- 1Department of Cognitive Science, Central European University, Budapest, Hungary
- 2Center for Subjectivity Research, Copenhagen University, Copenhagen, Denmark
- 3Interacting Minds Centre, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
- 4School of Psychological Science, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA
- 5Center for Semiotics, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
- 6Department of Sociology, Philosophy, and Anthropology, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
- 7Department of Pediatrics, Aarhus University Hospital, Skejby, Denmark
In the exploratory study reported here, we tested the efficacy of an intervention designed to train teenagers with Möbius syndrome (MS) to increase the use of alternative communication strategies (e.g., gestures) to compensate for their lack of facial expressivity. Specifically, we expected the intervention to increase the level of rapport experienced in social interactions by our participants. In addition, we aimed to identify the mechanisms responsible for any such increase in rapport. In the study, five teenagers with MS interacted with three naïve participants without MS before the intervention, and with three different naïve participants without MS after the intervention. Rapport was assessed by self-report and by behavioral coders who rated videos of the interactions. Individual non-verbal behavior was assessed via behavioral coders, whereas verbal behavior was automatically extracted from the sound files. Alignment was assessed using cross recurrence quantification analysis and mixed-effects models. The results showed that observer-coded rapport was greater after the intervention, whereas self-reported rapport did not change significantly. Observer-coded gesture and expressivity increased in participants with and without MS, whereas overall linguistic alignment decreased. Fidgeting and repetitiveness of verbal behavior also decreased in both groups. In sum, the intervention may impact non-verbal and verbal behavior in participants with and without MS, increasing rapport as well as overall gesturing, while decreasing alignment.
Introduction
Möbius syndrome (MS) is a form of congenital facial paralysis – typically complete and bilateral – resulting from maldevelopment of the sixth and seventh cranial nerves (1). MS is extremely rare, occurring in 2–20 births/million (2). Given the centrality of the face for expressing emotions and other mental states, and for communicating other non-verbal social cues, it is natural to expect that people with MS may experience difficulties in their social interactions.
Indeed, some studies corroborate these expectations. One study by Briegel (3) found that people with MS had a significantly higher incidence of inhibition, introversion, and interpersonal sensitivity (feelings of inadequacy and inferiority), a lower satisfaction with life, a less pronounced achievement orientation, and a greater tendency toward psychological distress. However, in a larger study, Bogart and Matsumoto (4) compared 37 Americans with MS to 37 matched controls without facial paralysis and found no differences between the groups in depression or anxiety, although they did find that participants with MS had lower social competence. The picture is, thus, far from clear, and some of the differences in results may plausibly be attributed to differences in sample size, measures, or cultural differences in disability stigma (5). The conclusion to be drawn from these studies is that some people with MS manage well, whereas others may benefit from psychosocial support.
In order to identify ways of improving the level of comfort and satisfaction in social interactions of people with MS, it is important to distinguish several (mutually consistent and related) reasons why facial paralysis may lead to difficulties in social interaction, ranging from impairment and processing of facial expression, to the way this resonates in the interaction and impacts the interlocutor. First of all, some researchers have speculated that facial mimicry may contribute to social understanding by playing a central role in the identification of others’ emotions on the basis of their facial expressions (6, 7). The suggestion is that in order to identify what type of emotion an individual is experiencing, it may be necessary to facially mimic, or simulate, their facial expression. Thus, individuals with deficits in producing, experiencing, or expressing an emotion may also have a deficit in the face-based recognition of that same emotion when they see it in others [e.g., Ref. (6, 8, 9)]. Given the lack of facial expressivity on the part of people with MS, the simulation model predicts that they may be impaired at face-based emotion recognition – which could explain some of the difficulties they experience with social interaction.
There has been some research bearing upon this hypothesis, so far with mixed results. Most recently, Bate et al. (10) reported that five of six participants with MS were impaired in at least one of the three tasks they employed to assess face-based emotion recognition. However, only one of the participants in this study was impaired on a task that involved imagining facial expressions of emotions and answering questions about them. As the authors point out, this is not consistent with the simulation model. Moreover, in another recent study, Bogart and Matsumoto (11) compared 37 Americans with MS to 37 matched American controls without facial paralysis, and found no significant difference in performance on a face-based emotion recognition task. The differences between these findings may be due in part to differences in the methodologies employed in the two studies. It is possible, for example, that people with MS employ alternative means for face-based emotion recognition to compensate for the absence of facial mimicry. If so, one might expect them to perform equally well if not better than controls on measures, such as detailed facial processing, but worse on others, for example, facial processing under time pressure.
In any case, several other factors are likely to be equally or even more important than any deficit in face-based emotion recognition on the part of people with MS. First, it is highly probable that other people have difficulties in dealing with their interlocutors’ facial paralysis, creating awkward interactions. Since people are accustomed to receiving information about others’ mental states from their facial expressions, the absence of this expected information may interrupt an interaction partners’ facial mimicry and cause him or her to feel uncomfortable or confused about what the person with MS is thinking or feeling. This conjecture is corroborated by evidence that individuals with facial movement disorders, such as MS or Parkinson’s disease, are often perceived as unhappy, unfriendly, depressed, disinterested, or unintelligent (12–15), making others less likely to pursue engagement and friendships with them (16). In sum, the social difficulties experienced by many people with MS may lie partially with their interaction partners without MS, who may for various reasons feel uncomfortable or confused. Consequently, the smooth flow of interaction is interrupted through their uncertainty about how to interact in what is for them a new and sensitive situation. In turn, children with MS might find themselves in more awkward and difficult interactions than non-MS children, potentially impacting the development of their social skills (9, 10, 17)1, and perhaps also leading to an impoverishment of their own emotional experience (19).
