Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 05 December 2019
Sec. Neurorehabilitation

Assessing Oromotor Capacity in ALS: The Effect of a Fixed-Target Task on Lip Biomechanics

  • 1Speech and Feeding Disorders Lab, MGH Institute of Health Professions, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Speech Kinematics and Acoustics Lab, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, United States
  • 3Speech Science and Disorders Lab, Department of Speech-Language-Hearing: Sciences and Disorders, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States
  • 4Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
  • 5Speech Production Lab, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Alternating motion rate (AMR) is a standard measure often included in neurological examinations to assess orofacial neuromuscular integrity. AMR is typically derived from recordings of patients producing repetitions of a single syllable as fast and clear as possible on one breath. Because the task places high demands on oromotor performance, particularly articulatory speed, AMRs are widely considered to be tests of maximum performance and, therefore, likely to reveal underlying neurologic deficits. Despite decades of widespread use, biomechanical studies have shown that speakers often circumvent the presumed speed challenge of the standard AMR task. Specifically, speakers are likely to manipulate their displacements (movement amplitude) instead of speed because this strategy requires less motor effort. The current study examined the effectiveness of a novel fixed-target paradigm for minimizing the truncation of articulatory excursions and maximizing motor effort. We compared the standard AMR task to that of a fixed-target AMR task and focused specifically on the tasks' potential to detect decrements in lip motor performance in persons with dysarthria due to amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Our participants were 14 healthy controls and 17 individuals with ALS. For the standard AMR task, participants were instructed to produce the syllable /bα/ as quickly and accurately as possible on one breath. For the fixed-target AMR task, participants were given the same instructions, but were also required to strike a physical target placed under the jaw during the opening phase of each syllable. Lip kinematic data were obtained using 3D electromagnetic articulography. 16 kinematic features were extracted using an algorithmic approach. Findings revealed that compared to the standard task, the fixed-target AMR task placed increased motor demands on the oromotor system by eliciting larger excursions, faster speeds, and greater spatiotemporal variability. In addition, participants with ALS exhibited limited ability to adapt to the higher articulatory demands of the fixed-target task. Between the two AMR tasks, the maximum speed during the fixed-target task showed a moderate association with the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscore. Employment of both standard and fixed-target AMR tasks is, however, needed for comprehensive assessment of oromotor function and for elucidating profiles of task adaptation.

Introduction

Oral alternating motion rate (AMR)—elicited during fast repetition of a syllable on a single breath—is a diadochokinetic (DDK) rate measure that, according to Duffy (1), has “the primary value of assessing the speed and regularity of rapid, repetitive articulatory movement” (p. 81). AMR tasks have been used in both research and clinical settings to evaluate neuromuscular integrity by placing high performance demands on the speech subsystems. Impairments in the articulatory, respiratory, phonatory, and resonatory subsystems can result in abnormal AMR task performance, including, but not limited to a shortened task duration, imprecise consonant production, and variable rate of syllable repetition (1).

The AMR task has several well-recognized strengths as an assessment tool of neuromuscular integrity. First, it is time and cost-efficient, non-invasive, and simple to administer. Second, because the task requires only the repetition of short syllables, it can be administered to individuals within a wide age range (26), linguistic backgrounds, speech severities (7), and cognitive and linguistic impairments (8, 9). Third, widely-published normative data for AMR tasks across the life-span provide a rich empirical basis for identifying impaired oromotor function (10). Finally, AMR tasks are highly responsive to neuromotor impairments and have previously been used to study neurological disorders such as amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) (11, 12), multiple sclerosis (13), Parkinson's disease (1416), and traumatic brain injury (17).

Speech-motor planning, coordination, and control have typically been indexed using acoustic features derived from AMR tasks including the number, duration, and rate of syllable repetitions, intersyllabic gap durations, energy maxima, and voice onset time (1719). More recently, Rong et al. (12) developed an automated analysis for comprehensively profiling lip and jaw motor performance directly using speech kinematics during AMR tasks. The fine-grained kinematic analyses of AMR tasks have great potential to detect early bulbar function decline in ALS (12, 20).

