- 1Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
- 2KBR, Houston, TX, United States
- 3German Aerospace Center (DLR), Cologne, Germany
- 4NASA Johnson Space Center, Houston, TX, United States
- 5Norman Fixel Institute for Neurological Diseases, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
Introduction: Studies have shown that microgravity results in high dual task costs when crewmembers perform cognitive-motor dual tasking. Head-down tilt bedrest (HDBR) has been widely used as a spaceflight analog environment, recreating some of the sensorimotor and cognitive changes, headward fluid shifts, and unloading of bones and muscles. Here, we examined whether artificial gravity (AG) mitigates changes in cognitive performance and associated brain activity that occur in the HDBR environment.
Methods: We tested one group of participants (n = 16) that received 30 min of daily AG (half received it continuously while the other half experienced it in 5-min bouts), and one group that did not (n = 8 controls) during the course of 60 days HDBR. Participants performed spatial working memory and cognitive-motor dual tasking prior to entering HDBR, during HDBR and post-HDBR.
Results: Brain activation patterns associated with these two tasks changed with HDBR, but there was no difference between the AG and control groups. Compensatory brain-behavioral change-change correlations were observed, where those who increased activation the most had the least decrease in motor tapping accuracy from pre-HDBR to late-HDBR.
Discussion: These results suggest that AG does not reduce the need for compensatory brain responses that occur with HDBR, but longer duration and/or more optimal AG phasing may be required.
1 Introduction
Long duration spaceflight missions to the International Space Station (ISS) have widespread effects on human neurophysiology and performance. Sensory and motor deficits have been identified post-flight (1–13), and in-flight (14, 15). Cognitive changes due to spaceflight have been more difficult to identify.
Studies have reported decreased attentional capacity, decreased dual task performance, decreased spatial awareness, impaired performance on a visuospatial task, and complaints of “space fog” in microgravity (14, 16–22). Dual task performance has been shown to suffer when crewmembers are initially exposed to microgravity, but it then recovers over the course of weeks to months inflight (19, 20), suggesting that some manner of adaptation occurs. Similarly, deficits in dual-tasking postflight have been observed soon following return to Earth but recovers during the first week following long-duration missions (23). In the NASA Twins Study, the twin that went to space displayed more risk taking on the balloon analog risk test (BART; a decision making test to assess risk-taking behavior) during the entirety of his 1 year in spaceflight and also showed decreased task speed in the emotion recognition task and the digit symbol substitution task, as well as a decline in abstract matching (17). However, in a previous study we also analyzed digit symbol substitution task performance changes from pre-to postflight, finding no effects of microgravity in a larger cohort of astronauts (n = 15) that spent 6–12 months on the ISS (6). Further, we found no changes in spatial working memory performance from pre-to post-flight, but we did see alterations in brain functional connectivity (24) during task performance from pre-to postflight. This suggests that performance may be preserved with recruitment of additional compensatory neural resources post-flight, similar to what we identified post-flight in a sensorimotor adaptation task (25). In sum, some studies show cognitive performance changes with spaceflight whereas others do not. These inconsistencies could be due to a variety of factors including small sample sizes, individual differences, varying flight durations, times tested relative to g-transitions, varying tasks and cognitive domains assessed, and different task difficulty levels across studies.
We have previously assessed the effects of HDBR on cognition, finding increased performance on the card rotation task due to task practice (26). Further, we found decreased functional MRI activity during a spatial working memory task (SWM) within the right inferior frontal gyri and the left dentate nucleus. Moreover, participants that increased activation within the right angular gyrus increased spatial working memory performance, and those with a greater decrease in the inferior frontal gyri showed less declines in SWM accuracy (27). Further investigations have identified a general slowing in cognitive processing due to HDBR, particularly in sensorimotor processing speed (28, 29). Overall, these findings illustrate that even though cognitive performance may be maintained, there may still be underlying brain changes occurring with HDBR. These brain changes may reflect compensation or decreased neural efficiency as a result of HDBR. Recruitment of additional neural resources to maintain task performance levels may reduce the ability for dual tasking or to direct attention elsewhere.
HDBR involves adaptation to a new environment and sensory reweighting due to reduced foot sole and other somatosensory inputs and rotation of the body relative to the gravitational vector (30). Due to this, HDBR serves as a model environment to test integrated countermeasures such as artificial gravity (AG). This can be applied by having participants lay at the end of a short arm centrifuge spinning at a constant angular velocity. During the NASA Artificial Gravity Bed Rest – European Space Agency (AGBRESA) campaign, this velocity was adjusted for participant height and applied such that there was 1Gz at the center of mass (CoM), 2Gz at the feet and 0.3Gz at the head. AG is applied along the long axis of the body, increasing proprioceptive and somatosensory inputs, and stimulating the vestibular system, resulting in improved orthostatic tolerance relative to HDBR without AG (31–33). In a 5 day HDBR pilot study, AG reduced orthostatic intolerance with 30 min of daily AG (33). AG also reduced orthostatic intolerance in the 60 day AGBRESA campaign (76). We found that AG participants in AGBRESA performed better on the paced auditory serial addition task (PASAT, performed during active centrifugation) compared to control participants who performed the task in HDBR with no centrifugation, while other cognitive tasks showed no effects of AG (34). This was further supported by work showing learning effects (faster reaction time and increased accuracy) during simple and complex visual and auditory tasks accompanied by changes in electrocortical responses that may be related to impaired attentional processing, however, there were no counteractive effects of AG (35). Another study identified an overall slowing of cognitive performance across the AGBRESA campaign, including prolonged time to identify facial expressions during HDBR and an overall decrease in sensorimotor processing speed; these negative effects were not mitigated by AG (29).