In examining social interactions, it would, thus, be just as naïve to focus narrowly on the interaction partner with MS as it would be to focus narrowly on the interaction partner without MS, as their individual behaviors are not generated in a vacuum, but resonate with and constrain each other (20, 21). In other words, some of the most relevant consequences of MS for social interaction may stem from the effects that the lack of facial expressivity has upon the dynamics of the interaction itself. After all, people with MS are deprived of an important means for displaying emotional appraisal, giving feedback and indicating understanding. Facial expressivity is, thus, an important component in the continuous reciprocal adaptations between interlocutors that supports the alignment and sharing of emotional states, perspectives and attitudes, and facilitates understanding. Although there is a broad consensus about the importance of reciprocal facial expressivity for social interaction and social cognition, there are also many open questions. To what extent are facial expressions flexibly coordinated, deliberately controlled and potentially replaced, and how do they interact with other aspects of interactive behavior, such as vocal prosody and hand gestures (22–24)? This latter cluster of questions may be especially relevant for people with MS, who may benefit by compensating for their limited facial expressivity with some other form of expressivity.
Use of Compensatory Strategies
Although many people with MS report difficulties in their social interactions, others clearly are professionally and personally happy, and have little trouble with social interactions. Bogart et al. (25) suggest that, as observed in many neuropsychiatric conditions (26, 27), some people with MS may have developed compensatory strategies to manage their social interactions. To understand how they achieve this would be valuable, as it would potentially help other people with MS (as well as people with facial paralysis and facial difference arising from other causes) in adopting the compensatory strategies employed by these more socially successful individuals.
Anecdotal evidence suggests that such compensatory strategies likely include expressing oneself more with hand gestures and body language as well as prosody (19). Moreover, in qualitative interviews, many people with MS reported that they deliberately compensated by using eye contact to display confidence, and prosody, body language, and verbal disclosure to express emotion (25, 28, 29). In a behavioral study, Bogart et al. (30) found that people with MS used more of these compensatory expressive behaviors than people with acquired facial paralysis, who may not be as well adapted to their impairment due to the later stage at which they acquired it. Moreover, there is evidence that social skills training programs can benefit people with facial differences who experience problems with social interaction (31). Thus, developing a social skills workshop that promotes the use of alternative channels, such as prosody, gestures, and verbal disclosure, to communicate emotion may benefit people with MS, or other types of facial paralysis, or facial difference.
The conjecture that bodily and prosodic expressivity can substitute for facial expressivity is also supported by the results of a recent study in which Bogart et al. (13) asked participants to judge the emotions of people with facial paralysis based on all communication channels or a limited number of channels (e.g., face only or voice + speech + body, etc.). Results showed that participants were better able to assess the emotions when more communication channels were available, suggesting that they were perceiving in a holistic manner, taking into account information from multiple channels. Furthermore, they formed more positive impressions of individuals who used more compensatory means of expression.
Interactive Alignment and Rapport
In addition to communicating and sharing emotions, an increase in bodily expressivity may support social interaction via interactive alignment, which can be defined as the tendency of interaction partners spontaneously to imitate each other’s patterns of movement and speech (32). Interactive alignment has been observed in many aspects of interactive behavior from the spontaneous temporal synchronization of movements (33), to expressive and postural mimicry (34, 35), and to multiple dimensions of linguistic coordination (32, 36–38) and has been speculated to subserve important social functions. Alignment has been found to promote rapport, defined as a state of connectedness involving mutual attentiveness, positivity, and responsiveness (39–43), and thereby also increase participants’ willingness to cooperate with each other (44, 45). Thus, a possible lack of alignment in interactions involving people with MS – due to the impaired facial expressivity in one interlocutor and/or to general uneasiness – could have significant consequences, leading to lower rapport and coordination with potentially impaired reciprocal understanding.
However, a handful of studies point to a different set of predictions. Borrie and Liss (46) have shown that individuals tend to align their verbal behavior more if one of the interlocutors has a speech impairment. Analogously, in an analysis of psychotherapeutic sessions, Reich and colleagues highlighted the presence of higher prosodic alignment for poorer relationships between therapist and client as well as for greater client distress (47). These findings imply that alignment might decrease after an intervention that successfully reduced the level of social anxiety and the need to compensate for a lack of interactional fluency. In a similar vein, it has been argued that in many forms of interactions, complementary patterns and not just alignment of behavior might be a better predictor of rapport and efficacy of the interaction. In good interactions, the prosodic pattern indicating a question will be followed by the prosodic pattern indicating an answer; an impromptu rising of the voice could be followed by a lowering of the intensity to avoid an emotional escalation; and interlocutors will not repeat each other’s words, but contribute new topics to the conversations (20, 21, 24, 38).
In other words, a change in interactive alignment might be crucial in assessing the efficacy of the interaction, but it is unclear whether increased or decreased alignment would be the desired development.
The Current Study
As the foregoing remarks indicate, there are at least three factors that may present challenges to people with MS in social interactions: (1) emotion perception and recognition may be difficult for some people with MS; (2) the absence of facial expressivity can make it difficult for others to understand them, and can create a cold or disinterested impression; and (3) their facial impairment may interfere with one central medium for interactive alignment processes which can engender rapport.
Insofar, as these three factors may present challenges to people with MS, they also provide three entry points for a social skills intervention designed to help people with MS. We, therefore, incorporated exercises targeting all three factors into an intervention for teenagers with MS. The main aim of the present study was to test the efficacy of this workshop. Specifically, we expected that the level of rapport experienced in social interactions would increase from before the intervention to afterward, as measured by independent coders and by self-report. A further aim of the study was to identify the mechanisms responsible for any such increase in rapport – focusing not only on the behavior of the participants with MS but also on their interaction partners without MS and on the dynamics of the interaction itself. We predicted that all participants would show greater expressivity of their body and voice after the intervention than before, as measured by behavioral coders rating the videos, and also that the level of expressivity would correlate with rapport. Moreover, we hypothesized that interactive alignment would change after the intervention, and that it, too, would correlate with rapport. Either direction of change would be supported by the literature. Consequently, we endeavored to use our findings to further articulate our understanding of interactive alignment.
Materials and Methods
Design Overview
The present study was built around an intervention designed to train people with MS to adopt alternative strategies to compensate for the unavailability of facial expression in social interactions and to increase social comfort. The experiment was conducted as a 2-day workshop at the Pindstrup Centre in Denmark, and was approved by the local chapter of the Danish Research Ethics Committee. In order to assess possible changes in expressive behavior and rapport following the intervention, participants completed dyadic interactions with naïve interlocutors both at baseline and after the intervention.