Despite the widespread use of AMR tasks for early detection and progress monitoring of bulbar signs in ALS, like most measures of maximum performance, AMRs are subject to high inter- and intrasubject variability (10). To maximize the diagnostic efficacy of the assessment, research is needed to understand and subsequently minimize sources of variability related to test administration procedures and participant factors. Although not well-understood, performance on an AMR task can be influenced by a number of factors including familiarity, pre-test practice, instructions, motivation, feedback, age, and sex (2, 3, 10, 21). Perhaps equally concerning is the questionable validity of the task as a maximum performance test. Specifically, contrary to expectations, the task does not elicit maximum movement speeds; rather, many speakers adapt by truncating their articulatory excursions to achieve a fast syllable repetition rate (2224). The caveat is that although articulatory truncation is an adaptive strategy to economize effort under fast speaking rate conditions, it may mask underlying impairments when used as a compensatory strategy in persons with motor speech disorders.

To minimize opportunities for articulatory truncation, Mefferd et al. (25) developed a fixed-target AMR task to investigate orofacial motor speed capacities in healthy controls and in persons with ALS. Similar approaches have been used to investigate limb motor function in which the speed of reaching movement was derived under a controlled condition by implementing a fixed target (26, 27). The fixed-target AMR task developed by Meffed et al. (25) required talkers to strike a target with the lower jaw during the opening cycle of each syllable. When compared to a standard AMR task, the fixed-target task elicited larger jaw movements, faster jaw speeds, and a more consistent extent of jaw displacement across repetitions in both healthy controls and in talkers with ALS (25). These findings suggested that compared to the standard AMR task, a fixed-target AMR task imposed greater control demands on the oromotor system. Although the study conducted by Mefferd et al. (25) showed that a fixed-target AMR task induced differences between the early-stage bulbar ALS group and healthy controls, only the speed of movement was studied. Therefore, it remains to be determined if kinematic features that can index other aspects of motor performance (e.g., fine motor tuning to spatial, temporal, and spatiotemporal aspects of articulatory biomechanics), are affected by this variant of the AMR task. In addition, fixed-target tasks are not only physically challenging, but also demand greater spatial precision of the lower lip and jaw, which putatively engages extramotor processes such as attention and somatosensation. These additional extramotor processes may engage different neural circuits that otherwise would be absent during habitual speech production. More research is warranted to investigate the putative role of these extramotor processes on speech motor performance.

In this study, we tested the hypothesis that the reduced oromotor capacity due to neurologic disease would negatively impact the overall motor performance during a fixed-target AMR task. In addition, we hypothesized that a fixed-target AMR task would probe both the temporal and spatial aspects of articulatory performance. Our interest was to develop methodologies that can be used to index limitations in oromotor capacity to facilitate improved understanding of the neuromotor basis of speech and swallowing deficits. The aims of the current study were to (1) determine the effect of the fixed-target task on lip kinematics (between-task comparisons) in healthy controls and persons with ALS (between-group comparisons); (2) identify task-related kinematic features that differentiate dysarthric speech due to ALS from the speech of healthy controls; and (3) provide a preliminary evaluation of the clinical validity of the AMR tasks by examining the strength of association between the kinematic measure of “maximum velocity” extracted from the lip and the bulbar subscores on the ALS Functional Rating Scale-Revised (ALSFRS-R). The kinematic measure of “maximum velocity” was of particular interest because the intended purpose of the fixed-target AMR task is to test the velocity generating capacity of the lip by minimizing opportunities for articulatory truncation.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants included 14 healthy controls (8 males and 6 females, mean age = 62.23, SD = 9.04) and 17 patients with ALS (8 males and 9 females, mean age = 53.65, SD = 7.08). Healthy controls reported no history of speech, language, hearing, or neurological disorders. All participants were native speakers of American English, passed a hearing screening, and had cognition adequate to follow the task instructions. Of 17 participants with ALS, the ALS Cognitive Behavioral Screen (ALS CBS (28)) scores were available for 10. The test yields a total cognitive score ranging from 0 to 20. The mean ALS CBS score for these participants was 15.7 (SD = 2.3). A score of 16 has been identified as the optimal cognitive cut-off score to differentiate intact cognition from potential cognitive impairment (28), 6 participants demonstrated cognitive scores above this threshold (mean = 17.33, SD = 0.52, ranging from 17 to 18), and four participants demonstrated cognitive scores below that (mean = 13.25, SD = 1.5, ranging from 12 to 15). Participants with ALS had been diagnosed with ALS by a neurologist based on the El Escorial criteria (29) and were required to have no history of speech, language, hearing, or other neurological disorders. Participants with ALS varied in the site of onset (11 with spinal onset, 4 with bulbar onset, and 2 unknown). Of 17 participants with ALS, the ALSFRS-R (30) scores were available for 13 participants. The mean ALSFRS-R total score was 30.23 (SD = 7.21) and the mean bulbar sub-score was 8.85 (SD = 1.63). Table 1 displays the scores of individual items on the ALSFRS-R self-rated by the participants with ALS.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. ALSFRS-R scores of individual items in patients with ALS.

