Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Neurol., 16 October 2025

Sec. Neurorehabilitation

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2025.1601454

Twenty minutes of Corsi block tapping task training does not improve mental rotation in adults with stroke

  • 1Division of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Physical Therapy, Crean College of Health and Behavioral Science, Chapman University, Irvine, CA, United States
  • 3School of Biological and Health Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, United States
  • 4Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Background: Visuospatial function is commonly impaired post-stroke and is associated with motor learning and recovery. A single, twenty-minute Corsi Block Tapping Task (CBTT) training session improved visuospatial function in young neurotypical adults; however, it is unclear whether this training would improve visuospatial function in adults with stroke.

Objective: To understand if a single, twenty-minute CBTT training session improved visuospatial function in adults with stroke compared to a no-training control group of adults with stroke.

Methods: Participants post-stroke were assigned to one of two groups. The training group completed twenty minutes of computerized CBTT training. The control group completed a survey and watched a video for twenty minutes. Both groups completed a mental rotation task to assess visuospatial function pre- and post-training. To understand if training impacted mental rotation reaction time, we fit a robust mixed effects model with fixed effects for time, group, and time by group interaction. We also investigated whether lesion side impacted CBTT performance using a robust mixed effects model with fixed effects for log(time), lesion side, and log(time) by lesion side interaction.

Results: Nineteen participants post-stroke were included. Neither the control nor training group improved mental rotation reaction time (time p = 0.61, group p = 0.65; interaction p = 0.52). We also found no effect of lesion side on CBTT performance [log(time) p = 0.001, lesion side p = 0.49, interaction p = 0.89].

Discussion: Twenty minutes of CBTT training did not improve post-stroke mental rotation. Longer training bouts or a different type of visuospatial training may be necessary to improve visuospatial function in adults with stroke.

1 Introduction

Visuospatial function is commonly impaired in adults with stroke (1, 2) and is associated with lower quality of life (35), reduced participation (6), and difficulty completing activities of daily living (5, 6). Visuospatial function broadly reflects someone’s ability to perceive the spatial properties of a two- or three-dimensional figure or object (7). However, different components of visuospatial function can be measured separately including visuospatial/constructional skills and visuospatial working memory. Visuospatial/constructional skills represent someone’s ability to perceive a visual image, break it down into parts, and reconstruct the image (8). Visuospatial/constructional skills are commonly measured using the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) (9) and the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (10). Visuospatial working memory represents the ability to store and manipulate visual information (11). It is commonly measured using the Corsi Block Tapping Task (CBTT) (12) and the Spatial Addition from the Wechsler Memory Scale-IV (13).

Evidence suggests that both visuospatial/constructional skills and visuospatial working memory may be related to motor learning in neurotypical adults (1421). Visuospatial/constructional skills are related to one-week retention of an upper extremity task in neurotypical older adults (15). Visuospatial working memory is associated with skill acquisition (17), one-week retention (17), and one-month retention (14, 19) of a functional upper extremity task in neurotypical older adults. Visuospatial working memory is also related to sensorimotor adaptation and motor sequence learning (18).

Motor learning is the foundation of many post-stroke rehabilitation interventions (22). Outcomes from post-stroke rehabilitation studies are often variable between individuals (23, 24). Variability in post-stroke motor learning likely arises from multiple factors, one possibly being cognitive impairment (25). Specifically, there is some evidence that visuospatial function may impact motor learning in individuals post-stroke (14, 26). Visuospatial/constructional skills are related to performance on a gait biofeedback task in adults post-stroke (26) and visuospatial working memory predicts one-month retention of a functional upper extremity task in adults with stroke (14). Additionally, visuospatial/constructional skills have been linked to long-term functional rehabilitation outcomes post-stroke (27, 28). Combined, these results suggest a link between visuospatial function, motor learning, and functional rehabilitation outcomes; thus, improving visuospatial function with targeted interventions may have downstream effects on motor learning during rehabilitation after stroke.

One potential way to improve visuospatial function after stroke may be through computerized visuospatial training paradigms. Recent work demonstrated that a single, twenty-minute visuospatial training session (using a computerized version of the CBTT) is sufficient to improve mental rotation abilities in neurotypical young adults (29). This suggests that improving visuospatial function is possible with a short training bout. However, whether twenty minutes of visuospatial training is adequate to improve visuospatial function post-stroke is unclear.