Our primary aim was to examine whether AG applied along the long axis of the body via centrifugation would mitigate HDBR-induced changes in cognitive performance and associated brain activity. We administered the same spatial working memory and motor-cognitive dual task tests that we have previously used in our prior HDBR and spaceflight studies (26, 36–44, 77, 78). We found that during HDBR, there was increased functional brain activation during dual tasking across a large network containing frontal, parietal, cingulate, temporal and occipital cortices (45). In another HDBR study combined with elevated CO2 we found increased dual task cost effects in the middle temporal gyrus (78). We also observed decreased activation in the right middle frontal gyrus and left cerebellum while participants performed a SWM task in HDBR. Moreover, participants that improved SWM performance decreased their brain activity throughout HDBR, suggestive of practice effects (27).
Cognitive and sensorimotor declines that occur in HDBR and microgravity such as those that we have described here have been argued to stem from disrupted sensory inputs, sensory reweighting, and interference from adaptation to the microgravity environment. This study is a continuation of our previous efforts (34) to investigate the use of AG to counteract cognitive and sensorimotor brain and behavior deficits due to HDBR. In that study, we assessed overall behavior along the timelines as here and found that AG had minimal effects of cognitive performance, however, those that received AG had better performance on the PASAT, while other cognitive measures were largely unaffected. Functional mobility and balance performance showed significant effects due to HDBR, but not AG, but there was a large difference between those that received AG and those that did not in balance performance (34). Here, we hypothesized that those receiving daily AG would have reduced changes in brain activity during dual tasking during HDBR as compared to controls (in HDBR but receiving AG). Moreover, we predicted that this decreased activation would be associated with better cognitive performance. This would be suggestive of greater neural efficiency in the AG group.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Participants
There were 24 (8F, 33.3 ± 9.17 yrs., 174.6 ± 8.6 cm, 74.2 ± 10.0 kg) participants that volunteered for this study and were assigned to one of three groups. The first two groups received daily centrifugal artificial gravity applied either (1) continuously in one 30-min bout daily (cAG); or (2) intermittently in 6 bouts of 5 min with 3 min rest in between (iAG). We established that there were no significant differences in brain or behavior changes (p > 0.05) (34) between the iAG and cAG groups, and thus pooled them into one group (AG) to increase our statistical power. The other group received no artificial gravity but were in HDBR (control group, CTRL). All subjects were familiarized with AG twice (BDC-11 and BDC-4) during the baseline phase, prior to being separated into groups. Participants were screened for tolerability through a tolerance test (AG2 protocol) to ensure they would be able to complete centrifugation. They were also selected to be similar in age, sex and education level to astronauts. The University of Florida and NASA Institutional Review Boards as well as the local ethical commission of the regional medical association (Ärztekammer Nordrhein) approved all study procedures. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants first underwent 14 days of baseline data collection prior to entering HDBR, followed by 60 days of 6° HDBR and then 14 days post-HDBR (Figure 1). They performed a wide range of neurocognitive tasks including the paced auditory serial addition task, digit symbol substitution task, rod and frame task, and sensorimotor tasks four times, 7 days prior to entering HDBR, in the BDC phase (BDC-7), on days 29 and 58 during HDBR (HDBR29, HDBR58, respectively), and 10 days post HDBR to assess recovery (R + 10). The measures reported in this study were acquired in tandem with those in our previous AGBRESA reports (34, 83) and overlap with our ongoing NASA supported flight and prior bed rest studies (26, 27, 36–40, 42–44, 79).

Figure 1. Cognitive task timeline. (A) Cognitive performance data were collected twice pre- (−13 and −7 days), twice during (29 and 58 days) and once post-HDBR (+10 days). Black dots indicate when data was collected for all individuals. The gray box extending across the figure from day 0 to day 60 represents the HDBR period in which all participants were head down tilt; the AG group received centrifugal AG for 30 min a day during this time. (B) Predicted activation patterns due to AG. We hypothesized that the CTRL group would experience increased functional activation while performing the task due to HDBR (causing interference), whereas the AG group would show decreased brain activation (i.e., increased neural efficiency). (C) In the case that AG would not have an effect, we also tested for increases in functional activation for all participants.
2.2 Dual-tasking
During each of the data collections, participants performed a cognitive task, a motor task and both combined for dual tasking while in a Siemens 3T Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scanner. During the cognitive task participants were instructed to watch a small box that rapidly changed colors and to count the number of times that it turned blue. They were instructed to remember that count over the 4 trials (two as single task, two as dual task) and verbally report following all trials. The box changed colors at a rate of 3 Hz, with a low appearance rate of the blue color (1–3%), requiring sustained attention and oddball detection. During the motor task, participants were instructed to watch two small boxes that were side by side. When an “X” appeared in either box, they were to respond by pressing the corresponding button (i.e., left box, left button) on an MRI safe button box with either the left or the right index finger. The stimulus was presented randomly, with an inter-stimulus interval of 800 ms. Participants performed each of these tasks twice under single tasking and twice under dual tasking. We calculated participants’ reaction time, accuracy and dual-task cost for both measures on each task. The behavioral task results have been reported previously (34); we include the results here for brain-behavior correlation analyses (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Cognitive-Motor dual-task. The gray boxes with a black X alternate rapidly, and the participant is required to hit a button on a left or right button box, corresponding with the side on which the x flashes. The blue box changes color rapidly; participants are instructed to count, and remember for the duration of the task, how many times the box changed to blue. They perform each as a single task, and also simultaneously under dual-task conditions.
