a break. This protocol was implemented in a custom Labview program that sent the head position and treadmill speed to the Unity computer via UDP and saved the GVS current and treadmill speed at 100 Hz.

Forty-four reflective markers were placed on the subject, using the Plug-in Gait marker set (Davis III et al., 1991), with six additional markers on the anterior thigh, anterior tibia, and 5th metatarsal of each foot, to improve the accuracy of the 3-degrees of rotation calculations about the knee and ankle. Marker positions were recorded at 200 Hz using a Qualisys motion capture system with 13 cameras. Ground reaction forces and moments were collected at 1,000 Hz from both sides of the split-belt treadmill and transformed into a common coordinate frame to calculate whole-body center of pressure (CoP).

The vestibular perturbations were triggered on heel-strikes of either foot. The direction of the fall, determined by the polarity of the current, was always chosen in the direction of the foot triggering the stimulus. We identified heel-strikes as the local maxima of forward progression of the heel marker. Due to the alternation of the zones in the outer lanes, this could provide a sensation of falling either toward a No-Step or toward a Neutral zone. We also included a sham condition with no stimulus as an unperturbed reference, and randomly chose each trigger to be GVS or sham, with equal likelihood. GVS perturbations consisted of a 1 mA current passed between two round electrodes (3.2 cm diameter, Axelgaard Manufacturing 103 Co., Ltd, Fallbrook, CA, USA) placed on the mastoid processes for 1 s, inducing a feeling of falling to the side. The perturbation could only be triggered in the middle third between a No Step and a Neutral zone to avoid possible confusion near the zone switch lines, and was randomized to trigger 1–4 steps after entering the valid inner area. To allow the effects of the perturbation to wash out, further triggers were blocked for at least 10 steps.

Data Analysis

We low-pass filtered force plate data with a 4th order Butterworth filter at a cut-off frequency of 20 Hz and kinematic data with a 4th order Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 10 Hz. We calculated joint angle data from the marker data, based on a geometric model with 15 segments (pelvis, torso, head, thighs, lower legs, feet, upper arms, forearms, hands) and 38 degrees of freedom (DoF) in OpenSim (Delp et al., 2007; Seth et al., 2018) using an existing base model (Zajac et al., 1990). Joint angles were optimized by minimizing the weighted sum of distances between the observed marker positions and the positions of the virtual markers determined by the joint angles (Lu and O'Connor, 1999). We calculated the body CoM trajectories based on this model. To normalize the EMG trajectories, we divided each signal by the average over all control strides for each channel and subject.

We normalized the data between heel-strikes to 100 time steps. To estimate the motor response to the perturbations, we subtracted the average of the unperturbed sham triggers within each subject for all data. Thus, all of results will be presented as a difference from control, referred to as a response.

To improve the estimate of the foot placement response to the stimulus, we fitted a linear regression model relating the foot placement changes for each subject to the changes of lateral position and velocity of the CoM at midstance using the control data (Wang and Srinivasan, 2014). Then for each stimulus step, we used this model to estimate the expected foot placement change based on the CoM state, and subtracted this from the observed foot placement change, resulting in an estimate of the foot placement change due to the vestibular stimulus (Reimann et al., 2017). We will refer to this model-based estimate as model-corrected foot placement change.

The experimental design contains two distinct stimulus conditions, where each stimulus could induce a fall sensation either toward a No-Step zone or toward a Neutral zone. We will refer to these two conditions simply as No-Step or Neutral stimuli from here on. Spatially, the fall was always toward the triggering leg, which could be either the left or the right. After processing and filling gaps in the kinematic data, we were left with 1211 No-Step and 1207 Neutral stimuli.

Subjects were able to successfully maintain balance while walking on the self-paced treadmill, with no instances of stepping off the treadmill or making use of the body-weight support system. Occasionally subjects violated the task by stepping into the No-Step zone. Across all subjects, 64 such violations occurred during or after No-Step stimuli and 47 during or after Neutral stimuli. We removed these stretches of data from further analysis, which left 1,147 No-Step and 1,160 Neutral stimuli.