Here, we aimed to understand if a single, twenty-minute CBTT training session improved mental rotation performance (measured using reaction time) in adults with stroke compared to a no-training control group of adults with stroke. Given that mental rotation reaction time improved in neurotypical young adults after twenty minutes of CBTT training (29), we hypothesized that a single, twenty-minute CBTT training session would improve mental rotation performance (i.e., reduce reaction time) compared to a no-training group of adults with stroke.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

Twenty-two individuals at least six months post-stroke were recruited. Participants were recruited from an established, IRB-approved database of people living with stroke who have an interest in participating in research, as well as through local outpatient therapy clinics by our network of clinical partners. Inclusion criteria for participation included age eighteen to eighty, paresis confined to one side, and no orthopedic or pain conditions in the hands. Exclusion criteria included damage to the pons, basal ganglia, or cerebellum, signs of cerebellar involvement or extrapyramidal symptoms, hemispatial neglect, and a Montreal Cognitive Assessment five-minute protocol score of less than nineteen (26, 30). Written informed consent was provided before participation. The University of Southern California Institutional Review Board approved the study procedures.

2.2 Assessment of cognitive status

Measures of immediate memory, visuospatial/constructional skills, language, attention, and delayed memory were assessed with the RBANS (9). The RBANS has been found to be an appropriate test to detect domain-specific cognitive impairment post-stroke (31). Data for all participants were age-normalized using the RBANS scoring manual. For all domains, higher scores indicate better cognitive function. All RBANS were independently double-scored to identify and resolve any discrepancies in scoring.

2.3 Mental rotation task

We used the computer-based Mental Rotation Task from the open-source Psychology Experiment Building Language (32) for our primary visuospatial function measure. The protocol for the mental rotation task is described in detail in previous work (29). Briefly, the mental rotation task presents participants with a pair of 2D asymmetrical objects rotated with respect to one another. The participants were asked to identify whether the two objects were identical as quickly as possible using the right and left arrows on the keyboard. The original protocol used the left and right shift keys, but the right and left arrows made the task unimanual and more attainable for participants post-stroke. Each participant completed 64 trials each session with four additional practice trials (none included in the analyses). The participant had 3,000 ms to provide an answer. Feedback (correct or incorrect) was presented 500 ms after each response. The trial was marked incorrect if the participant did not answer within 3,000 ms. The outcome measures were reaction time for correctly completed trials and number of correctly completed trials. After completing the initial mental rotation task, participants were randomly assigned to the training or control group. After twenty minutes, both groups repeated the mental rotation task (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Flowchart depicting a sequence: RBANS followed by rest, mental rotation, rest, and two parallel paths labeled control and training. Control leads to

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm. RBANS, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status; MISS, Multidimensional Iowa Suggestibility Scale.

2.4 Corsi block tapping task

The experimental group completed twenty minutes of visuospatial training using the CBTT from the Psychology Experiment Building Language (32). The protocol for the CBTT training is described in detail in previous work (29). Briefly, the CBTT is a visuospatial working memory task that presents nine square blocks (12) on the computer screen (32). During each trial, blocks sequentially lit up in yellow. The participant was asked to remember the sequence, then click on each square in the same order they were given. The task’s difficulty increased by increasing the sequence length when participants clicked the correct sequence twice in a row. Participants began their training with a sequence of three blocks and increased to a maximum of nine-block sequences based on performance. Once a nine-block sequence was reached, the sequence would remain nine blocks for the rest of the training. The primary outcome of this task was the best span (highest number of blocks correctly memorized) per trial.

2.5 Control condition

The control group completed a computerized version of the Short Suggestibility Scale, a subscale of the Multidimensional Iowa Suggestibility Scale (33). If the participants completed the questionnaire before the twenty-minute block was complete, a nature walk video was played on the computer for the remainder of the twenty minutes. The control paradigm engaged participants on the computer screen with minimal visuospatial demands. All participants in the control group were adults post-stroke.

2.6 Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed in RStudio (R version 4.4.1) (34). To determine if CBTT training impacted mental rotation reaction time, we used a linear mixed effects model with fixed effects for time (pre- and post-), group (control and training), time by group interaction, and a random intercept for participant. We also included a fixed effect for sex to account for sex differences in mental rotation that have been previously reported (35, 36). We checked model assumptions using the performance package (37). The model included outliers (determined using the check_outliers function (37)); thus, we fit a robust linear mixed effects model to downweight the effect of these outliers (38). We also examined whether the number of correct mental rotation trials changed after CBTT training. We fit the same model described above, with the number of correct trials as the outcome.