2.3 Spatial working memory
In addition to the cognitive-motor dual task, participants performed a spatial working memory task that we have previously used in HDBR campaigns (77) and in spaceflight (6, 24). Participants performed the task in an MRI scanner, allowing us to capture functional MRI (fMRI). In this task, participants are shown three solid circles that appear for 500 ms before disappearing for 3,000 ms, leaving the screen blank. Participants are instructed to connect the dots into the shape of a triangle before the dots appear again in different positions. Participants must then identify if these dots are in the same triangular configuration but rotated, or if they form an entirely different triangle. Participants performed this task in two runs of 30 trials each. For the control phase of the task, participants must simply identify if a singular dot (presented for 500 ms) appearing on the probe screen is in the same position as one of the three dots previously presented, with a 200 ms delay. Participants performed one, 40-trial run of the control phase. The primary outcome metric of this task was accuracy (deciding whether or not the three dots were in the same configuration at the probe stage). We acquired fMRI during this task while participants were in the HDBR position inside of the MRI scanner (laying head down tilt on a foam wedge, with the head supine inside the coil).
2.4 MRI parameters
Data were acquired at the: envihab facility in Cologne, Germany with a 3-Tesla Siemens Biograph MRI Scanner. We used a gradient echo T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters to acquire fMRI data: TR: 2500 ms, TE: 32 ms, flip angle: 90°, FOV: 192 × 192 mm, matrix: 64 × 64, slice thickness: 3.5 mm, voxel size: 3 × 3 × 3.5 mm, 37 slices. We also acquired a T1-weighted gradient-echo pulse sequence with the following parameters: TR: 1.9 s, TE: 2.4 ms, flip angle: 9°, FOV: 250 × 250 mm, matrix: 512 × 512, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, voxel size: 0.49 × 0.49 × 1.0 mm, 192 slices. During the fMRI collection, subjects were still in the HDBR position laying upon a foam wedge, however the head was flat within the MRI head coil.
2.5 MRI pre-processing
Pre-processing and statistical analysis of the fMRI data were conducted using Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM12; version 7,219), MATLAB R2019a, Advanced Normalization Tools [ANTs; (46)] and FMRIB Software Library [FSL; (47)]. First, fMRI field maps were created to map and remove B0 inhomogeneities utilizing the FSL topup tool (47). Next, we corrected the images for slice timing and then realigned and resliced to the mean slice to correct for volume-to-volume head motion in SPM12. We used the Artifact Detection Tool (ART1) to perform further quality checks. We covaried out volumes with a motion threshold greater than 2.0 mm, and a global brain signal Z threshold equal to or greater than 9. We then used ANTs (46) to normalize the MRI images to the Montreal Neurological Institute 152 (MNI152) standard template in multiple steps. First, participant-specific T1 templates were created across time points using the AntsMultivariateTemplateConstruction.sh function; then, we created participant-specific mean functional templates using the same ANTs function. We co-registered each participant-specific functional template to their respective T1 template to acquire transform parameters using the AntsRegistration.sh function. Further, we normalized each participant’s specific template to the MNI152 standard space using AntsRegistrations.sh function as well. We then applied the resulting warp parameters to the participants’ fMRI runs using ANTs’ ApplyTransforms.sh function. We then used SPM12 to spatially smooth with an 8 mm full-width half-maximum three-dimensional Guassian kernel. This pre-processing procedure is the same as we have used in our past HDBR neuroimaging work (24, 27, 39).
2.6 Cerebellar pre-processing
We utilized special pre-processing methods for the cerebellum using a combination of the CEREbellum Segmentation [CERES; (80)] pipeline and the Spatially Unbiased Infratentorial Template [SUIT; (48, 49)] pipeline. We have used this pipeline previously to more clearly identify changes in the cerebellum [cf. (24, 27, 39)]. First, we ran the participant-specific T1 templates in the CERES pipeline to segment the cerebellum from the rest of the brain in each T1-weighted image. Then, we created a binary mask from the CERES native space output using ImCalc in SPM12 and used this to mask out the brain from the participant-specific T1 template in FSL. Then we used ANTs AntsRegistration.sh to transform the T1 cerebellar template to SUIT space. We then transformed the slice time corrected, realigned and resliced (as described above) functional runs into the participants’ T1 template space using AntsTransform.sh and masked with the binary cerebellar mask in FSL. Finally, we normalized the masked, functional runs into SUIT space using AntsApplyTransforms.sh. We then applied a 2 mm full-width half-maximum three-dimensional smoothing Gaussian kernel to SUIT space cerebellar images in SPM12. We chose the 2 mm kernel based on the small lobule size in the cerebellum; this is similar to other studies that have focused on cerebellar analyses (48–52).
2.7 Behavioral statistical analysis
The full statistical analysis is covered in-depth separately from here (34). To summarize, we used the R 3.6.1 (81) package nlme (82) to fit linear mixed models with restricted maximum likelihood to test for changes over time as participants enter HDBR. In each model, subject was considered as a random intercept to control for each participant’s different starting point. Mean centered age and sex were included as covariates within the statistical models.
2.8 Subject-level fMRI statistics
Single subject level brain activation for both tasks was calculated separately for the cerebellum and the whole brain. For dual-tasking, we calculated four statistical maps for each participant, at each time point, on a voxel-by-voxel basis with the following contrasts: (1) single motor task > rest, (2) single cognitive task > rest, (3) cognitive-motor dual task > rest, (4) dual task > both single tasks. For our SWM task, we assessed on a voxel-by-voxel basis the SWM task compared to rest (SWM > rest). As with our previous work (24, 27, 39, 79, 85), we used a first level masking threshold of –infinity to mask out non-brain regions using SPM12’s intracranial volume mask. With this, we were able to include all voxels in our first-level general linear models, as opposed to the SPM12 default where we would be limited to voxels with a mean value of ≥80% of the global signal.