Participants with ALS were identified to have bulbar symptoms because they exhibited a habitual speaking rate of slower than 150 words per min (w/m) following Rong et al. (31). Speaking rate was used as a criterion to identify these participants because previous research has demonstrated that the decline of speaking rate precedes, and has a faster rate of decline, than speech intelligibility during the early stage of ALS (32, 33). The mean speaking rate for the participants with ALS was 114.07 w/m (ranged from 54.61 to 146.02 w/m, SD = 28.07) and for healthy controls was 188.20 w/m (ranged from 155.76 to 216.26 w/m, SD = 18.22). Mean speech intelligibility scores for participants with ALS was 88.50% (ranged from 23.64 to 100%, SD = 22.23) and for healthy controls was 99.94% (ranged from 99.09 to 100%, SD = 0.24). Speaking rate and speech intelligibility were measured using the procedures described in the Speech Intelligibility Test (SIT) manual (34). This study was approved by the Partners Healthcare Institutional Review Board (IRB) and all participants provided written, informed consent to participate in the study.

Procedures

For the standard AMR task, participants were asked to take a deep breath and to produce the syllable /bα/ as quickly and accurately as possible until they ran out of breath. For the fixed-target AMR task, participants were given the same instructions but additionally, the experimenter held a target under the chin of the participant. The target was a stick with a blunt plastic tip. The distance between the target and the lower jaw was determined by asking participants to produce the syllable /bα/ and hold their mouths open for a few seconds. The target was positioned so that it touched the underside of the chin during maximum jaw opening, which occurs during the vowel. To stabilize the target position, the experimenters stood behind the participants with their left hand holding the target in position while stabilizing it against the participants' sternum. The experimenters' right hand was then used to stabilize the participants' head for the purpose of minimizing forward head rotation. Participants were instructed to hit the stick with each production. If participants failed to hit the target, they were encouraged to continue and to attempt to hit the target on the next production. Participants practiced the task before recording began.

Kinematic Recordings

Movements of the articulators during the AMR tasks were captured using a 3D electromagnetic articulograph (Wave; Northern Digital, Inc.). Following procedures described by Rong et al. (12), the 3D Euclidean distance between sensors attached to the midline of the upper and lower lip was derived during each of the AMR tasks, and subsequently low-passed filtered at 15 Hz. These movement signals were used in the following data analyses.

Extraction of Kinematic Features

A semi-automatic MATLAB algorithm was used to quantify features of lip performance from the 3D lip movement time-series. This algorithm derives features that probe various aspects of oromotor performance including spatial, temporal, and spatiotemporal characteristics of articulatory movement as well as the overall motor performance (12). The algorithm automatically segmented the opening and closing phases of the lip distance traces and identified cycles with large spatial or temporal deviations from the average pattern across cycles for manual inclusion or exclusion of the deviant cycles (12). Based on the lip movement traces, the algorithm extracted 22 lip movement kinematic features during the AMR tasks. Of the 22 output kinematic features, only 16 features were selected to study because they were considered to be clinically interpretable. All 16 features were included in our analysis because (1) there was no a priori basis for excluding variables and (2) prior research will benefit from identifying the best subset of features for staging bulbar motor involvement.

Table 2 displays the features of interest used in this study. Spatial, temporal, and spatiotemporal features as well as the features of overall motor performance are consistently color-coded in blue, orange, green, and pink, respectively in Table 2 and further illustrations hereafter.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Kinematic features extracted from the upper and lower lip movement during AMR tasks.