To provide preliminary effect sizes to inform future work, we calculated Hedges’ g for change in reaction and change in number of correct trials between groups (39). To assess the overall within-subject changes in mental rotation reaction time, we also computed an effect size for paired samples (40).

Since the right hemisphere plays a large role in spatial memory (4143), and previous work has demonstrated that lesion side impacted non-computerized CBTT performance (44), we also performed an exploratory analysis examining whether stroke lesion side impacted CBTT performance. We fit a linear mixed effects model with fixed effects for time (in minutes), lesion side, and a random slope for time and intercept for participant. Before fitting the model, we log-transformed time because participants experienced larger performance gains in early training and smaller gains later in training. To ensure the log transformation of time was appropriate, we compared a model with the logarithmic transformation of time and a linear model using Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC). We checked model assumptions using the performance package (37). The model included outliers; thus, we fit a robust linear mixed effects model to downweight the effect of these outliers (38).

3 Results

We included nineteen participants in the analysis. Ten participants were included in the control group (age: 51 ± 14, sex: 4 female, years since stroke: 6 ± 3, side affected: 5 left, RBANS total: 83 ± 11, RBANS visuospatial/constructional: 81 ± 15) and nine participants were in the training group (age: 58 ± 14, sex: 4 female, years since stroke: 8 ± 4, side affected: 4 left, RBANS total: 80 ± 11, RBANS visuospatial/constructional: 75 ± 11). There was no difference between groups in baseline cognitive function, measured by the RBANS total score (β = 2.6, p = 0.62). Of the 23 participants recruited, one participant was excluded from the training group due to hemispatial neglect, one was excluded from the control group due to technical difficulties, and one was excluded from the training group due to not improving past the first CBTT sequence. Participant demographics are included in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics.

3.1 Twenty minutes of CBTT training did not improve mental rotation

The control group exhibited a slightly greater reduction in mental rotation reaction time compared to the experimental group at the post-test [Hedges’ g = 0.28 (−0.62, 1.19)]. However, we found no statistically significant between-group difference in mental rotation reaction time at the post-test [Figure 2A; time β(SE) = −56.45(107.5), p = 0.61; time by group interaction: β(SE) = −96.59(148.19), p = 0.52; control reaction time: 1637 ± 303 ms, training reaction time: 1636 ± 457 ms]. This suggests that twenty minutes of CBTT training did not impact mental rotation reaction time. Both groups had similar average starting reaction times [control: 1773 ± 387 ms, training: 1673 ± 453 ms; group β(SE) = 93.39(204.75), p = 0.65]. There was no effect of sex on performance [β(SE) = 40.01(103.03), p = 0.84].

Figure 2
Graph A shows reaction time in milliseconds from pre to post measurements, with separate lines for control and CBTT training groups. Graph B displays the number of correct responses pre and post, also for control and CBTT groups. The control group is marked with blue circles, and the CBTT group with black triangles.

Figure 2. Mental rotation performance. Each line represents data from an individual participant. (A) Reaction time on pre- vs. post-test. (B) Number of correct trials on pre- vs. post-test.

Since mental rotation reaction time was not significantly different between groups, we investigated the potential effect of repeated mental rotation testing by calculating an effect size for paired samples. We found a small effect of repeated testing with an effect size of −0.26 [−0.75, 0.23]. However, because the 95% confidence interval crosses zero, there is likely a minimal effect of re-testing mental rotation after twenty minutes.

The training group showed a slightly greater change in the number of correct trials than the experimental group at the post-test [Hedges’ g = 0.25 (−0.66, 1.15)]. However, we found no statistically significant between-group difference in the number of correct trials [Figure 2B; group β(SE) = 3.63(5.20), p = 0.49; time β(SE) = 3.63(2.12), p = 0.11; time by group β(SE) = 0.18(2.84), p = 0.95; sex β(SE) = −1.57(5.10), p = 0.76]. This suggests that twenty minutes of CBTT training did not impact the number of correct trials on the mental rotation task. Mental rotation performance values for each participant can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

3.2 Lesion side did not impact CBTT performance

The maximum CBTT span ranged between six and eight, suggesting potential for further improvement with longer training periods. Similar to neurotypical adults (29), improvement in the CBTT over time in persons post-stroke followed a logarithmic trend (Figure 3; logarithmic BIC 486.2, linear BIC: 607.9). We found no effect of lesion side on CBTT performance [log(time) β(SE) = 0.78(0.16), p = 0.001; lesion side β(SE) = −0.38(0.52), p = 0.49; log(time) by lesion side β(SE) = 0.03(0.21), p = 0.89].