2.9 Group level statistical analyses
To assess brain activation changes throughout HDBR we tested multiple group-level statistical models, described in detail below. First, we modeled hypothesis driven longitudinal changes where we predicted the AG group would reduce neural activation during task performance following entrance to HDBR and the CTRL group to increase neural activation; we also tested for the reverse contrast. Second, we tested all individuals as one group, in the case that AG did not show any effects. In this model, we tested for an overall increase in neural activation after all participants had entered the HDBR environment. We built these models using the Sandwich Estimate Toolbox for SPM12 [SWE; (53)]. SWE utilizes a noniterative marginal model to prevent within subject convergence problems that are inherent in longitudinal studies. This provides a superior analysis of longitudinal MRI data that can handle small subject samples sizes and missing data. We maintained the SwE defaults except for testing with 999 permutations for the non-parametric wild bootstrapping (54). Additionally, threshold free cluster enhancement was used [TFCE; (84)], as this approach does not require us to pre-specify an arbitrary cluster size and is more sensitive (55).
2.9.1 Effects of AG versus control
To test whether the AG group differed from CTRL, we tested an a priori hypothesized weighted longitudinal model (Figure 1). We created longitudinal contrasts that included all 4 time points with each weighted to test for our hypothesized changes. We have used the same approach and weights in our previous AGBRESA investigations (83, 85) and similar approaches in other HDBR analog and spaceflight experiments (24, 27, 37–39, 43, 45, 79). The models reflect hypothesized linear changes, whether that be increases or decreases. Mean centered age and sex were included in the model as covariates. Significance was analyzed at a p < 0.05, family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons. For whole brain analysis, an explicit mask was used to run analysis only on gray matter. This mask was created through binarizing the Computational Anatomy Toolbox 12 [CAT12; (56, 57)] MNI-space gray matter template at a threshold of 0.1. The cerebellum was masked out with the SUIT.nii cerebellar template. Cerebellar analyses was conducted only on the cerebellum as discussed above in “Cerebellar Pre-processing” with no mask utilized; all other specifications were the same.
2.9.2 Effects of HDBR
To assess the effects of HDBR on cognitive task function we utilized a similar approach as discussed above. However, here we pooled all subjects (AG and CTRL) into one large group. This allows us to search for brain regions that show a similar pattern of change for all participants, regardless of AG dosage.
2.9.3 Brain and behavior change – change correlations
To assess the effects of AG on HDBR, we used a similar approach in brain-behavioral correlations as discussed above. However, here we assessed the change in cognitive behavioral performance between BDC-7 and HDBR58 and the changes in brain activation at the same time points to assess for overall changes that may occur due to 60 days of HDBR. We conducted this analysis with all participants pooled into one group.
3 Results
To assess significance, we used a family wise error (FWE) corrected p value of FWE < 0.05 and voxel extent of k˃10. Our initial analyses identified no significant or trending differences in brain activation or behavior between the two AG groups (intermittent versus continuous). We therefore pooled them together to increase statistical power.
3.1 Behavior
As mentioned previously, full details of the behavioral performance are provided in a previous publication (34). We identified no significant group by time interactions in the motor single task (tapping), the cognitive single task (counting), dual tasking, dual task cost nor SWM performance. We include the data here to examine relationships between individual differences in brain and behavior changes.
3.2 HDBR + AG
To assess the counter-active effects of AG on HDBR, we tested our hypothesis that those that did not receive AG daily would have greater changes in brain activity with HDBR, whereas the AG group would either maintain or decrease their activation. We identified no brain regions that followed this hypothesized trend in either dual-task or SWM brain activity that met our pre-determined alpha level of FWE < 0.05. Further, there were no trending effects approaching significance.
3.3 HDBR
To assess the effects of HDBR on cognitive-motor dual-tasking, regardless of the effects of AG, we evaluated our hypothesis that activation would increase in task relevant brain regions after entering the HDBR environment, potentially reflecting decreased neural efficiency. We identified one cluster (FWE < 0.05, Table 1) that displayed a significant effect of HDBR during dual task performance. All participants increased brain activation within the right pre-supplementary motor area (58) following their entrance into HDBR; this change recovered over time across the 60 days spent in the HDBR environment (Figure 3). There were no other identified brain changes in either single task or dual task cost.

Figure 3. Activation changes across HDBR in all participants during cognitive-motor dual-tasking, where all participants show an increased activation shortly upon entering HDBR, that decreases by HDBR58. The activation profile for the right pre-supplementary motor region is shown for illustration. Results are overlaid onto the MNI standard template. pFWE-corr, yellower colors represent smaller p-values.
To assess the effects of HDBR on SWM, in the absence of counter-active AG effects, we assessed all participants’ neural activation in a single group. We identified three significant clusters (FWE < 0.05, Table 2) that showed an increase in activation following entrance to the HDBR environment that continued to increase throughout the HDBR period before returning to baseline levels in the recovery phase (Figure 4).

Figure 4. Activation changes across HDBR in all participants during SWM; all participants showed increased activation shortly upon entering HDBR that was maintained throughout HDBR. The activation profile for the right precuneus, left paracentral lobule and right postcentral gyrus regions are shown. Results are overlaid onto the MNI standard template. pFWE-corr, yellower colors represent smaller p-values.