Figures 1, 2 display examples of MATLAB plots for Slp_1, Slp_2, and Sti features extracted from the /bα/ syllables produced by a healthy control participant and a participant with ALS during the standard AMR task. Because the rest of features were mainly mathematic notations, corresponding plots of other features were not produced.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Kinematic features of Slp_1, Slp_2, and individual cycles (from which the Sti feature was calculated) extracted from lip movements of one healthy control participant during a standard AMR task.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Kinematic features of Slp_1, Slp_2, and individual cycles (from which the Sti feature was calculated) extracted from lip movements of one participant with ALS during a standard AMR task.

Statistical Analysis

Data were statistically analyzed using R version i386 3.5.3 (35) to examine task and group differences and p-values were compared against the 0.05 significance level selected a priori. Paired sample t-tests were used to examine the within-group task related differences and independent samples t-tests were used to compare the motor performance of groups across standard and fixed-target AMR tasks. Additionally, linear regression analyses were performed to examine the numerical association between the Max_vel in both types of AMR tasks (i.e., standard and fixed-target tasks) and the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscores.

Results

Within-Group Comparison of Standard and Fixed-Target AMR Tasks

Healthy controls showed statistically significant differences between the standard and fixed-target AMR tasks for the majority of kinematic features of lip movement (spatial, temporal, spatiotemporal, and overall performance) (p < 0.05). Except for the kinematic features of F and Ncyc, which were greater in the standard AMR task, the change in the rest of kinematic features were directed toward the fixed-target AMR task. These findings verified that the fixed-target AMR task elicited larger kinematic changes than did the standard AMR task (e.g., increased lip opening and maximum velocity in the fixed-target task as compared to the standard task). Compared to the healthy controls, participants with ALS had a smaller number of features that were statistically significantly different between the standard and fixed-target AMR task (p < 0.05). Similar to healthy controls, the between-task changes of F and Ncyc were directed toward the standard AMR tasks in the ALS group. In addition to F and Ncyc, standard the AMR task elicited larger changes in the kinematic feature of Slp_1 in patients with ALS.

Figures 3, 4 display the within-group comparison of standard and fixed-target AMR tasks in healthy controls and participants with ALS, respectively. In each table, the effect sizes are represented by dots at the middle of each horizontal line and each line represents the 95% confidence interval (CI) around the corresponding effect size. The red lines indicate statistically significant comparisons between kinematic measures derived from standard and fixed-target AMR tasks (p < 0.05). Horizontal lines deviated to the left indicate the directionality of change toward the fixed-target AMR task and those deviated to the right show the directionality of change toward the standard AMR task.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Kinematic features extracted from lip movement during standard (S) and fixed-target (FT) AMR tasks in the healthy control (HC) group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Kinematic features extracted from lip movement during standard (S) and fixed-target (FT) AMR tasks in the ALS group.

Between-Group Comparison of Standard and Fixed-Target AMR Tasks

In the standard AMR task, significant group differences were observed for temporal kinematic features (Tsd, Jitter, and F) and for features of overall motor performance (Dur, Ncyc) (p < 0.05). The fixed-target AMR task revealed significant between-group differences for spatial kinematic features (Slp_1, Sse2, Scanning d2), spatiotemporal kinematic features (Max_vel), temporal kinematic features (Jitter, F), and features of overall motor performance (Dur, Ncyc) (p < 0.05). Figures 5, 6 represent results of between-group comparisons in the standard and fixed-target AMR tasks, respectively. In each table, the effect sizes are represented by dots at the middle of each horizontal line and each line represents the 95% confidence interval (CI) around the corresponding effect size. The red lines indicate statistically significant comparisons between healthy controls and participants with ALS (p < 0.05). Horizontal lines deviated to the left indicate the directionality of change toward the ALS group and those deviated to the right show the directionality of change toward healthy controls.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Comparison of kinematic features between the healthy control (HC) and ALS groups obtained during the standard AMR task.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Comparison of kinematic features between the healthy control (HC) and ALS groups obtained during the fixed-target AMR task.

In comparison to the standard AMR task, the fixed-target AMR task yielded higher inter-subject variability in healthy controls. Figures 7, 8 illustrate inter-subject variability within each kinematic feature that induced significant differences between the two groups in the standard and fixed-target AMR tasks, respectively.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Significant kinematic features that differentiate between the healthy control (HC) and ALS groups in the standard AMR task.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Significant kinematic features that differentiate between the healthy control (HC) and ALS groups in the fixed-target AMR task.