Figure 3
Nine scatter plots show the relationship between time in minutes (x-axis) and span (y-axis) for different datasets labeled 9, 11, 12, 14, 15, 2, 19, 3, and 4. Each plot displays data points with a curve indicating trend, typically showing a rapid increase that stabilizes over time.

Figure 3. Corsi block tapping task performance across participants in the training group. Each panel shows Corsi block tapping task training data from an individual participant on correct trials. Individual models from the mixed effects model are plotted on each panel.

4 Discussion

We aimed to understand if a single, twenty-minute CBTT training session improved mental rotation performance in adults with stroke compared to a no-training control group of adults with stroke. We found that twenty minutes of CBTT training did not improve post-stroke mental rotation in this sample. This suggests that longer CBTT training bouts or a different type of visuospatial training may be necessary to improve mental rotation in adults with stroke.

Twenty minutes of CBTT training did not improve reaction time or number of correct trials in the mental rotation task. This contrasts with our previous work that found twenty minutes of CBTT training improved mental rotation performance in neurotypical young adults (29). Increased dosage of CBTT training may be necessary to improve mental rotation in adults post-stroke. There is evidence that individuals with cognitive impairment need a higher dosage of computerized cognitive training (45) than we provided in our study. For cognitively impaired older adults, the ideal dosage of a computerized cognitive training program that included CBTT (among other cognitive trainings) was between 45–50 min/day, 6 days/week (45). While the exact computerized cognitive training paradigm differed from the one used in our study, the results indicate that individuals with possible cognitive impairment (such as adults with stroke) may need higher dosage and frequency than younger neurotypical adults to see improvements in cognition. There is also previous work suggesting that listening to classical music can improve CBTT performance in neurotypical adults (46), suggesting that other interventions could be paired with CBTT training to potentially enhance visuospatial performance.

There was considerable variability in mental rotation performance within our sample, with some participants exhibiting significant reductions in reaction time at the post-test while others showed substantial increases in reaction time (Figure 2A). Individual variability and our relatively small sample size may have limited our ability to detect a clear group effect. Our work provides preliminary effect sizes for future research on mental rotation or CBTT training. Further research is needed to understand the factors that may contribute to between-individual variability.

It is also possible that a different visuospatial training paradigm may have a more substantial effect on mental rotation performance than only training with the CBTT. Though the CBTT and mental rotation both require visuospatial working memory, they may use different subsystems of visuospatial working memory (47, 48). Mental rotation primarily relies on the visual subsystem (48), where the CBTT primarily relies on the spatial subsystem (47). Thus, the transfer between tasks may be lower than a task that primarily trains the visual subsystem, such as a pattern span task (47). There is evidence that visuospatial working memory training (using a matrix task) has limited transfer to other visuospatial tasks (i.e., CBTT, Stroop test, etc.), particularly in neurotypical adults older than 75 years (49). It may be more beneficial to incorporate various visuospatial trainings to maximize improvements in visuospatial function after stroke.

We found that stroke lesion side did not impact CBTT performance. Previous work has found that individuals with right hemisphere lesions have worse CBTT performance compared to individuals with left hemisphere lesions (44). However, others have found no effect of lesion side (50, 51). This is in line with evidence that individuals with left hemisphere lesions can also have impaired spatial performance, specifically with spatial visualization (52, 53). Additionally, the CBTT is likely not a purely visuospatial task and may require executive function resources, particularly with longer sequence lengths (54). This suggests that brain areas outside of the right-dominant spatial areas of the brain may be active. Previous work has found that spatial memory was correlated with distributed bilateral damage to cortical and subcortical structures (51, 55). Spatial memory deficits also appear to be correlated with damage to functional networks and white matter tracts (51, 55, 56), which may contribute to why we did not find an effect of lesion side.