Outcome Variables

We expected that the effect of the vestibular perturbations would be different for fall stimuli toward No-Step zones compared to those toward Neutral zones. Our first hypothesis was that the whole-body CoM excursion would be larger for No-Step vs. Neutral. We further hypothesized that responses in the foot placement mechanism would be smaller, and responses in the ankle roll and push-off mechanism would be larger for No-Step vs. Neutral. Here we specify main and secondary outcome variables we calculated to test these hypotheses statistically.

For the overall balance response, we used:

(i) the Δ shift of the whole-body CoM at the end of the fourth step.

(ii) the Δ velocity at the end of the second post-stimulus step, based on previous results showing that the maximal changes are near these times (Reimann et al., 2018).

For the ankle roll mechanism, we analyzed four variables related to the stance leg lateral ankle activation:

(iii) the Δ CoP-CoM distance integrated over the first post-stimulus single stance.

(iv) the Δ stance leg ankle eversion/inversion angle integrated over the first post-stimulus single stance.

(v) the Δ stance leg peroneus longus EMG integrated over the first post-stimulus single stance.

(vi) the Δ stance leg tibialis anterior EMG integrated over the first post-stimulus single stance.

For the foot placement mechanism, we analyzed five variables related to the first post-stimulus swing leg heel strike:

(vii) the foot placement, defined as the Δ swing leg heel position relative to the trigger leg heel position at swing leg heel strike.

(viii) the model-corrected foot placement, defined as the difference between the measured foot placement value and the foot placement value predicted based on the position and velocity of the CoM at mid-stance using the linear model (see above).

(ix) the Δ trigger leg knee internal/external rotation angle at swing leg heel-strike.

(x) the Δ swing leg hip internal/external rotation angle at swing leg heel-strike.

(xi) the Δ swing leg hip abduction/adduction angle at swing leg heel-strike.

For the push-off mechanism, we analyzed two variables related to the push-off of the trailing leg before and during the second post-stimulus double stance:

(xii) the Δ plantarflexion angle integrated over the second post-stimulus double stance phase.

(xiii) the Δ medial gastrocnemius EMG of the stance leg integrated over the first post-stimulus swing phase.

Statistical Analysis

We confirmed the assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity by visual inspection of the residual plots for the variables related to foot placement, lateral ankle, and pushoff mechanisms. Our primary focus of analysis is the whole-body CoM shift and the kinematic, kinetic, and electromyographical variables associated with the ankle roll, foot placement, and push-off balance mechanisms.

To test our hypotheses about whether humans use the balance mechanisms differently across stimulus directions, we used R (R Core Team, 2013) and lme4 (Bates et al., 2009) to perform a linear mixed effects analysis. For each outcome variable, we fitted a linear mixed model and performed an ANOVA to analyze the use of the mechanisms and interaction of stimulus direction, using Satterthwaite's method (Fai and Cornelius, 1996) implemented in the R-package lmerTest (Kuznetsova, 2017). As fixed effects, we used the direction of the perturbation toward a No-Step or a Neutral zone. As random effects, we used individual intercepts for subjects. Table 1 displays the results for this statistical test on the outcome variables.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of the linear mixed model ANOVA.

Results

Table 1 shows the results of the linear mixed model ANOVA. Statistically significant differences between No-Step and Neutral are marked in bold.