Verification of the Max_vel Kinematic Feature Based on the ALSFRS-R Bulbar Subscores

The kinematic feature of Max_vel extracted from the lip movement during the fixed-target AMR task was able to statistically significantly predict the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscores (R2 = 0.60, p = 0.002) (Figure 9). No statistically significant association was observed between the measure of Max_vel extracted from the lip during the standard AMR task and the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscores (R2 = 0.25, p = 0.25).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Numerical association between the Max_vel kinematic feature extracted from the lip movement during the AMR tasks (standard and fixed-target) and the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscores.

Discussion

In this methodology study, we compared two types of AMR task (i.e., standard and fixed-target) in their ability to test maximum motor performance in healthy individuals and patients with ALS who demonstrated a wide range of severity in terms of intelligibility, speaking rate, and ALSFRS-R total scores and bulbar subscores. We used a semi-automated algorithm to extract kinematic features that probe spatial, temporal, and spatiotemporal properties of lip articulatory movement as well as the overall motor performance. Four main findings of the study are summarized below:

The Fixed-Target AMR Significantly Increased Motor Demands

Results of this study revealed that, in general, the fixed-target AMR task imposed greater articulatory demands on the motor system relative to the standard AMR task. The fixed-target task was effective in minimizing the truncation of articulatory displacement by eliciting larger articulatory displacements.

In healthy controls, the fixed-target AMR task elicited larger articulatory excursions (Max-open), faster velocities (Max-vel), greater variability in cycle durations (Tsd), larger absolute difference in duration of consecutive cycles (jitter), and increases in indices of spatiotemporal variability (i.e., Sti, D_dtw). Similarly, in the ALS group, spatial and spatiotemporal features indexed by maximum lip opening and closing gestures (Max-open, Max-close), spatiotemporal variability (Sti), and maximum velocity (Max-vel) were significantly different between the two tasks, likely resulting from larger displacements and greater variability in cycle production induced by the fixed-target task. The elicitation of fast movement speeds while maintaining consistent articulatory displacements suggests that the fixed-target AMR task is a more valid test of maximum performance relevant to the standard AMR task. These findings are consistent with those reported by Mefferd et al. (25) in which movement speed was induced by implementing a metronome-paced fixed-target AMR task in order to drive jaw displacement.

As expected, the number of cycles (Ncyc) and frequency of syllable repetitions (F) significantly decreased in the fixed-target AMR task compared to the standard AMR task in both groups. To complete each syllable cycle, the lower jaw traveled a longer distance to reach the target than it did in the standard AMR task, and subsequently, the total number of syllables decreased. Additionally, the increased motor demands of the fixed-target AMR task may have induced fatigue. The steeper negative slope of lip opening (Slp-1) observed in the ALS group is consistent with the presence of fatigue because it suggests a progressive truncation of movement extent across syllable repetitions.

Reduced Task-Adaptation Is a Prominent Characteristic of Bulbar Motor Involvement in ALS

Between-task comparisons (standard vs. fixed-target AMR) in healthy controls and participants with ALS revealed that speakers with impaired motor systems have a limited capacity to accommodate the higher articulatory demands imposed on the neuromotor system during the fixed-target AMR task. Healthy controls, however, uniformly adjusted articulatory control to accommodate each task, as evidenced by the large effect sizes presented in Figure 3. In contrast to the healthy controls, fewer of the kinematic features differed between tasks in the ALS group. These findings are consistent with our prior work in which individuals at the early stage of ALS (bulbar asymptomatic), who had speaking rate and speech intelligibility within normal limits, exhibited reduced patterns of task adaptation during a fixed-target AMR task (20). Additional research is warranted to determine if the inability to adapt to the demands of a fixed-target AMR task is an early indicator of bulbar motor involvement due to ALS.