Participants
We recruited five teenagers (three females) with MS between 14 and 19 years old (mean age 16.20, SD = 1.92) (henceforth “MS participants”). We judged that this age group would stand to benefit most from the social skills intervention, as people with MS report their teenage years being the most challenging (25, 28). MS participants were observed to have moderate to severe facial paralysis. They were referred for the study by a physician (the sixth author of the study, who is a trained neurologist and pediatrician) who affirmed that they were cognitively able, and all were attending mainstream school at a level appropriate to their age. Using a university database as well as word of mouth, we also recruited 10 people without MS (five females) in the same age range (mean age 17.70, SD = 2.79). All 10 non-MS participants were informed that they would be asked to engage in brief, videotaped interactions with teenagers with bilateral facial paralysis. All participants (and, for those under 18, their parents) gave their informed written consent. Of the 10 participants without MS (henceforth “non-MS participants”), five participated on the first day (before the intervention) and five on the second day (after the intervention). The reason for this was that if the non-MS participants had participated on both days, a confound would have arisen insofar as our results might have been explained by the increasing familiarity with the procedure, with the participants with MS, and with MS in general, rather than by the intervention. Thus, each MS participant interacted with six different non-MS participants, and each non-MS participant interacted with three MS participants. Multiple interactions with different interlocutors were important to enable the individual variability of MS and non-MS participants to be accounted for in the statistical analysis.
Procedure
Intervention
The intervention was based on the social skills workshop developed by the UK organization for people with visible differences, Changing Faces. A randomized controlled trial found that burned adolescents who received the intervention were significantly less withdrawn, had fewer somatic complaints, and less behavioral problems compared to the control group at the 1-year follow-up (31). To address the unique expressive challenges of facial paralysis, the intervention was modified to encourage the use of compensatory expressive behaviors. The intervention was designed by the second author and delivered by the second and fifth authors of the study, both psychologists. The 2-day group workshop involved instruction (targeting non-verbal and verbal expression and recognition, conversational skills), group discussion of personal experiences, role-playing, group activities, and writing exercises. The first day involved a group discussion of different modes of expression, including the face, body, prosody, gesture, posture, backchannel responses, and style. They were instructed that people typically look to the face as the primary source of information, but that with MS, other modes of communication can be used instead. The role of mimicry in communication was also discussed. Group activities involved observing and recognizing emotions in themselves and others. For example, one exercise was a charades game in which participants had to express and recognize emotions without naming them. At the end of the first day, participants were assigned homework to interact with one new person and with a family member, and to observe their communication skills with each person. On the second day, they discussed their observations from their homework assignment. Then, participants discussed their feelings during social interactions and how others might feel when meeting them. Communication skills to put others at ease, including behaving in a friendly, confident manner, were trained. In a writing assignment, participants prepared several ways to quickly explain MS to others and shared them with the group. Additionally, participants generated personal examples of social situations that were successful and that were challenging. They then role-played the challenging situations and discussed what could have been done differently. The workshop manual is available upon request.
Dyadic Interactions
During test-phase interactions, the participants with MS were paired with non-MS interlocutors. Pairing was randomized and blocked to ensure that participants of similar ages were paired (the maximum age difference was 2 years). In order to elicit emotional communication in a naturalistic way, the interlocutors were instructed to tell each other about recent, enjoyable experiences. Each MS participant went through six interactions in total, three before the intervention and three afterward. Each interaction lasted approximately 6–8 min and was considered as a whole. Participants sat in armless chairs to allow for unrestricted arm movement. There were two video cameras per dyad; one to record each participant.
Measures
In order to assess the impact of intervention on the quality of the interactions, we employed multiple measures of rapport as well as verbal and non-verbal behaviors in all interactions before and after the intervention. Based on previous research involving behavioral coding of compensatory expression of people with facial paralysis, fidgeting, and head and face movements were expected to be particularly relevant non-verbal behaviors (30). Pitch and speech rate were chosen amongst the most commonly reported measures of prosodic coordination in conversations, and for their reported correlation with rapport and efficacy of interpersonal coordination (21, 47–49). We also administered questionnaires to assess the social competence (50) and social anxiety (51) of MS participants at three points: immediately before the experiment, immediately afterward, and 6 months later.
Rapport
All participants completed self-report rapport scales (52) after each interaction. An example item is “did you feel in rapport with him or her?” Responses to items were averaged, with scores ranging from 1 to 6, with 6 indicating higher rapport. To measure observed rapport, the two video recordings from each interaction were synced and spliced together using Adobe Premiere Pro CS6 with a handclap as a sync point. Thus, each interlocutor could be viewed at the same time in a split screen. Based on the method of Bernieri (41), five behavioral coders who were blind to the hypotheses viewed these videos with the sound turned off and rated the shared liking of the dyad on a scale from 1 to 5. Inter-rater reliability (Chronbach’s α) was 0.70.
Non-Verbal Behaviors
Five behavioral coders blind to the hypotheses watched the videos and scored the quality, intensity, and frequency of gestures and bodily cues used by both interaction partners. The coding system employed in Bogart et al. (30) was used to measure the expressivity of individuals with facial paralysis and of their interaction partners. Coders rated the following four items: gesture (α = 0.86), fidgeting (α = 0.78), head movements (α = 0.68), and facial expressivity (α = 0.93). Ratings were performed on a 1–5 scale, with higher numbers indicating more expressivity.