We observed a whole-body balance response in both the No-Step and Neutral conditions. Figure 2A shows the effect of the balance response in the CoM shift in both conditions. The average CoM trajectories for the two conditions start to separate around the third post-stimulus step. As hypothesized, the effect of the perturbation is stronger in the No-Step. This difference is reflected in the CoM velocity results, shown in Figure 2B. Both of these differences are statistically significant (see Table 1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in the medial-lateral CoM position (A) and velocity (B). Curves start at the heel-strike triggering the stimulus and span four steps, ending at heel-strike of the same foot. Red curves correspond to fall stimuli toward the No-Step and gray curves to fall stimuli toward the Neutral zone. The gray areas correspond to double stance phases and the white areas correspond to single stance phases. Data shown are changes relative to the unperturbed steps, with shaded areas around the means giving the 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 3 shows the displacement of the CoP relative to the CoM. The CoP shifts in the direction of the perceived fall in both conditions, as expected. There appears to be no difference in the response to the perceived fall between the two conditions during the single stance period, though after the first post-stimulus heel-strike the average trajectories begin to diverge.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Changes in the medial-lateral CoP-CoM displacement in response to the perturbations. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and end at the triggering foot toe-off. Red curves correspond to fall stimuli toward the No-Step and gray curves to fall stimuli toward the Neutral zone. The gray areas correspond to double stance phases and the white areas correspond to single stance phases. Data shown are changes relative to the unperturbed steps, with shaded areas around the means giving the 95% confidence intervals.

Ankle Roll

The shift of the CoP and CoM are generated by a temporally coordinated set of balance mechanisms. Due to the stimulus occurring on heel strike, the first available response is in the ankle roll mechanism. Figure 4 shows the kinematic and electromyographic response of the lateral ankle mechanism. Figure 4A displays a triggering leg inversion during the first post-stimulus step relative to the unperturbed pattern. The ankle inversion change trends higher toward the end of single stance, but is not statistically different. Figure 4B displays the peroneous longus muscle activity, an ankle everter, and Figure 4C displays the tibialis anterior, an ankle inverter. Together, the changes in activation of these two muscles provide evidence the lateral ankle mechanism is actively generated. A decrease in peroneous longus activity and an increase in tibialis anterior activity yield an ankle inversion.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Variables illustrating the use of the lateral ankle mechanism. (A) The joint angle that contributes to the CoP-COM changes; and (B,C) the EMG that contributes to joint angle changes. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and end at the triggering foot toe-off. Red curves correspond to fall stimuli toward the No-Step and gray curves to fall stimuli toward the Neutral zone. The gray areas correspond to double stance phases and the white areas correspond to single stance phases. Data shown are changes relative to the unperturbed steps, with shaded areas around the means giving the 95% confidence intervals.

Foot Placement

Figure 5A shows that the foot placement was used in both conditions, as evidenced by the shift of the average response in the direction of the perceived fall. The difference in foot placement response was not statistically significant between the No-Step and Neutral conditions (1). The better estimate of the model-corrected foot placement change, however, shows a stronger difference between zone conditions that did reach statistical significance (Table 1), as seen in Figure 5B. Contrary to our expectation, the foot placement response to stimuli toward the No-Step zone was larger than the response to stimuli toward the Neutral zone, rather than smaller. Figure 6 shows the kinematic variables related to the foot placement. The the swing leg hip adducts, and internally rotates, and the trigger leg knee internally rotates, for both conditions. The combination of these three joint angle changes yield the foot placement change in the direction of the fall stimulus, displayed in Figure 6A. None of these kinematic measures are significantly different between conditions (Table 1).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The foot placement response (A) and the model-corrected foot placement (B) at the first post-stimulus step. Bars indicate the mean and the errors the 95% confidence interval.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Variables illustrating the use of the change of the foot placement mechanism over time. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in the heel position (A), and joint angles (B–D) that contribute to change in heel position. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and end at the triggering foot toe-off. Red curves correspond to fall stimuli toward the No-Step and gray curves to fall stimuli toward the Neutral zone. The gray areas correspond to double stance phases and the white areas correspond to single stance phases. Data shown are changes relative to the unperturbed steps, with shaded areas around the means giving the 95% confidence intervals.