Our study had a few limitations. First, participants only completed a single, twenty-minute training session, limiting our ability to understand the impact of repeated CBTT training on mental rotation. Future work is needed to understand the optimal dosage and frequency of CBTT training. Second, our sample size was relatively small, with only nine participants completing the CBTT training. This limited our ability to rigorously assess factors that may impact CBTT performance, as smaller sample sizes may not accurately detect the experimental effect (57). However, this work provides data (see Supplementary Table 1) that can be used in future work to perform a priori power calculations to help determine an appropriate sample size (57).

In conclusion, we found that twenty minutes of CBTT training did not improve mental rotation in adults with stroke and that lesion side did not impact CBTT performance. More research is needed to understand the optimal dosage, frequency, and content for computerized cognitive training to improve visuospatial function in adults with stroke.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Southern California. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SK: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Data curation, Visualization. GK: Project administration, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. AH: Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing – review & editing. CH: Project administration, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. SS: Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization. KL: Funding acquisition, Resources, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Data curation, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the National Institute of Health Grants R03 HD104217 (KL), K01 AG073467 (KL), and the Magistro Family Foundation Research Grant from the Foundation for Physical Therapy Research (KL).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2025.1601454/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Nys, GMS, van Zandvoort, MJE, de Kort, PLM, Jansen, BPW, de Haan, EHF, and Kappelle, LJ. Cognitive disorders in acute stroke: prevalence and clinical determinants. Cerebrovasc Dis. (2007) 23:408–16. doi: 10.1159/000101464

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Jokinen, H, Melkas, S, Ylikoski, R, Pohjasvaara, T, Kaste, M, Erkinjuntti, T, et al. Post-stroke cognitive impairment is common even after successful clinical recovery. Eur J Neurol. (2015) 22:1288–94. doi: 10.1111/ene.12743

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cumming, TB, Brodtmann, A, Darby, D, and Bernhardt, J. The importance of cognition to quality of life after stroke. J Psychosom Res. (2014) 77:374–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Stolwyk, RJ, Mihaljcic, T, Wong, DK, Hernandez, DR, Wolff, B, and Rogers, JM. Post-stroke cognition is associated with stroke survivor quality of life and caregiver outcomes: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. (2024) 34:1235–64. doi: 10.1007/s11065-024-09635-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Watson, PA, Gignac, GE, Weinborn, M, Green, S, and Pestell, C. A Meta-analysis of neuropsychological predictors of outcome following stroke and other non-traumatic acquired brain injuries in adults. Neuropsychol Rev. (2020) 30:194–223. doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09433-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Stolwyk, RJ, Mihaljcic, T, Wong, DK, Chapman, JE, and Rogers, JM. Poststroke cognitive impairment negatively impacts activity and participation outcomes: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Stroke. (2021) 52:748–60. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.120.032215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. APA dictionary of psychology. Available online at: https://dictionary.apa.org/

Google Scholar

8. Mervis, CB, Robinson, BF, and Pani, JR. Visuospatial construction. Am J Hum Genet. (1999) 65:1222–9. doi: 10.1086/302633

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Randolph, C. Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status (RBANS) manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (1998).

Google Scholar

10. Osterrieth, PA. Le test de copie d’une figure complexe; contribution a l’etude de la perception et de la memoire. Arch Psychol. (1944) 30:206–356.

Google Scholar

11. McAfoose, J, and Baune, BT. Exploring visual–spatial working memory: a critical review of concepts and models. Neuropsychol Rev. (2009) 19:130–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9063-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Corsi, PM. Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. (1972) 34:89.

Google Scholar

13. Wechsler Memory Scale–Fourth Edition. (2012). doi: 10.1037/t15175-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Lingo VanGilder, J, Hooyman, A, Bosch, PR, and Schaefer, SY. Generalizing the predictive relationship between 1-month motor skill retention and Rey–Osterrieth delayed recall scores from nondemented older adults to individuals with chronic stroke: a short report. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil. (2021) 18:94. doi: 10.1186/s12984-021-00886-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lingo VanGilder, J, Hengge, CR, Duff, K, and Schaefer, SY. Visuospatial function predicts one-week motor skill retention in cognitively intact older adults. Neurosci Lett. (2018) 664:139–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wang, P, Infurna, FJ, and Schaefer, SY. Predicting motor skill learning in older adults using visuospatial performance. J Mot Learn Dev. (2020) 8:38–51. doi: 10.1123/jmld.2018-0017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wang, P, Lingo VanGilder, J, Schweighofer, N, and Schaefer, SY. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure recall scores and motor skill learning in older adults: a non-linear mixed effect model-based analysis. Hum Mov Sci. (2022) 86:103004. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2022.103004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Seidler, RD, Bo, J, and Anguera, JA. Neurocognitive contributions to motor skill learning: the role of working memory. J Mot Behav. (2012) 44:445–53. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2012.672348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lingo VanGilder, J, Lohse, KR, Duff, K, Wang, P, and Schaefer, SY. Evidence for associations between Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test and motor skill learning in older adults. Acta Psychol. (2021) 214:103261. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lingo VanGilder, J, Walter, CS, Hengge, CR, and Schaefer, SY. Exploring the relationship between visuospatial function and age-related deficits in motor skill transfer. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2020) 32:1451–8. doi: 10.1007/s40520-019-01345-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hooyman, A, Lingo VanGilder, J, and Schaefer, SY. Mediation analysis of the effect of visuospatial memory on motor skill learning in older adults. J Mot Behav. (2022) 55:1–10. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2022.2105793