Verbal Behaviors
Pitch and speech rate were extracted from the audio recordings of the conversations employing a semi-automated process. First, a research assistant blind to the hypotheses carefully watched the videos and manually tagged onsets and offsets of the interlocutors’ speech turns to ensure accurate speaker attribution. The manual tags were then adjusted to a 10-ms precision scale through an automated analysis of pitch presence/absence and intensity changes using Matlab (Mathworks Inc.). We extracted pitch (fundamental frequency, Hertz) within normal human voice range (70–400 Hz) and intensity (decibel) at a 10 ms scale using Praat (53) and corrected for artifactual octave jumps. We then calculated speech rate as estimated syllables per minute. To do so, we first isolated all utterances by interlocutor, excluding speech overlapping, and individuated voiced peaks in intensity as proxies for vowel onsets according to the procedure in de Jong and Wempe (54). In order to be able to capture shared dynamics between the two interlocutors, we needed uniformly sampled time-series. The vowel onsets were, therefore, converted to estimated syllables per minute sampled every 333 ms, following the validated procedure in Wallot et al. (55).
Analysis
Impact of Intervention on Rapport
We chose two measures of rapport: the mean of self-reported rapport from the two interlocutors, and rapport as assessed by the coders. First, we measured the relation between self-reported rapport in interlocutors using a mixed effect correlation with identity of the individual interlocutors as random factor, and we tested the impact of intervention by comparing the base model with a model including a before/after intervention fixed factor (56). We then measured the impact of the intervention on rapport by employing a mixed-effects multivariate model with random intercepts with before/after intervention as fixed factor and the identity of the individual interlocutors as random factors (random intercepts only, as the models would generally not converge with random slopes). Multiple testing was corrected using Student–Newman–Keuls corrections. We report estimates of effect size of the overall model (R2) and of the fixed factors (R2 marginal), following the procedure described in Nakagawa and Schielzeth (57). Estimates and confidence intervals of the descriptive statistics were calculated via bootstrapping 1000 times, stratifying by interlocutor identity. The mixed-effects models were calculated using the lme4 package for R (58).
Impact of Intervention on Individual Behaviors
First we assessed the impact of intervention on individual behaviors: gesture, fidgeting, facial expressivity, head movement, pitch SD, speech rate mean and SD. Pitch mean was not employed, as it would vary greatly according to the gender and age of the participants. We employed 2-by-2 repeated measures models with two fixed factors (MS vs. non-MS and before vs. after intervention). Interlocutors’ identity was defined as a random factor (random intercepts only, as the models would generally not converge with random slopes). Estimates and confidence intervals of the descriptive statistics were calculated via bootstrapping 1000 times, stratifying by interlocutor identity. Again the mixed effect models were calculated using the lme4 package for R. Descriptive statistics of individual behaviors before and after intervention divided by group are available in Table 1.
Impact of Intervention on Alignment
In order to assess the presence of coordination of non-verbal behaviors between interlocutors, we employed mixed effect models evaluating the relation between the coded levels (e.g., gesture) in the two interlocutors, using interlocutor identity as a random effect. This enabled us to assess whether the actual interaction created statistically significant alignment between interlocutors. Second, we assessed whether intervention changed the strength of the matching. To achieve this, we examined whether the addition of before/after intervention as an additional fixed factor in the model would statistically improve it and influence the direction of the impact. The more fine-grained nature of our linguistic data enabled us to assess not only the presence of behavioral matching but also whether it was due to the actual dynamics of interaction as opposed to the general structure of conversation. We, therefore, employed cross recurrence quantification analysis (CRQA) (59), a non-linear and more flexible analog of cross correlation, which quantifies shared dynamics between time-series and has been shown to be more sensitive to the dynamics of interpersonal coordination [Ref. (60); for a comprehensive review of the application of CRQA to interpersonal coordination, cf. Ref. (20, 38)]. Relying on the time-series produced by each interlocutor (e.g., a sequence of estimated speech rate regularly sampled over time), CRQA reconstructs the phase space of possible combinations of states and quantifies the structure of recurrence, that is, of the number of instances in which the two time-series are displaying similar dynamics and the characteristics of these instances. This generates several indexes of fundamental frequency and speech rate coordination:
• Level of coordination: defined as the percentage of values that recur (are present) in both time-series independently of the lag [recurrence rate (RR)]
• Stability of coordination, articulated in tendency of recurrence to happen in sequences (as opposed to isolated points being repeated, DET), average length of sequences repeated across time-series (L), and length of longest repeated sequence (LMAX)
• Complexity of coordination: defined as low if all repeated sequences are of the same length, high is repeated sequences vary in length [entropy (ENTR)], thus suggesting that coordination is flexible and not mechanical imitation.
We then tested whether the intervention impacted the level of coordination employing mixed-effects models as in the previous analysis. CRQA was performed using the CRP toolbox for Matlab. Estimates and confidence intervals of the descriptive statistics were calculated by bootstrapping 1000 times, stratifying by interlocutor identity. The mixed-effects models were calculated using the lme4 package for R.
Relation between Rapport and Interactional Behaviors
Finally, we assessed the relation between interactional behaviors and rapport. In particular, we attempted to statistically predict self-reported and observer-coded rapport as a function of our measures of verbal and non-verbal interactional behaviors, including measures of alignment. This enabled us to assess (i) how well the behavioral indexes could predict experience of the interaction, (ii) whether employing multiple behavioral indexes adds to our ability to assess the interaction or they all express the same underlying dynamic, and (iii) whether our results would be sensitive to individual baselines, or generalize across participants. To obviate the issues due to the large number of features, for each measure of rapport we used a fivefold cross-validated process (61) in which we first selected a limited amount of minimally overlapping features using ElasticNet (62, 63) and then ran a multiple regression, calculating the accuracy of the model using adjusted R square (Adj R2) (63). The cross-validation process ensured that the accuracy of the model was calculated only for test datasets on which the model had not been trained. Cross-validation was performed at the level of the interlocutors, meaning that the model was never tested on conversations including interlocutors on which the model was trained. These analyses were performed using the statistics and bioinformatics toolboxes for Matlab.
Results
Impact of Intervention on Rapport
Self-reported rapport was positively correlated between interlocutors: R2 = 0.42 (CI: 0.26, 0.57), with fixed factor accounting for a marginal R2 of 0.17 (CI: 0.03, 0.41), p = 0.001. The correlation did not statistically change due to intervention (p = 0.61). Self-reported rapport did not statistically change after intervention, while the coder-assessed rapport showed a significant increase (cf. Table 2).