Push-Off

Figure 7 shows that the push-off mechanism was also used to respond to the balance perturbation, with an increased plantarflexion and increased medial gastrocnemius activity for both conditions. According to Table 1, the integrated plantarflexion angle change quantifying the kinematic response did not significantly differ between conditions, but the medial gastrocnemius activity did differ significantly, with more medial gastrocnemius activity in the No-Step condition.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Variables illustrating the use of the push-off mechanism. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in ankle dorsiflexion (A), and the medial gastrocnemius EMG (B). Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and end at the triggering foot toe-off. Red curves correspond to fall stimuli toward the No-Step and gray curves to fall stimuli toward the Neutral zone. The gray areas correspond to double stance phases and the white areas correspond to single stance phases. Data shown are changes relative to the unperturbed steps, with shaded areas around the means giving the 95% confidence intervals.

Discussion

This experiment served as the first attempt in identifying how environmental constraints modify the use of the different balance mechanisms in walking. We expected virtual constraints would alter the use of the balance mechanisms. The virtual environment, shown in Figure 1B, paired with instructions not to step onto the red No-Step zones, was used to determine if environmental constraints alter the use of the balance mechanisms. We found that this paradigm produced subtle changes in the balance mechanisms that led to a robust difference in the whole-body CoM displacement.

We observed a separation of the CoM movement in the third and fourth steps following the stimulus onset. There was more of an overall shift of the CoM when the balance perturbation was toward the No-Step zone compared to the Neutral zone. Although there were no strong differences between the zone conditions for any of the individual balance mechanisms, the resulting shift of the CoM illustrates a collective effect which is not dominated by any particular mechanism.

Each balance mechanism is characterized with multiple variables and while all variables indicated change in the No Step condition, not all were statistically significant. For example, the lateral ankle mechanism appears to be consistently larger in the No-Step condition, with larger ankle inversion, decreased peroneous muscle activity, and increased tibialis anterior activity, shown in Figure 4. However, only the tibialis anterior muscle activity showed a significant difference between the No-Step and Neutral conditions. The foot placement mechanism was consistently larger in the No-Step Zone condition, contrary to our hypothesis. But only when foot placement was corrected to reflect the stimulus alone was statistical significance achieved. The push-off provides less compelling evidence to suggest a difference between conditions, despite a difference in medial gastrocnemius activity. Figure 7A shows a greater plantarflexion occurs in Neutral zone perturbations, but Figure 7B shows the muscle activity is trending lower relative to No-Step zone perturbations. The combination of the differences in the lateral ankle and the foot placement mechanisms in the first step post-stimulus likely generate the CoM shift we observe in the third and fourth steps post-stimulus. This is evidence that humans can indeed alter the mechanisms they use to maintain balance, particularly in the medial-lateral direction, based on perceived environmental constraints.

The modification of the balance response in the presence of an environmental constraint suggests substantial influence from central, supraspinal components similar conceptually to the idea of central set in standing balance control (Horak et al., 1989). Once the participant knows that a perturbation to the No Step zone is possible, the central nervous system must be prepared to change its response relative to the Neutral Zone.

The balance response was altered, despite randomization of perturbation direction, suggesting a descending command that alters the execution of the balance response. With a direction dependent central set in place, a primed activation of the balance mechanisms can push the body further from the undesired location (No-Step zone). In general, we argue the external constraint imposed leads to a tuning bias that effects the response to the perturbation.