Fixed-Target AMR Tasks Comprehensively Probed Multiple Aspects of Speech Motor Performance

The findings of the current study suggested that the kinematic features of jitter, F, Ncyc, and duration are robust indices of motor performance that can differentiate between the two groups of participants regardless of the type of the AMR task. In the standard AMR task, participants with ALS were significantly different from healthy controls in temporal features (Tsd, Jitter, and F), as well as in features of overall motor performance (Dur, Ncyc). Fixed-target AMR tasks, however, induced significant differences between the two groups in a wider range of kinematic features that included spatial (Slp-1, Sse2, Scanning d2) and spatiotemporal features (Max-vel) along with temporal features (Jitter, F) and features of overall motor performance (Dur, Ncyc). Therefore, while the standard AMR task elicited changes primarily in temporal features, the fixed-target AMR task elicited changes in all aspects of motor performance—primarily in spatial and spatiotemporal features and secondarily in temporal features. Additional work is needed to test the clinical efficacy of the task for early detection, indexing disease severity, and monitoring disease progression.

Maximum Velocity of Lip Movement During the Fixed-Target AMR Task Was Associated With the ALSFRS-R Bulbar Subscores

The maximum velocity (Max_vel) was associated with the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscore. The observed coefficient of determination suggested a moderate goodness-of-fit relevant to the regression line (R2 = 0.6), which is acceptable particularly given that ALSFRS-R bulbar subscores are calculated based on patients' self-reports. This finding provides additional support for (1) validity the fixed-target task in eliciting the velocity of movement by preventing articulatory truncation under fast speaking rate condition, and (2) the clinical utility of this measure for staging bulbar motor involvement.

Limitations of the Study

Several limitations of this study need to be acknowledged. First, because of the small sample size in both groups, conducting Bonferroni corrections to control the familywise error rate could have increased the occurrence of type II error; interpretation of effect sizes of the corresponding comparisons was, therefore, used to support our inferences. Second, because the data used in this study was obtained as part of the protocol for a larger project, the standard AMR task consistently preceded the fixed-target AMR task. Inability to randomize the order of the tasks could have introduced potential confounds. Third, due to the small sample size, sub-analyses considering differential subsystem involvement on speech performance of patients with ALS were not performed. Additionally, the neuropsychological profile of the patients with ALS was unknown as cognitive data were not available for all patients with ALS. More studies with larger sample sizes, controlled jaw-to-target distance, and known neuropsychological and subsystem (e.g., respiratory) profiles of patients with ALS are warranted to substantiate the findings of this study.

Conclusions

The current study provided empirical evidence in support of the effectiveness of fixed-target AMR tasks in comparison to standard AMR tasks to prevent speakers from truncation of articulatory displacement rather than speeding articulatory movements under fast speaking conditions. Findings demonstrated that although the fixed-target AMR task elicited larger articulatory displacements and larger temporal and spatiotemporal variabilities in healthy controls, participants with ALS exhibited reduced capability to make the required articulatory adjustments due to underlying neurologic deficits. Reduced task adaptation was considered as an indicator of bulbar motor involvement that could be used clinically for monitoring neuromotor performance across disease progression. Additionally, while the standard AMR task predominantly relied on temporal variabilities to differentiate between healthy controls and participants with ALS, the fixed-target AMR task was able to discriminate between the two groups using spatial and spatiotemporal kinematic features in addition to the temporal features. These findings suggest that fixed-target AMR tasks can facilitate a multi-faceted evaluation of motor capacity by challenging the neuromotor system in both spatial and temporal domains of speech motor performance. For a comprehensive neuromotor evaluation, however, the combination of these two AMR tasks is preferable as each predominately probes different aspects of motor performance. Finally, the lip maximum velocity during the fixed-target AMR task showed acceptable association with the ALSFRS-R bulbar subscores.

Findings from this study have several clinical implications. First, results can help clinicians better understand the shortcomings of the traditional AMR task and may be encouraged to use the additional fixed-target AMR task for the assessment of neuromuscular integrity of the orofacial system in patients with ALS. Given that the administration of the fixed-target AMR task does not require sophisticated instrumentation or a high-tech laboratory setting, patients can be easily instructed to perform the task during a clinical evaluation. The algorithm to extract the kinematic data from the lip movement can, in the future, be embedded into a software application on a laptop computer, mobile, or tablet, where simultaneous recording of the audio and video signals is possible. Acoustic and kinematic data obtained from the input signals can facilitate clinical decision making and allow clinicians to better monitor disease progression.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by PARTNER's health care IRB. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