Analysis of Individual Behaviors
We observed significant main effects for both MS vs. non-MS and before vs. after intervention on individual behaviors (for full descriptive statistics, cf. Table 1). Non-MS participants exhibited higher degrees of gesturing, and facial expressivity as well as fidgeting (but not head movements). They also exhibited a less variable speech rate (cf. Table 3).
After intervention, we observe a statistical increase of gesturing, facial expressivity, and head movements in both groups of interlocutors. Speech rate slowed down, but it exhibited higher variability (cf. Table 4).
We observed non-significant trends for the interaction between the two factors (intervention and MS) in speech rate variability, facial expressivity, gesture, and fidgeting.
After the intervention, MS participants (compared to non-MS) displayed higher gesturing (mean difference: 0.58, CI: 0.21 0.95, R2 marginal = 0.15, p = 0.0003) and facial expressivity (mean difference: 0.28, CI: 0.17 0.53, R2 marginal = 0.01, p = 0.029), while non-MS displayed lower fidgeting (mean difference: −0.35, CI: −0.76 0.05, R2 marginal = 0.06, p = 0.05), and higher speech rate variability (mean difference: 6.33, CI: 3.81 8.84, R2 marginal = 0.09, p < 0.0001).
Impact of Intervention on the Structure of Interactional Behavior
We observed a significant correlation between the interlocutors’ facial expressivity, and a marginally significant correlation for gesture. The correlations for fidgeting and head movements were not significant (cf. Table 5). These correlations did not change statistically due to intervention (p > 0.28).
We observed a statistical decrease in linguistic coordination after the intervention (cf. Table 6)2.
Möbius syndrome participants’ responses to the questionnaires probing social competence and social anxiety revealed no significant differences across the three points (immediately before the experiment, immediately afterward, and 6 months later).
Relation between Interactional Behaviors and Rapport
Based on interactional behaviors, we can statistically predict coder-assessed rapport. Amount (RR) and stability (L and LMAX) of speech rate coordination were selected by ElasticNet as the minimal set of significant, combined predictors and explained 28% of the variance of rapport: Adj R2 = 0.28, p = 0.0007. In other words, the lesser the coordinated and stable speech rate dynamics, the higher the coder-assessed rapport. Self-reported rapport could not be statistically predicted.
Discussion
Summary of the Results
Our findings suggest that the intervention had an impact on individual expressive behavior and behavioral alignment. Observer-coded rapport was statistically higher after the intervention, though not self-reported rapport. The increase in observed rapport provides initial support for the prediction that the deliberate employment of compensatory expressive strategies by people with MS can enhance the rapport that they – and their interaction partners – experience in social interactions.
Both interlocutors displayed higher non-verbal expressivity and a more varied speech rate after the intervention. MS participants in particular showed a more marked increase in gesturing and facial expressivity, while non-MS participants showed a more marked decrease in fidgeting and their speech rate became more varied. Matching of non-verbal behavior between interlocutors did not statistically change, while linguistic alignment – statistically present throughout the whole testing – decreased after intervention. Most importantly, the change in alignment seems to be related to the change in experience of the interaction. Externally rated rapport (but not self-reported rapport) was shown to be most strongly – and negatively – correlated with speech rate alignment.
Although these findings generally corroborate the predictions we had articulated, there are several points that merit careful consideration: the lack of results for self-reported rapport, the decrease in linguistic alignment associated with increased rapport, and the limited sample size of the study. We shall now briefly discuss each of these as well as several possibilities for further research that are suggested by our findings.
The Effect of Social Norms on Self-Reported Rapport
It is interesting to note that self-reported rapport did not statistically increase with intervention, and could not be correlated with interactional behaviors, while externally coded rapport could. Personal experience is a crucial dimension to investigate in interactions, and self-report often provides useful information [e.g., Ref. (64)]. However, self-report is often influenced by social norms and by various forms of self-deception (65). One possibility is that both interlocutors may have rated the interaction positively because of politeness norms. Indeed, self-reported rapport was relatively high regardless of whether the intervention had occurred. Non-verbal behavior is considered less susceptible to social desirability and more indicative of internal states because it is more difficult to consciously control than verbal reports (66, 67). So, although more research is needed on getting reliable self-report of rapport, the finding that observer-rated rapport and expressivity improved, and that we could relate changes in alignment to higher observer-rated rapport, suggests we accomplished the more difficult task of improving implicit intergroup rapport.
Higher Rapport is Related to Decreased Interactive Alignment
We observed a significant decrease in both pitch and speech rate alignment, but no significant change in non-verbal alignment, and this difference might be due to differences in the dynamics of different behavioral modalities. However, a more plausible explanation lies in the difference in the methods employed to quantify the behaviors and to assess the alignment. Verbal behavior was extracted as a temporal sequence, and analyzed for shared dynamics between interlocutors. Non-verbal behavior was aggregatively assessed in terms of quantity (e.g., of gesture) and analyzed for analogous amounts between interlocutors. The first procedure is more sensitive, and we endeavor to apply analogous measures to non-verbal behavior in future work.
As to the decrease in linguistic alignment and its positive relation to rapport, the finding resonates with a series of recent studies (20, 21, 38, 46, 47, 68) associating alignment with struggles to repair disfluent interactions. Thus, higher alignment in pre-intervention interactions might be an index of higher levels of social impairment, which would decrease after intervention. In support of this interpretation, we observe that both interlocutors – and non-MS participants in particular – show lower degrees of fidgeting and more variance in speech rate after intervention. This could be indicative of a decrease in awkwardness and nervousness. Higher alignment (in the form of more similar temporal dynamics) before intervention might be due partly to awkward and repetitive behaviors.
In sum, our findings add to the growing literature contributing to a richer articulation of current models of alignment: alignment seems to be built upon multiple mechanisms and not to relate in a straightforward fashion to rapport and fluent interactions. It follows that instead of being conceptualized as a simple and automatic mechanism, alignment should be carefully situated in the context of the interaction and its goals and related to the characteristics of the individual behaviors of the interlocutors (20, 38).