3.4 Brain and behavior correlations
We conducted brain-behavior change-change correlations with all participants in one group (AG and Control). In the SWM task, we did not identify any significant brain-behavior correlations from BDC-7 to HDBR58. For dual-tasking, we also identified no significant relationships between brain activity changes and changes in the cognitive single or cognitive dual task accuracy. However, for the dual task tapping accuracy, we identified a significant relationship between activation changes in the left postcentral gyrus (pFWE < 0.05; Table 3) and dual task tap accuracy. Individuals who increased activation (reduced their de-activation) the most had the smallest decrease in motor tapping accuracy from pre-HDBR to late-HDBR (HDBR day 58, Figure 5).

Figure 5. Brain and behavior change-change correlation of dual-task tapping. Change on the De-activation scale indicates a lower activation during late-HDBR compared to pre-HDBR, and change on dual-task tap accuracy scale indicates lower accuracy during late-HDBR compared to pre-HDBR. pFWE-corr, brighter colors represent smaller p-values.
4 Discussion
We investigated whether AG would mitigate HDBR effects on brain changes that occur during cognitive-motor dual-tasking or during SWM task performance. We observed no effect of AG on brain changes in single tasking, nor dual-task cost. We identified an initial increase followed by decreased activation within the right pre-supplementary motor cortex that was specific to the HDBR phase during dual-tasking (Figure 3). We also identified that during the SWM task, the right precuneus, the right postcentral gyrus and the left paracentral lobule increased activation once participants had entered the HDBR environment; this effect remained constant throughout HDBR and was resolved by 10 days post-HDBR. Finally, we identified a dual-tasking brain-behavior change-change correlation in the left postcentral gyrus, in which individuals that increased their brain activation the most maintained their dual-task tapping performance relative to baseline. Taken together, we interpret these findings to reflect that 30 min of AG daily has minimal effects on brain changes underlying cognitive function during HDBR, at least for the duration and dosage used in the AGBRESA campaign.
While we did not identify any counter-active effects of AG on dual-tasking brain activation, we did see increased activation in the right pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA) for the combined groups during dual tasking performance that was specific to the HDBR period (Figure 3). This increase in activation subsided by the end of HDBR, suggesting adaptation occurred over time in HDBR. We observed similar effects in a different HDBR study (45), suggesting that this region may be particularly affected by HDBR. The preSMA has been argued to be important for movement control, particularly for complex actions and cognitive control of movement (59). This region also plays a critical role in stopping action (60–62), suggesting that HDBR may affect cognitive-motor interactions. We further identified a brain-behavior change-change correlation during dual-tasking where those that decreased their activation within the left postcentral gyrus the most maintained their tapping performance relative to baseline at the end of HDBR (Figure 5). The postcentral gyrus comprises of the somatosensory cortex, the primary region associated with sensory perception from the body (63). Interestingly, this correlation would suggest that those that were the most able to decrease activation in a region heavily involved in sensory perception region maintained their pre-HDBR motor performance. This may reflect sensory reweighting. It is not directly clear how this result relates to performance, however, since the cluster is in a region that is substantially inferior to the hand knob location.
Similar to cognitive-motor dual-tasking, AG had no effects on SWM performance and functional brain activation. All participants (AG and CTRL) increased activation in various brain regions during spatial working memory task performance after they entered the HDBR environment. Specifically, we identified increased activation within the right precuneus, the right postcentral gyrus and the left paracentral lobule (Figure 4) that persisted throughout HDBR and resolved by 10 days after participants exited the HDBR environment. The precuneus has been shown to play a role in visuo-spatial and higher cognitive function (64). The precuneus has also been argued to serve as a major node in various functional brain networks, including two subnetworks of the default mode network (65). The default mode network is heavily involved in introspection and rumination, and is considered an anti-task network, exhibiting decreased activity when participants perform a task relative to rest (66). This increased activation of the precuneus could indicate difficulty with network switching between task and default mode networks; similar effects have been seen in older adults (67). The postcentral gyrus receives somatosensory projections via the thalamus (63), as discussed above. The somatosensory cortex is primarily lateralized, with the left hemisphere receiving inputs from the right side of the body (68, 69). Here we saw increased activation in the right hemisphere, while participants were using their right hand. This implies a compensatory response, as compensation typically involves recruitment of bilateral brain regions (70). Due to the reduced somatosensory inputs that occur in HDBR (with the body largely unloaded), bilateral recruitment of the postcentral gyrus may increase the gain of somatosensory signaling. We also observed increased activation in the left paracentral lobule, another region which has been shown to process somatosensory inputs (71). This would also align with previous findings showing that HDBR results in increased functional connectivity between the posterior parietal cortex and somatosensory cortices, and AG was associated with decreased connectivity between these regions, suggesting compensatory responses in somatosensory processing due to HDBR (86). Further, those in the AG group that decreased their connectivity the most had minimal mobility declines from pre-to post-HDBR (86). Overall, these changes in brain activation patterns suggest that the HDBR environment is making this SWM task more difficult to perform, resulting in recruitment of additional brain regions. It is interesting that these are largely in somatosensory cortical regions rather than in frontoparietal areas, typically engaged for SWM tasks (72).