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Leech, KA, Roemmich, RT, Gordon, J, Reisman, DS, and Cherry-Allen, KM. Updates in motor learning: implications for physical therapist practice and education. Phys Ther. (2022) 102:pzab250. doi: 10.1093/ptj/pzab250

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bowden, MG, Behrman, AL, Neptune, RR, Gregory, CM, and Kautz, SA. Locomotor rehabilitation of individuals with chronic stroke: difference between responders and nonresponders. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2013) 94:856–62. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2012.11.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Dobkin, BH, Nadeau, SE, Behrman, AL, Wu, SS, Rose, D, Bowden, M, et al. Prediction of responders for outcome measures of locomotor experience applied post stroke trial. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2014) 51:39–50. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2013.04.0080

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lingo VanGilder, J, Hooyman, A, Peterson, DS, and Schaefer, SY. Post-stroke cognitive impairments and responsiveness to motor rehabilitation: a review. Curr Phys Med Rehabil Rep. (2020) 8:461–8. doi: 10.1007/s40141-020-00283-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kettlety, SA, Finley, JM, and Leech, KA. Visuospatial skills explain differences in the ability to use propulsion biofeedback post-stroke. J Neurol Phys Ther. (2024) 48:207–16. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000487

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Larson, EB, Kirschner, K, Bode, RK, Heinemann, AW, Clorfene, J, and Rebecca Goodman, RG. Brief cognitive assessment and prediction of functional outcome in stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. (2003) 9:10–21. doi: 10.1310/84YN-Y640-8UEQ-PDNV

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wagle, J, Farner, L, Flekkøy, K, Bruun Wyller, T, Sandvik, L, Fure, B, et al. Early post-stroke cognition in stroke rehabilitation patients predicts functional outcome at 13 months. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2011) 31:379–87. doi: 10.1159/000328970

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Schaefer, SY, Hooyman, A, Haikalis, NK, Essikpe, R, Lohse, KR, Duff, K, et al. Efficacy of Corsi block tapping task training for improving visuospatial skills: a non-randomized two-group study. Exp Brain Res. (2022) 240:3023–32. doi: 10.1007/s00221-022-06478-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Wong, A, Black, SE, Yiu, SYP, Au, LWC, Lau, AYL, Soo, YOY, et al. Converting MMSE to MoCA and MoCA 5-minute protocol in an educationally heterogeneous sample with stroke or transient ischemic attack. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2018) 33:729–34. doi: 10.1002/gps.4846

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Green, S, Sinclair, E, Rodgers, E, Birks, E, Lincoln, N, Hofgren, C, et al. The repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status (RBANS) for post-stroke cognitive impairment screening. Int J Ther Rehabil. (2013) 20:536–42. doi: 10.12968/ijtr.2013.20.11.536

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Mueller, ST, and Piper, BJ. The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. J Neurosci Methods. (2014) 222:250–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kotov, R, Bellman, S, and Watson, D. Multidimensional Iowa suggestibility scale (MISS). (2004).