Limitations
The current findings should be considered as pointers for further studies. The small number of available participants with MS in the target age group in Denmark made it very difficult to increase the sample size and to include a control group. We employed conservative statistics accounting for repeated measures and individual variability to better assess effect size uncertainty, and when assessing the relation between behavior and rapport we employed cross-validation to maximize generalizability of the results. However, additional and possibly larger studies are necessary to properly assess the findings, as effect sizes and consequently power analyses are known to be unreliable with small samples (69). Thus, our study provides initial results on which to build future studies, but will have to be re-evaluated in the light of their findings, e.g., via a meta-analysis. Additionally, it is not possible to rule out the possibility that some of our results may be due to a confound, namely that the participants with MS become more confident and/or comfortable because they expected to benefit from the social skills workshop (i.e., rather than because of what they specifically learned and practiced in the workshop), or because they became more comfortable with the procedures as the experiment progressed. We used a different group of non-MS participants on day 2 in order to rule out the possibility that the interactions would run more smoothly simply because non-MS participants became familiar with the procedure. However, as previously argued, individual behaviors resonate in the interaction, making it difficult to clearly separate MS from non-MS participants’ behaviors. A follow-up study, including a control group, would be highly desirable.
Implications and Directions for Future Research
Practically speaking, the findings that the intervention may have increased compensatory expressivity and behavioral rapport suggest that this program could be valuable in improving social communication for people with MS. Given that the observed behavioral changes were not accompanied by changes in self-reported social competence or social anxiety, it is possible that participants were not conscious of these changes (65). With long-term intervention and with follow-up, perhaps participants’ self-perception of their social skills would improve.
More generally, the intervention may be useful for people with other conditions resulting in facial paralysis, such as Bell’s palsy, Ramsey–Hunt syndrome, stroke, and acoustic neuroma. Indeed, because people who acquired facial paralysis after birth have been found to use less compensatory expression than people with MS (30), these individuals may especially benefit from the intervention. Craniofacial conditions, such as cleft lip and palate, hemifacial microsomia, and facial burns, have long been noted to result in social difficulty. The social challenges associated with these conditions have been generally thought to derive from interlocutors’ responses to an esthetically different face. However, these conditions can distort facial features, skin, or muscles and impair the ability to produce recognizable facial expressions, which may exacerbate social interaction problems. Many other conditions result in reduced expressivity, including depression, schizophrenia, autism, and Parkinson’s disease. Although social functioning difficulty has long been noted in these conditions, there is a paucity of interventions focused on improving expression. As suggested by the current research, increased expression could improve rapport, leading to increased social comfort and belonging.
The decrease in fidgeting and increase in speech rate variability on the part of non-MS participants in our study provides support for the conjecture that some of the difficulties experienced by people with MS in social interactions may arise from other people’s discomfort or uncertainty about how to behave. In other words, people without MS who interact with people with MS may interrupt the smooth flow of interaction through their uncertainty about how to interact in what is for them a new and sensitive situation. Along these lines, social impairment might not be due simply to an impairment, but in important ways also to the context through which the disability is perceived and reacted upon in interactions.
Finally, it would be highly valuable for further research to explore the question as to whether some compensatory strategies (e.g., use of hand gestures, eye contact, and prosody) are more easily automated than others. If there are such differences among the degrees to which different strategies can be automated, this could be important for three concrete reasons. First of all, it may be taxing and distracting to employ deliberate strategies for expressing oneself in social interactions, and people may, therefore, find it tiring, and be less likely to continue doing it [cf. Ref. (19)]. Second, it may be important for some expressive, interactive processes that they occur without people’s awareness. Attempting to bring them about deliberately may actually interfere with the automatic processes that generally bring them about, and could even be counterproductive if it appears forced or unnatural. Third, it would be important for future social skills workshops to examine whether some compensatory strategies are more effectively taught indirectly, or whether some easily automated processes may trigger others. In other words, rather than telling participants to use more gestures or prosody, it may be possible to lead them to do so by some other means which does not require them to deliberately attend to their gestures or prosody, for example, by using more gestures and prosody when interacting with children with MS, by asking them to watch videos in which actors are highly expressive in their gestures and prosody, or by engaging them in role-playing games in which a high level of gesture and/or prosody is appropriate.
Conclusion
The findings reported here provide evidence that a social skills workshop for teenagers with MS can help to increase the level of rapport they achieve in social interactions. They also provide insight into the mechanisms underlying the increase in rapport. Specifically, the workshop appears not only to have increased the level of expressivity of the participants with MS but also to have led indirectly to an increase in expressivity, and to a decrease in fidgeting and other nervous behavior, in their interaction partners without MS. These results support the idea that one important way of improving social interactions for people with MS (and perhaps for individuals with facial impairment owing to other causes, or with other forms of disability) is to help them to find ways to communicate clearly and put their interaction partners at ease. In addition, the observed decrease in alignment after the intervention, and the negative relation between rapport and alignment provides constraints for theorizing about the mechanisms and functions of alignment processes in social interactions.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The study was supported by a seed-funding grant (no. 26117) from the Interacting Minds Centre at Aarhus University, as well as by The Danish Council for Independent Research – Humanities (RF and KT), the EuroCores EuroUnderstanding program – Digging for the Roots of Understanding (JM, RF, and KT) and the Interacting Minds Center (JM and RF). The development of the social skills workshop was made possible by the generous assistance of the UK-based charity organization Changing Faces. We are grateful to James Partridge, Henrietta Spalding, Jonathon Cole, Sarah Bate, Frank Bernieri, and Andreas Roepstorff for their comments and suggestions, to Shivangi Agrawal, Cassandra Colton, Mariah Estill, Andreea Barbu, Nicholas Davis, Megan McBride, Thanh Nguyen, Benjamin Riise, Lauren Sewell, and Thomas Wolf for their help preparing, transcribing, and coding the videos, and especially to Christian Kordt Højbjerg for his invaluable support at many stages. We would also like to thank the Pindstrup Centre for hosting the social skills workshop and experiment, and our participants for their time and effort.