These cognitive assessments were included as part of a large HDBR campaign with funding from American, German and European space associations (NASA, DLR & ESA; AGBRESA). The overall aim was to determine whether AG would serve as an effective integrated countermeasure for mitigating many HDBR changes that occur. In this campaign, another group reported that HDBR resulted in cognitive performance declines, with no benefit of AG (28). We found largely similar results, with the exception of cognitive task performance during AG centrifugation. Those that received AG had better cognitive performance on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT) (34) than those that performed it during HDBR alone. This, taken in conjunction with the present findings, suggests that AG does not have a strong impact on cognition, but it could be useful for cognitive tasks performed during centrifugation.
The literature suggests that the primary advantages of AG are benefits to sensory and motor behavior. We have shown that AG can increase neural efficiency in visuomotor rotation tasks (83); further, we observed that those who decreased brain activity the most during task performance showed the least HDBR associated performance declines. We also found that the AG group showed greater decreases in resting state functional connectivity with the posterior parietal cortex, a major hub of sensory integration, than controls (86). Further, those in the AG group that showed the largest decreases in connectivity had minimal mobility declines from before to after HDBR (86). We have also shown that HDBR results in balance declines; however, those in the AG group had less declines, as evident by the large effect size even though not statistically significant (34). Thus, while AG ameliorates some of the sensorimotor effects of HDBR, the dosage of 30 min daily may be insufficient to see counter-active effects in all aspects of cognition. Moreover, the centrifugation velocity resulted in 1 Gz at the heart, but only 0.3 Gz at the level of the head. Increasing the dosage to reach levels of 1Gz at the head may result in more pronounced effects of AG on cognition and the brain. However, this would also increase Gz at the heart and the feet and may not be optimal for function in those areas. Other doses/durations of AG may have greater effects on neurocognitive function. It should be kept in mind, however, that findings in HDBR do not completely map onto those observed in the microgravity environment [for review see: Barkaszi et al. (73)].
The sample size of this study is limited, particularly if the AG groups are kept separate. This makes it difficult to identify more nuanced group differences, particularly when investigating cognitive changes with HDBR as effect sizes are generally not large. Moreover, the time spent in HDBR for this campaign was 60 days, whereas a typical mission to the ISS lasts approximately 6 months. We do not currently understand whether or how the duration of HDBR scales to the duration of microgravity, particularly in terms of neurocognitive changes. Sixty days of HDBR does result in many similar effects to microgravity on the musculoskeletal and cardiovascular systems, though. Moreover, the centrifugation velocity in this study resulted in 1 Gz at the CoM, approximately 2 Gz at the foot, and approximately one third Gz at the head. It is possible that the lack of effects here are due to the level of AG at the head not being sufficient to mitigate structural and functional brain changes from HDBR. Future studies should investigate greater levels of Gz at the head to potentially increase neurological benefits. It is also important to note, that the HDBR environment may increase levels of physiological stress, such as with sleep disturbances that could have impacts brain connectivity and cognitive task performance (74, 75). Ultimately, while AG does not appear to have a clear effect on cognitive function in HDBR, further examination is warranted with a greater emphasis on larger sample sizes and varying AG doses.
Here, we investigated whether AG is an effective countermeasure for HDBR induced performance changes in cognitive-motor dual tasking and spatial working memory. We identified no effects of AG on either task, instead finding increased activation in somatosensory brain regions for all participants after they entered the HDBR environment. Further, we identified a brain-behavior correlation in dual-tasking where those that decreased their activation the most in the left postcentral gyrus had the highest level of performance. Overall, these findings would suggest that AG, as applied in this study, is not sufficient to counteract functional brain changes that occur in HDBR, and possibly spaceflight.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the University of Florida Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
GT: Writing – original draft, Data curation, Writing – review & editing. HM: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. YD: Writing – review & editing, Investigation. EM: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Methodology. JB: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology. AM: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. SW: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Investigation. RS: Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by grants from the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA 80NSSC18K0783) to RS, AM, SW, and JB. During the completion of this work GT was supported by the University of Florida’s (UF) Graduate Student Funding Award and by NIH T32-NS082128.
Conflict of interest
Authors YD and AM were employed by company KBR.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
References
1. Bloomberg, JJ, Peters, BT, Smith, SL, Huebner, WP, and Reschke, MF. Locomotor head-trunk coordination strategies following space flight. J Vestibular Res. (1997) 7:161–77. doi: 10.3233/VES-1997-72-307
2. Layne, CS, Lange, GW, Pruett, CJ, McDonald, PV, Merkle, LA, Mulavara, AP, et al. Adaptation of neuromuscular activation patterns during treadmill walking after long-duration space flight. Acta Astronaut. (1998) 43:107–19. doi: 10.1016/S0094-5765(98)00148-9
3. McDonald, PV, Basdogan, C, Bloomberg, JJ, and Layne, CS. Lower limb kinematics during treadmill walking after space flight: implications for gaze stabilization. Experimental Brain Res. (1996) 112:325–34. doi: 10.1007/BF00227650
4. Miller, CA, Kofman, IS, Brady, RR, May-Phillips, TR, Batson, CD, Lawrence, EL, et al. Functional task and balance performance in bed rest subjects and astronauts. Aerospace Medicine and Human Performance. (2018) 89:805–15. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5039.2018
5. Mulavara, AP, Peters, BT, Miller, CA, Kofman, IS, Reschke, MF, Taylor, LC, et al. Physiological and functional alterations after spaceflight and bed rest. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2018) 50:1961–80. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001615
6. Tays, GD, Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Salazar, AP, De Dios, YE, Beltran, NE, et al. The effects of long duration spaceflight on sensorimotor control and cognition. Front Neural Circuits. (2021) 15:723504. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.723504
7. Reschke, MF, Bloomberg, JJ, Harm, DL, Paloski, WH, Layne, C, and McDonald, V. Posture, locomotion, spatial orientation, and motion sickness as a function of space flight. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. (1998) 28:102–17. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00031-9
8. Reschke, MF, Bloomberg, JJ, Paloski, WH, Harm, DL, and Parker, DE. Neurophysiologic aspects: Sensory and sensorimotor function in space physiology and medicine. Philadelphia: Nicogossian AE, Huntoon CL, and Pool SL. Lea & Febiger (1994).