Google Scholar

34. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

35. Zapf, AC, Glindemann, LA, Vogeley, K, and Falter, CM. Sex differences in mental rotation and how they add to the understanding of autism. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0124628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124628

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Jansen, P, and Heil, M. Gender differences in mental rotation across adulthood. Exp Aging Res. (2009) 36:94–104. doi: 10.1080/03610730903422762

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lüdecke, D, Ben-Shachar, MS, Patil, I, Waggoner, P, and Makowski, D. Performance: an R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. J Open Source Softw. (2021) 6:3139. doi: 10.21105/joss.03139

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Koller, M. Robustlmm: an R package for robust estimation of linear mixed-effects models. J Stat Softw. (2016) 75:1–24. doi: 10.18637/jss.v075.i06

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hedges, LV. Distribution theory for glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat. (1981) 6:107–28. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Morris, SB, and DeShon, RP. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychol Methods. (2002) 7:105–25. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Milner, B. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Br Med Bull. (1971) 27:272–7. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070866

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. De Renzi, E, Faglioni, P, and Previdi, P. Spatial memory and hemispheric locus of lesion. Cortex. (1977) 13:424–33. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(77)80022-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rushworth, MF, Ellison, A, and Walsh, V. Complementary localization and lateralization of orienting and motor attention. Nat Neurosci. (2001) 4:656–61. doi: 10.1038/88492

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Kessels, RPC, Van Zandvoort, MJE, Postma, A, Kappelle, LJ, and De Haan, EHF. The Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. (2000) 7:252–8. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0704_8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Liu, L, Wang, H, Xing, Y, Zhang, Z, Zhang, Q, Dong, M, et al. Dose–response relationship between computerized cognitive training and cognitive improvement. NPJ Digit Med. (2024) 7:1–7. doi: 10.1038/s41746-024-01210-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Giannouli, V, Yordanova, J, and Kolev, V. Can brief listening to Mozart’s music improve visual working memory? An update on the role of cognitive and emotional factors. J Intelligence. (2024) 12:54. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence12060054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Baddeley, A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2003) 4:829–39. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hyun, JS, and Luck, SJ. Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation. Psychon Bull Rev. (2007) 14:154–8. doi: 10.3758/bf03194043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Borella, E, Carretti, B, Cantarella, A, Riboldi, F, Zavagnin, M, and De Beni, R. Benefits of training visuospatial working memory in young–old and old–old. Dev Psychol. (2014) 50:714–27. doi: 10.1037/a0034293

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kessels, RPC, Jaap Kappelle, L, de Haan, EHF, and Postma, A. Lateralization of spatial-memory processes: evidence on spatial span, maze learning, and memory for object locations. Neuropsychologia. (2002) 40:1465–73. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00199-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chechlacz, M, Rotshtein, P, and Humphreys, GW. Neuronal substrates of Corsi block span: lesion symptom mapping analyses in relation to attentional competition and spatial bias. Neuropsychologia. (2014) 64:240–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Mehta, Z, Newcombe, F, and Damasio, H. A left hemisphere contribution to visuospatial processing. Cortex. (1987) 23:447–61. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(87)80006-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Vogel, JJ, Bowers, CA, and Vogel, DS. Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities: a meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. (2003) 52:197–204. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00056-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Vandierendonck, A, Kemps, E, Fastame, MC, and Szmalec, A. Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. Br J Psychol. (2004) 95:57–79. doi: 10.1348/000712604322779460

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Corbetta, M, Ramsey, L, Callejas, A, Baldassarre, A, Hacker, CD, Siegel, JS, et al. Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron. (2015) 85:927–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Salvalaggio, A, De Filippo De Grazia, M, Zorzi, M, Thiebaut de Schotten, M, and Corbetta, M. Post-stroke deficit prediction from lesion and indirect structural and functional disconnection. Brain. (2020) 143:2173–88. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa156

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lohse, K, Buchanan, T, and Miller, M. Underpowered and overworked: problems with data analysis in motor learning studies. J Mot Learn Dev. (2016) 4:37–58. doi: 10.1123/jmld.2015-0010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stroke, cognition, visuospatial function, cognitive training, mental rotation

Citation: Kettlety SA, Kibler GL, Hooyman A, Holl CK, Schaefer SY and Leech KA (2025) Twenty minutes of Corsi block tapping task training does not improve mental rotation in adults with stroke. Front. Neurol. 16:1601454. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2025.1601454

Received: 27 March 2025; Accepted: 26 September 2025;
Published: 16 October 2025.

Edited by:

Victor W. Mark, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States

Reviewed by:

Vaitsa Giannouli, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
Chunjuan Zhang, Haiyan People's Hospital, China

Copyright © 2025 Kettlety, Kibler, Hooyman, Holl, Schaefer and Leech. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kristan A. Leech, a2xlZWNoQHB0LnVzYy5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.