The observed foot placement response, however, was contrary to our hypothesis. We expected a reduced foot placement response during No-Step stimuli, but instead observed an increased use of the foot placement mechanism. Multiple factors could contribute to this result. (1) Perception of the lower limbs was not salient while immersed in virtual reality. We intentionally did not provide any visual or auditory feedback of whether the individual was completely adhering to the protocol (i.e., never stepping in the red pathway). We opted for a minimalist development with no avatar or audio feedback. It is arguable that the presence of a virtual avatar would help the perception of the participant's own body in space. Previous research has shown motor adaptation in upper limb experiments in the presence of an avatar (Bourdin et al., 2019). However, studies about gait kinematics are still needed to understand the specific components of virtual reality immersion and their possible benefits (Ferreira dos Santos et al., 2015). (2) The green pathway was wide enough to make a foot placement while still abiding by the rules of the task. One strategy to improve the efficacy of the paradigm could be to narrow the path of normal walking or decrease the distance between the no-step zone and walking path. However, we would likely not want to constrain the path to the extent of narrow beam walking (Peterson and Ferris, 2018), as this form of walking entirely eliminates the ability to use the foot placement mechanism for the control of balance in the medial-lateral direction (Otten, 1999). (3) The participants did not perceive the task as having a high degree of risk (i.e., no consequences for stepping in area). We discuss these factors and possible solutions in the following paragraphs. In the current paradigm, there was no actual penalty for stepping in the No Step Zone besides the internal constraint of not following the task instructions. Despite this somewhat “weak” constraint, we still observed changes in the balance response. Our paradigm approximated the less extreme case of being disturbed toward a “muddy path” rather than a true no step zone (i.e., cliff).

We avoided the confounding factor of fear by creating a mundane environment and provided instructions to simply avoid stepping on a particular color. Fear and anxiety are known to modify behavior (Raffegeau et al., 2020) and spinal reflexes when exposed to increased heights (Sibley et al., 2007). Fear may be one method to provide an incentive to abide by such a constraint. It may also be possible to improve engagement through gamification of the task. Positive feedback for avoiding the no-step zone or negative feedback for stepping in the no-step zone may improve the engagement in this paradigm. Relative to other healthcare practices virtual reality is still in its infancy, yet many researchers have attempted to modify behavior through virtual reality in neurological populations (Felsberg et al., 2019). Further, a handful of researchers are attempting to modify and improve walking performance through gamification (Eggenberger et al., 2016; Schättin et al., 2016; Adcock et al., 2020).

This knowledge and paradigm will be particularly helpful for individuals that suffer from an injury or condition that prevent the use of a specific balance mechanism. For example, people with diabetic neuropathy or Parkinson's disease might refrain from using the ankle roll mechanism due to diminished proprioception. A paradigm similar to the one described here could be used to enhance the use of the ankle roll mechanism. We used vestibular stimulation to elicit balance responses, but similar elicitations could be provoked by mechanical perturbations (Banala et al., 2010; Acasio et al., 2017).

Conclusion

We created a task for walking in a virtual environment that can be used to study the effects of environmental constraints on the control of balance during locomotion. The results indicate that the neural control of balance changes in the presence of environmental constraints. These results contribute to the growing literature that balance control during walking is a coordinated series of responses that can flexibly change the recruitment of multiple mechanisms to remain upright across a wide variety of task and environmental constraints, as well as neurological state. Such flexibility suggests an avenue to target and rehabilitate specific balance mechanisms and strategies when balance is degraded due to neurological conditions.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from the individual for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

TF, FV, HR, and JJ designed the experiment. TF, HR, and SD performed the experiment and analyzed the data. TF, FV, HR, SD, and JJ wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

HR was funded from a Research Fellowship from the German Research Foundation (RE 3780/1-1). HR, TF, and JJ were funded by the National Science Foundation (NSF CRCNS 1822568).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work partially overlaps with work reported in TF′s dissertation (Fettrow, 2019).