Footnotes
- ^For a similar take on language development in infants with autism spectrum disorder, cf. Warlaumont et al. (18).
- ^These results are not simply due to a change in individual behaviors (e.g., increase in individual variability). To rule these out, we tested surrogate pairs (matching interlocutors from different conversations) before and after intervention and used their level of coordination as baseline for the real pairs. Even in this, more conservative analysis alignment decreased after intervention.
References
1. Briegel W. Neuropsychiatric findings of Möbius sequence: a review. Clin Genet (2006) 70:91–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2006.00649.x
2. Verzijl HT, Van Der Zwaag B, Cruysberg JR, Padberg GW. Möebius syndrome redefined. Neurology (2003) 61(3):327–33. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000076484.91275.CD
3. Briegel W. Psychopathology and personality aspects of adults with Moebius sequence. Clin Genet (2007) 71:376–7. doi:10.1111/j.1399-0004.2007.00787.x
4. Bogart KR, Matsumoto D. Living with Moebius syndrome: adjustment, social competence, and satisfaction with life. Cleft Palate Craniofac J (2010) 47(2):134–42. doi:10.1597/08-257.1
5. Yang L, Kleinman A, Link B, Phelan J, Lee S, Good B. Culture and stigma: adding moral experience to stigma theory. Soc Sci Med (2007) 64(7):1524–35. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.11.013
6. Goldman AI, Sripada CS. Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition (2005) 94:193–213. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.005
7. Niedenthal PM, Brauer M, Halberstadt JB, Innes-Ker AH. When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression. Cogn Emot (2001) 15:853–64. doi:10.1080/02699930143000194
8. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Dissociable neural systems for recognizing emotions. Brain Cogn (2003) 52:61–9. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00009-5
9. Calder AJ, Keane J, Cole J, Campbell R, Young AW. Facial expression recognition by people with Mobius syndrome. Cogn Neuropsychol (2000) 17:73–87. doi:10.1080/026432900380490
10. Bate S, Cook SJ, Mole J, Cole J. First report of generalized face processing difficulties in möbius sequence. PLoS One (2013) 8(4):e62656. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0062656
11. Bogart KR, Matsumoto D. Facial mimicry is not necessary to recognize emotion: facial expression recognition by people with Moebius syndrome. Soc Neurosci (2010) 5(2):241–51. doi:10.1080/17470910903395692
12. Bogart KR, Tickle-Degnen L. Looking beyond the face: a training to improve perceivers’ impressions of people with facial paralysis. Patient Educ Couns (2015) 98:251–6. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2014.09.010
13. Bogart KR, Tickle-Degnen L, Ambady N. Communicating without the face: holistic perception of emotions of people with facial paralysis. Basic Appl Soc Psych (2014) 36(4):309–20. doi:10.1080/01973533.2014.917973
14. Lyons KD, Tickle-Degnen L, Henry A, Cohn E. Impressions of personality in Parkinson’s disease: can rehabilitation practitioners see beyond the symptoms? Rehabil Psychol (2004) 49:328. doi:10.1037/0090-5550.49.4.328
15. Tickle-Degnen L, Lyons KD. Practitioners’ impressions of patients with Parkinson’s disease: the social ecology of the expressive mask. Soc Sci Med (2004) 58:603–14. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00213-2
16. Hemmesch AR, Tickle-Degnen L, Zebrowitz LA. The influence of facial masking and sex on older adults’ impressions of individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Psychol Aging (2009) 24:542. doi:10.1037/a0016105
18. Warlaumont AS, Richards JA, Gilkerson J, Oller DK. A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychol Sci (2014) 25(7):1314–24. doi:10.1177/0956797614531023
19. Cole J, Spalding H. The Invisible Smile: Living Without Facial Expression. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2009).
20. Fusaroli R, Raczaszek-Leonardi J, Tylén K. Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas Psychol (2014) 32:147–57. doi:10.1111/cogs.12251
21. Fusaroli R, Tylén K. Investigating conversational dynamics: interactive alignment, interpersonal synergy, and collective task performance. Cogn Sci (2015). doi:10.1111/cogs.12251
22. Krueger J, Michael J. Gestural coupling and social cognition: Moebius syndrome as a case study. Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:81. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00081
23. Michael J, Christensen W, Overgaard S. Mindreading as social expertise. Synthese (2014) 191:817–40. doi:10.1007/s11229-013-0295-z
24. Dale R, Fusaroli R, Duran ND, Richardson DC. The self-organization of human interaction. In: Ross BH, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Evanston: Elsevier Inc Academic Press (2014). p. 43–96.
25. Bogart KR, Tickle-Degnen L, Joffe M. Social interaction experiences of adults with Moebius syndrome: a focus group. J Health Psychol (2012) 17(8):1212–22. doi:10.1177/1359105311432491
26. Dressler RA, Butler EH, Cannito MP. Rhythmic patterns during conversational repairs in speakers with aphasia. Aphasiology (2009) 23:731–48. doi:10.1080/02687030802165582
27. Goodwin C. Building action in public environments with diverse semiotic resources. Versus (2011) 11(2–113):169–82.
28. Bogart KR. “People are all about appearances”: a focus group of teenagers with Moebius syndrome. J Health Psychol (2014). doi:10.1177/1359105313517277
29. Meyerson MD. Resiliency and success in adults with Moebius syndrome. Cleft Palate Craniofac J (2001) 38:231–5. doi:10.1597/1545-1569(2001)038<0231:RASIAW>2.0.CO;2
30. Bogart KR, Tickle-Degnen L, Ambady N. Compensatory expressive behavior for facial paralysis: adaptation to congenital or acquired disability. Rehabil Psychol (2012) 57(1):43–51. doi:10.1037/a0026904
31. Blakeney P1, Thomas C, Holzer C III, Rose M, Berniger F, Meyer WJ III. Efficacy of a short-term, intensive social skills training program for burned adolescents. J Burn Care Res (2005) 26(6):546–55. doi:10.1097/01.bcr.0000185455.81677.a2
32. Pickering MJ, Garrod S. Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behav Brain Sci (2004) 27:169–89. doi:10.1017/S0140525X04000056
33. Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower RW, Goodman JR, Schmidt RC. Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum Mov Sci (2007) 26:867–91. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002
34. Chartrand TT, Bargh JA. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol (1999) 76:893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
35. Bernieri FJ, Rosenthal R. Interpersonal coordination: behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In: Feldman RS, Rime B, editors. Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press (1991). p. 401–32.