ME had major role in study conceptualization and design, data analyses, interpretation of the findings, and writing the manuscript. KS contributed to the data collection and interpretation of the findings, reviewed the manuscript, and provided the feedback. AM contributed to the interpretation of the findings, reviewed the manuscript, and provided the feedback. PR contributed to the analysis tool and interpretation of the findings, reviewed the manuscript, and provided the feedback. JB reviewed the manuscript and provided the feedback. YY contributed to the interpretation of the findings, reviewed the manuscript, and provided the feedback. JG had major role in the study conceptualization and design, interpretation of the findings, and manuscript preparation.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH-NIDCD) under award numbers R01DC009890, R01DC0135470, and K24DC016312.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The views and content expressed herein are solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Health.

References

1. Duffy JR. Motor Speech Disorders: Substrates, Differential Diagnosis, and Management. 3rd ed. St. Louis, MO: Elsevier Mosby (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Ben-David BM, Icht M. The effect of practice and visual feedback on oral-diadochokinetic rates for younger and older adults. Lang Speech. (2018) 61:113–34. doi: 10.1177/0023830917708808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Icht M, Ben-David BM. Oral-diadochokinetic rates for Hebrew-speaking school-age children: real words vs. non-words repetition. Clin Linguist Phon. (2015) 29:102–14. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2014.961650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Modolo DJ, Berretin-Felix G, Genaro KF, Brasolotto AG. Oral and vocal fold diadochokinesis in children. Folia Phoniatr Logop. (2011) 63:1–8. doi: 10.1159/000319728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pierce JE, Cotton S, Perry A. Alternating and sequential motion rates in older adults. Int J Lang Commun Disord. (2013) 48:257–64. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Williams P, Stackhouse J. Rate, accuracy and consistency: diadochokinetic performance of young, normally developing children. Clin Linguist Phon. (2000) 14:267–93. doi: 10.1080/02699200050023985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Shriberg LD, Lohmeier HL, Campbell TF, Dollaghan CA, Green JR, Moore CA. A nonword repetition task for speakers with misarticulations: the syllable repetition task (SRT). J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2009) 52:1189–212. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0047)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Duffy JR, Peach RK, Strand EA. Progressive apraxia of speech as a sign of motor neuron disease. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (2007) 16:198–208. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/025)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Duffy JR, Strand EA, Clark H, Machulda M, Whitwell JL, Josephs KA. Primary progressive apraxia of speech: clinical features and acoustic and neurologic correlates. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (2015) 24:88–100. doi: 10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kent RD, Kent JF, Rosenbek JC. Maximum performance tests of speech production. J Speech Hear Disord. (1987) 52:367–87.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

11. Allison K, Yunusova Y, Campbell T, Wang J, Berry J, Green JR. The diagnostic utility of patient-report and speech-language pathologists' ratings for detecting the early onset of bulbar symptoms due to ALS. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Frontotemporal Degener. (2017) 18:358–66. doi: 10.1080/21678421.2017.1303515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rong P, Yunusova Y, Richburg B, Green JR. Automatic extraction of abnormal lip movement features from the alternating motion rate task in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. (2018) 20:610–23. doi: 10.1080/17549507.2018.1485739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tjaden K, Watling E. Characteristics of diadochokinesis in multiple sclerosis and Parkinson's disease. Folia Phoniatr Logop. (2003) 55:241–59. doi: 10.1159/000072155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ackermann H, Konczak J, Hertrich I. The temporal control of repetitive articulatory movements in Parkinson's disease. Brain Lang. (1997) 56:312–9. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Midi I, Dogan M, Koseoglu M, Can G, Sehitoglu MA, Gunal DI. Voice abnormalities and their relation with motor dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Scand. (2008) 117:26–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2007.00965.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Rosen KM, Kent RD, Duffy JR. Task-based profile of vocal intensity decline in parkinson's disease. Folia Phoniatr Logop. (2005) 57:28–37. doi: 10.1159/000081959

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wang YT, Kent RD, Duffy JR, Thomas JE, Weismer G. Alternating motion rate as an index of speech motor disorder in traumatic brain injury. Clin Linguist Phon. (2004) 18:57–84. doi: 10.1080/02699200310001596160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Amerman JC, Parnell MM. Oral motor precision in older adults. J Natl Student Speech Lang Hear Assoc. (1982) 10:55–66.