References

Acasio, J., Wu, M. M., Fey, N. P., and Gordon, K. E. (2017). Stability-maneuverability trade-offs during lateral steps. Gait Posture 52, 171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adcock, M., Sonder, F., Schättin, A., Gennaro, F., and De Bruin, E. D. (2020). A usability study of a multicomponent video game-based training for older adults. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Activ. 17, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/s11556-019-0233-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banala, S. K., Agrawal, S. K., Kim, S. H., and Scholz, J. P. (2010). Novel gait adaptation and neuromotor training results using an active leg exoskeleton. IEEE/ASME Trans. Mechatron. 15, 216–225. doi: 10.1109/TMECH.2010.2041245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2009). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Science 325, 883–885. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourdin, P., Martini, M., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2019). Altered visual feedback from an embodied avatar unconsciously influences movement amplitude and muscle activity. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56034-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruijn, S. M., and Van Dieën, J. H. (2018). Control of human gait stability through foot placement. J. R. Soc. Interface 15, 1–11. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, R. B. III, Ounpuu, S., Tyburski, D., and Gage, J. R. (1991). A gait analysis data collection and reduction technique. Hum. Mov. Sci. 10, 575–587. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(91)90046-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delp, S. L., Anderson, F. C., Arnold, A. S., Loan, P., Habib, A., John, C. T., et al. (2007). OpenSim: Open-source software to create and analyze dynamic simulations of movement. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 54, 1940–1950. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2007.901024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggenberger, P., Wolf, M., Schumann, M., and de Bruin, E. D. (2016). Exergame and balance training modulate prefrontal brain activity during walking and enhance executive function in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fai, A. H.-T., and Cornelius, P. L. (1996). Approximate F-tests of multiple degree of freedom hypotheses in generalized least squares analyses of unbalanced split-plot experiments. J. Stat. Comput. Simul. 54, 363–378. doi: 10.1080/00949659608811740