37. Fusaroli R, Tylén K. Carving language for social coordination: a dynamical approach. Interact Stud (2012) 13(1):103–24. doi:10.1075/is.13.1.07fus
38. Fusaroli R, Konvalinka I, Wallot K. Analyzing social interactions: the promises and challenges of using cross recurrence quantification analysis, In: Marwan N, Riley M, Giuliani A, Webber CL Jr, editors. Springer Proceedings in Mathematics and Statistics. Translational Recurrences (Vol. 103). London: Springer (2014) p. 137–55.
39. Tickle-Degnen L. Nonverbal behavior and its functions in the ecosystem of rapport. In: Manusov V, Patterson ML, editors. The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc (2006). p. 381–99.
40. Lakin J, Chartrand T. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci (2003) 14:334–9. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.14481
41. Bernieri F. Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. J Nonverbal Behav (1988) 12(2):120–38. doi:10.1007/BF00986930
42. Hove MJ, Risen JL. It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc Cogn (2009) 27:949–60. doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949
43. Valdesolo P, Ouyang J, DeSteno D. The rhythm of joint action: synchrony promotes cooperative ability. J Exp Soc Psychol (2010) 46:693–5. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004
44. van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Kawakami K, van Knippenberg A. Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol Sci (2004) 15:71–4. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.x
45. Wiltermuth SS, Heath C. Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol Sci (2009) 20:1–5. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x
46. Borrie SA, Liss JM. Rhythm as a coordinating device: entrainment with disordered speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res (2014) 57(3):815–24. doi:10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S-13-0149
47. Reich CM, Berman JS, Dale R, Levitt HM. Vocal synchrony in psychotherapy. J Soc Clin Psychol (2014) 33(5):481–94. doi:10.1521/jscp.2014.33.5.481
48. Levitan R, Gravano A, Willson L, Benus S, Hirschberg J, Nenkova A. Acoustic-prosodic entrainment and social behavior. Proceedings of the 2012 Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies. Madison: Omnipress Inc (2012). p. 11–9.
49. Manson JE, Bryant GA, Gervais M, Kline M. Convergence of speech rate in conversation predicts cooperation. Evo Hum Behav (2013) 34(6):419–26. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.001
50. Helmreich R, Stapp J. Short forms of the Texas Social Behavior Inventory (TSBI), an objective measure of self-esteem. Bull Psychon Soc (1974) 4(5a):473–5. doi:10.3758/BF03334260
51. Mattick RP, Clarke JC. Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety1. Behav Res Ther (1998) 36(4):455–70. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6
52. Puccinelli N, Tickle-Degnen L, Rosenthal R. Effect of target position and target task on judge sensitivity to felt rapport. J Nonverbal Behav (2004) 28(3):211–20. doi:10.1023/B:JONB.0000039650.49960.e5
53. Boersma P, Weenink DJM. Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot Int (2001) 5(9/10):341–5.
54. de Jong NH, Wempe T. Praat script to detect syllable nuclei and measure speech rate automatically. Behav Res Methods (2009) 41(2):385–90. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.2.385
55. Wallot S, Fusaroli R, Tylén K, Jegindø EM. Using complexity metrics with RR intervals and BPM heart rate measures. Front Physiol (2013) 4:211. doi:10.3389/fphys.2013.00211
56. Gelman A, Hill J. Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2006).
57. Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol (2013) 4(2):133–42. doi:10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x
58. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Eigen and S4. R Package version 1.1-9 (2015). Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
59. Marwan N, Romano MC, Thiel M, Kurths J. Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Physics Reports (2007) 438(5):237–329. doi:10.1016/j.physrep.2006.11.001
60. Dale R, Warlaumont A, Richardson DC. Nominal cross recurrence as a generalized lag sequential analysis for behavioral streams. Int J Bifurcat Chaos (2011) 21(4):1153–61. doi:10.1142/S0218127411028970
61. Rodriguez JD, Perez A, Lozano JA. Sensitivity analysis of k-fold cross validation in prediction error estimation. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell (2010) 32(3):569–75. doi:10.1109/TPAMI.2009.187
62. Zou H, Hastie T. Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol (2005) 67(2):301–20. doi:10.1093/brain/awv075
64. Jack A, Roepstorff A, editors. Trusting the Subject?: The Use of Introspective Evidence in Cognitive Science. (Vol. 2). Exeter: Imprint Academic (2004).
65. Wilson TD. Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (2009).
66. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol (2002) 82(1):62. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62
67. Kleck R. Physical stigma and task oriented interactions. Hum Relat (1969) 22(1):53–60. doi:10.1177/001872676902200103
68. Healey P, Purver M, Howes C. Divergence in dialogue. PLoS One (2014) 9(6):e98598. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098598
Keywords: Möbius syndrome, interaction, gesture, rapport, alignment
Citation: Michael J, Bogart K, Tylén K, Krueger J, Bech M, Østergaard JR and Fusaroli R (2015) Training in compensatory strategies enhances rapport in interactions involving people with Möbius syndrome. Front. Neurol. 6:213. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00213
Received: 09 February 2015; Accepted: 18 September 2015;
Published: 08 October 2015
Edited by:
Ossama Yassin Mansour, Freiburg University Hospital, GermanyReviewed by:
Nabil Kitchener, General Organization for Teaching Hospitals and Institutes (GOTHI), EgyptJonathan Cole, Poole Hospital, UK
Copyright: © 2015 Michael, Bogart, Tylén, Krueger, Bech, Østergaard and Fusaroli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: John Michael, Department of Cognitive Science, Central European University, Oktober 6 utca 7, 1051 Budapest, Hungary, johnmichaelaarhus@gmail.com