9. Reschke, MF, Bloomberg, JJ, Harm, DL, and Paloski, WH. Space flight and Neurovestibular adaptation. J Clin Pharmacol. (1994) 34:609–17. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.1994.tb02014.x
10. Black, FO, Paloski, WH, Doxey-Gasway, DD, and Reschke, MF. Vestibular plasticity following orbital spaceflight: recovery from post-flight postural instability. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. (1995) 115:450–4. doi: 10.3109/00016489509125296
11. Black, FO, Paloski, WH, Reschke, MF, Igarashi, M, Guedry, F, and Anderson, DJ. Disruption of postural Readaptation by inertial stimuli following space flight. J Vestibular Res. (1999) 9:369–78. doi: 10.3233/VES-1999-9506
12. Paloski, WH, Reschke, MF, Black, FO, Doxey, DD, and Harm, DL. Recovery of postural equilibrium control following spaceflight. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1992) 656:747–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25253.x
13. Paloski, WH, Bloomberg, JJ, Reschke, MF, and Harm, DL. Spaceflight-induced changes in posture and locomotion. J Biomech. (1994) 27:812. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(94)91366-8
14. Bock, O, Abeele, S, and Eversheim, U. Sensorimotor performance and computational demand during short-term exposure to microgravity. Aviat Space Environ Med. (2003) 74:1256–62.
15. Campbell, MR, Williams, DR, Buckey Jr, JC, and Kirkpatrick, AW. Animal surgery during spaceflight on the Neurolab shuttle Mission. Aviat Space Environ Med. (2005) 76:589–93.
16. Bock, O, Weigelt, C, and Bloomberg, JJ. Cognitive demand of human sensorimotor performance during an extended space Mission: a dual-task study. Aviat Space Environ Med. (2010) 81:819–24. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.2608.2010
17. Garrett-Bakelman, FE, Darshi, M, Green, SJ, Gur, RC, Lin, L, Macias, BR, et al. The NASA twins study: a multidimensional analysis of a year-long human spaceflight. Science. (2019) 364:eaau8650. doi: 10.1126/science.aau8650
18. Jones, CW, Basner, M, Mollicone, DJ, Mott, CM, and Dinges, DF. Sleep deficiency in spaceflight is associated with degraded neurobehavioral functions and elevated stress in astronauts on six-month missions aboard the international Space Station. Sleep. (2022) 45:zsac006. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsac006
19. Manzey, D, Schiewe, A, and Fassbender, C. Psychological countermeasures for extended manned spaceflights. Acta Astronaut. (1995) 35:339–61. doi: 10.1016/0094-5765(95)98736-S
20. Manzey, D, Lorenz, B, and Poljakov, V. Mental performance in extreme environments: results from a performance monitoring study during a 438-day spaceflight. Ergonomics. (1998) 41:537–59. doi: 10.1080/001401398186991
21. Strangman, GE, Sipes, W, and Beven, G. Human cognitive performance in spaceflight and analogue environments. Aviat Space Environ Med. (2014) 85:1033–48. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.3961.2014
22. Takács, E, Barkaszi, I, Czigler, I, Pató, LG, Altbäcker, A, McIntyre, J, et al. Persistent deterioration of visuospatial performance in spaceflight. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:9590. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-88938-6
23. Moore, ST, Dilda, V, Morris, TR, Yungher, DA, MacDougall, HG, and Wood, SJ. Long-duration spaceflight adversely affects post-landing operator proficiency. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:2677. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-39058-9
24. Salazar, AP, McGregor, HR, Hupfeld, KE, Beltran, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. Changes in working memory brain activity and task-based connectivity after long-duration spaceflight. Cereb Cortex. (2023) 33:2641–54. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac232
25. Tays, GD, Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Banker, LA, De Dios, YE, Bloomberg, JJ, et al. The microgravity environment affects sensorimotor adaptation and its neural correlates. Cereb Cortex. (2025):bhae502.