CrossRef Full Text

Felsberg, D., Maher, J. P., and Rhea, C. K. (2019). The state of behavior change techniques in virtual reality rehabilitation of neurologic populations: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 10:979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira dos Santos, L. L., Christ, O., Mate, K., Schmidt, H., Krüger, J., Dohle, C., et al. (2015). Movement visualisation in virtual reality rehabilitation of the lower limb: a systematic review. Biomed. Eng. OnLine 15, 76–88. doi: 10.1186/s12938-016-0289-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fettrow, T. (2019). The control of balance and locomotion (Thesis). University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fettrow, T., Reimann, H., Grenet, D., Crenshaw, J., Higginson, J., and Jeka, J. (2019). Walking cadence affects the recruitment of the medial-lateral balance mechanisms. Front. Sports Active Liv. 1:40. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2019.00040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fettrow, T. D., Reimann, H., Thompson, E. D., Crenshaw, J., Higginson, J., Grenet, D., et al. (2019). Interdependence of balance mechanisms during walking. PLoS ONE 14:e225902. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, A. L., and Duysens, J. (2013). Responses of human hip abductor muscles to lateral balance perturbations during walking. Exp. Brain Res. 230, 301–310. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3655-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, F. B., Diener, H. C., and Nashner, L. M. (1989). Influence of central set on human postural responses. J. Neurophysiol. 62, 841–853. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.62.4.841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, S. E., Toet, A., and Werkhoven, P. J. (2011). Human locomotion through a multiple obstacle environment: Strategy changes as a result of visual field limitation. Exp. Brain Res. 212, 449–456. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2757-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiemel, T., Elahi, A. J., and Jeka, J. J. (2008). Identification of the plant for upright stance in humans: multiple movement patterns from a single neural strategy. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 3394–3406. doi: 10.1152/jn.01272.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavy, B., Byberg, L., Michaëlsson, K., Melhus, H., and Åberg, A. C. (2015). The fall descriptions and health characteristics of older adults with hip fracture: a mixed methods study. BMC Geriatr. 15:40. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0036-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, T. W., and O'Connor, J. J. (1999). Bone position estimation from skin marker co-ordinates using global optimisation with joint constraints. J. Biomech. 32, 129–134. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(98)00158-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthis, J. S., and Fajen, B. R. (2014). Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain. J. Exp. Psychol. 40, 106–115. doi: 10.1037/a0033101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otten, E. (1999). Balancing on a narrow ridge: biomechanics and control. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 354, 869–875. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patla, A., Prentice, S., Robinson, C., and Neufield, J. (1991). Visual control of locomotion: strategies for changing direction and für going over obstacles. J. Exp. Psychol. 17, 603–634. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.17.3.603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patla, A. E., and Greig, M. (2006). Any way you look at it, successful obstacle negotiation needs visually guided on-line foot placement regulation during the approach phase. Neurosci. Lett. 397, 110–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterka, R. J. (2002). Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1097–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, S. M., and Ferris, D. P. (2018). Differentiation in theta and beta electrocortical activity between visual and physical perturbations to walking and standing balance. Eneuro 5:e0207–18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0207-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffegeau, T. E., Fawver, B., Clark, M., Engel, B. T., Young, W. R., Williams, A. M., et al. (2020). The feasibility of using virtual reality to induce mobility-related anxiety during turning. Gait Posture 77, 6–13. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rankin, B. L., Buffo, S. K., and Dean, J. C. (2014). A neuromechanical strategy for mediolateral foot placement in walking humans. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 374–383. doi: 10.1152/jn.00138.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimann, H., Fettrow, T., Grenet, D., Thompson, E. D., and Jeka, J. J. (2019). Phase-dependency of medial-lateral balance responses to sensory perturbations during walking. Front. Sports Active Liv. 1:25. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2019.00025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimann, H., Fettrow, T., Thompson, E. D., and Jeka, J. J. (2018). Neural control of balance during walking. Front. Physiol. 9:1271. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimann, H., Fettrow, T. D. T., Thompson, E. D. E., Agada, P., McFadyen, B., Jeka, J. J., et al. (2017). Complementary mechanisms for upright balance during walking. PLoS ONE 12:e172215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rietdyk, S., and Rhea, C. K. (2011). The effect of the visual characteristics of obstacles on risk of tripping and gait parameters during locomotion. Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 31, 302–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2011.00837.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schättin, A., Arner, R., Gennaro, F., and de Bruin, E. D. (2016). Adaptations of prefrontal brain activity, executive functions, and gait in healthy elderly following exergame and balance training: a randomized-controlled study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:278. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A., Hicks, J. L., Uchida, T. K., Habib, A., Dembia, C. L., Dunne, J. J., et al. (2018). OpenSim: Simulating musculoskeletal dynamics and neuromuscular control to study human and animal movement. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14:e1006223. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, K. M., Carpenter, M. G., Perry, J. C., and Frank, J. S. (2007). Effects of postural anxiety on the soleus H-reflex. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, M. A. (1985). Biped gait stabilization via foot placement. J. Biomech. 18, 21–38. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(85)90042-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., and Srinivasan, M. (2014). Stepping in the direction of the fall: the next foot placement can be predicted from current upper body state in steady-state walking. Biol. Lett. 10, 1–5. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Winter, D. A., Patla, A. E., Prince, F., Ishac, M., Gielo-perczak, K., Jc, Q., et al. (1998). Stiffness control of balance in quiet standing. J. Physiol. 80, 1211–1221. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajac, F. E. F., Topp, E. E. L., Hoy, M. M. G., Rosen, J. J. M., Loan, J. P., Delp, S. L. S., et al. (1990). An interactive graphics-based model of the lower extremity to study orthopaedic surgical procedures. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 37, 757–767 doi: 10.1109/10.102791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual reality, locomotion, dynamic balance, walking, obstacles

Citation: Fettrow T, DiBianca S, Vanderlinde dos Santos F, Reimann H and Jeka J (2020) Flexible Recruitment of Balance Mechanisms to Environmental Constraints During Walking. Front. Virtual Real. 1:5. doi: 10.3389/frvir.2020.00005

Received: 16 April 2020; Accepted: 23 June 2020;
Published: 07 August 2020.

Edited by:

Ronan Boulic, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Mubbasir Kapadia, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, United States
Tabitha C. Peck, Davidson College, United States

Copyright © 2020 Fettrow, DiBianca, Vanderlinde dos Santos, Reimann and Jeka. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tyler Fettrow, tfettrow@ufl.edu

These authors share senior authorship

Download