Google Scholar

19. Shipley K, McAfee J. Assessment in Speech Language Pathology: A Resource Manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group (1992.

Google Scholar

20. Eshghi M, Rong P, Mefferd AS, Stipancic KL, Yunusova Y, Green JR. Reduced task adaptation in alternating motion rate tasks as an early marker of bulbar involvement in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. In: Proceedings of Interspeech (2019). p. 4524–8.

Google Scholar

21. Ben-David BM, Icht M. Oral-diadochokinetic rates for Hebrew-speaking healthy ageing population: non-word versus real-word repetition. Int J Lang Commun Disord. (2017) 52:301–10. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Kuehn DP, Moll KL. A cineradiographic study of VC and CV articulatory velocities. J Phon. (1976) 4:303–20. doi: 10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31257-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Mefferd AS, Green JR. Articulatory-to-acoustic relations in response to speaking rate and loudness manipulations. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2010) 53:1206–19. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0083)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Westbury JR, Dembowski J. Articulatory kinematics of normal diadochokinetic performance. Annu Bull Res Inst Logop Phoniatr. (1993) 27:13–36.

Google Scholar

25. Mefferd AS, Green JR, Pattee G. A novel fixed-target task to determine articulatory speed contraints in persons with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Commun Disord. (2012) 45:35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kelly VE, Bastian AJ. Antiparkinson medications improve agonist activation but not antagonist inhibition during sequential reaching movements. Mov Disord. (2005) 20:694–704. doi: 10.1002/mds.20386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Rand MK, Squire LM, Stelmach GE. Effect of speed manipulation on the control of aperture closure during reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res. (2006) 174:74–85. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0423-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Woolley SC, York MK, Moore DH, Strutt AM, Murphy J, Schulz PE, et al. Detecting frontotemporal dysfunction in ALS: utility of the ALS cognitive behavioral screen (ALS-CBS). Amyotroph Lateral Scler. (2010) 11:303–11. doi: 10.3109/17482961003727954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Brooks BR, Miller RG, Swash M, Munsat TL. El Escorial revisited: Revised criteria for the diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Other Motor Neuron Disord. (2000) 1:293–9. doi: 10.1080/146608200300079536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cedarbaum JM, Stambler N, Malta E, Fuller C, Hilt D, Thurmond B, et al. The ALSFRS-R: a revised ALS functional rating scale that incorporates assessments of respiratory function. J Neurol Sci. (1999) 169:13–21. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00210-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Rong P, Yunusova Y, Green JR. Speech intelligibility decline in individuals with fast and slow rates of ALS progression. In: Proceedings of Interspeech (2015). p. 2967–71.

Google Scholar

32. Green JR, Yunusova Y, Kuruvilla MS, Wang J, Pattee GL, Synhorst L, et al. Bulbar and speech motor assessment in ALS: challenges and future directions. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Frontotemporal Degener. (2013) 14:494–500. doi: 10.3109/21678421.2013.817585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Rong P, Yunusova Y, Wang J, Green JR. Predicting early bulbar decline in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a speech subsystem Approach. Behav Neurol. (2015) 2015:183027. doi: 10.1155/2015/183027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Yorkston K, Beukelman D, Hakel M. Speech Intelligibility Test (SIT) for Windows. Lincoln, NJ: Madonna Rehabilitation Hospital (2007).

Google Scholar

35. R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2013).

Google Scholar

Keywords: alternating motion rate, fixed-target tasks, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, speech kinematics, maximum performance

Citation: Eshghi M, Stipancic KL, Mefferd A, Rong P, Berry JD, Yunusova Y and Green JR (2019) Assessing Oromotor Capacity in ALS: The Effect of a Fixed-Target Task on Lip Biomechanics. Front. Neurol. 10:1288. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01288

Received: 10 July 2019; Accepted: 21 November 2019;
Published: 05 December 2019.

Edited by:

Emilia Michou, University of Manchester, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Irene Battel, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Valentina Pasian, Azienda Sanitaria Locale “Città di Torino”, Italy
Jeannette D. Hoit, University of Arizona, United States

Copyright © 2019 Eshghi, Stipancic, Mefferd, Rong, Berry, Yunusova and Green. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jordan R. Green, jgreen2@mghihp.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.