26. Lee, JK, De Dios, Y, Kofman, I, Mulavara, AP, Bloomberg, JJ, and Seidler, RD. Head down tilt bed rest plus elevated CO2 as a spaceflight analog: effects on cognitive and sensorimotor performance. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 13:355. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00355
27. Salazar, AP, Hupfeld, KE, Lee, JK, Banker, LA, Tays, GD, Beltran, NE, et al. Sensorimotor adaptation brain changes during a spaceflight analog with elevated carbon dioxide (CO2): a pilot study. Front Neural Circuits. (2021) 15:659557. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.659557
28. Basner, M, Dinges, DF, Howard, K, Moore, TM, Gur, RC, Mühl, C, et al. Continuous and intermittent artificial gravity as a countermeasure to the cognitive effects of 60 days of head-down tilt bed rest. Front Physiol. (2021) 12:643854. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.643854
29. Basner, M, Stahn, AC, Nasrini, J, Dinges, DF, Moore, TM, Gur, RC, et al. Effects of head-down tilt bed rest plus elevated CO2 on cognitive performance. J Appl Physiol. (2021) 130:1235–46. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00865.2020
30. Hargens, AR, and Vico, L. Long-duration bed rest as an analog to microgravity. J Appl Physiol. (2016) 120:891–903. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00935.2015
31. Clément, GR, Bukley, AP, and Paloski, WH. Artificial gravity as a countermeasure for mitigating physiological deconditioning during long-duration space missions. Front Syst Neurosci. (2015) 9:92. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00092
32. Hargens, AR, Bhattacharya, R, and Schneider, SM. Space physiology VI: exercise, artificial gravity, and countermeasure development for prolonged space flight. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2013) 113:2183–92. doi: 10.1007/s00421-012-2523-5
33. Linnarsson, D, Hughson, RL, Fraser, KS, Clément, G, Karlsson, LL, Mulder, E, et al. Effects of an artificial gravity countermeasure on orthostatic tolerance, blood volumes and aerobic power after short-term bed rest (BR-AG1). J Appl Physiol. (2015) 118:29–35. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00061.2014
34. Tays, GD, McGregor, HR, Lee, JK, Beltran, N, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. The effects of 30 minutes of artificial gravity on cognitive and sensorimotor performance in a spaceflight analog environment. Front Neural Circuits. (2022) 16:784280. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2022.784280
35. Tölgyesi, B, Altbäcker, A, Barkaszi, I, Stuckenschneider, T, Braunsmann, L, Takács, E, et al. Effect of artificial gravity on neurocognitive performance during head-down tilt bedrest. NPJ Microgravity. (2024) 10:59. doi: 10.1038/s41526-024-00405-4
36. Koppelmans, V, Erdeniz, B, De Dios, YE, Wood, SJ, Reuter-Lorenz, PA, Kofman, I, et al. Study protocol to examine the effects of spaceflight and a spaceflight analog on neurocognitive performance: extent, longevity, and neural bases. BMC Neurol. (2013) 13:205. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-13-205
37. Yuan, P, Koppelmans, V, Reuter-Lorenz, P, De Dios, Y, Gadd, N, Riascos, R, et al. Change of cortical foot activation following 70 days of head-down bed rest. J Neurophysiol. (2018) 119:2145–52. doi: 10.1152/jn.00693.2017
38. Yuan, P, Koppelmans, V, Reuter-Lorenz, P, De Dios, Y, Gadd, N, Wood, S, et al. Vestibular brain changes within 70 days of head down bed rest. Hum Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:2753–63. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24037
39. Hupfeld, KE, Lee, JK, Gadd, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, Bloomberg, JJ, et al. Neural correlates of vestibular processing during a spaceflight analog with elevated carbon dioxide (CO2): a pilot study. Front Syst Neurosci. (2019) 13:80. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00080
40. Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Lee, JK, Beltran, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. The impact of 6 and 12 months in space on human brain structure and intracranial fluid shifts. Cerebral Cortex Commun. (2020) 1:tgaa023. doi: 10.1093/texcom/tgaa023
41. Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Koppelmans, V, Beltran, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. Brain and behavioral evidence for reweighting of vestibular inputs with long-duration spaceflight. Cereb Cortex. (2022) 32:755–69. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab239
42. McGregor, HR, Lee, JK, Mulder, E, De Dios, Y, and Beltran, NE. Ophthalmic changes in a spaceflight analog are associated with brain functional reorganization. bioRxiv. (2020). doi: 10.1101/2020.09.09.289827v1.abstract
43. McGregor, HR, Lee, JK, Mulder, ER, and De Dios, YE. Brain connectivity and behavioral changes in a spaceflight analog environment with elevated CO2. NeuroImage. (2021) 225:117450. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117450
44. Banker, LA, Salazar, AP, Lee, JK, Beltran, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. The effects of a spaceflight analog with elevated CO2 on sensorimotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol. (2021) 125:426–36. doi: 10.1152/jn.00306.2020
45. Yuan, P, Koppelmans, V, Reuter-Lorenz, PA, De Dios, YE, Gadd, NE, Wood, SJ, et al. Increased brain activation for dual tasking with 70-days head-down bed rest. Front Syst Neurosci. (2016) 10:71. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00071
46. Avants, BB, Tustison, NJ, Song, G, Cook, PA, Klein, A, and Gee, JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage. (2011) 54:2033–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025
47. Jenkinson, M, Beckmann, CF, Behrens, TEJ, Woolrich, MW, and Smith, SM. FSL. NeuroImage. (2012) 62:782–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015
48. Diedrichsen, J. A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. (2006) 33:127–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056
49. Diedrichsen, J, Balsters, JH, Flavell, J, Cussans, E, and Ramnani, N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. (2009) 46:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045
50. Diedrichsen, J, Maderwald, S, Küper, M, Thürling, M, Rabe, K, Gizewski, ER, et al. Imaging the deep cerebellar nuclei: a probabilistic atlas and normalization procedure. NeuroImage. (2011) 54:1786–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.035
51. King, M, Hernandez-Castillo, CR, Poldrack, RA, Ivry, RB, and Diedrichsen, J. Functional boundaries in the human cerebellum revealed by a multi-domain task battery. Nat Neurosci. (2019) 22:1371–8. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0436-x
52. King, M, Shahshahani, L, Ivry, RB, and Diedrichsen, J. A task-general connectivity model reveals variation in convergence of cortical inputs to functional regions of the cerebellum. eLife. (2023) 12:e81511. doi: 10.7554/eLife.81511
53. Guillaume, B, Hua, X, Thompson, PM, Waldorp, L, and Nichols, TEAlzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Fast and accurate modelling of longitudinal and repeated measures neuroimaging data. NeuroImage. (2014) 94:287–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.029
54. Guillaume, B., and Nichols, T. (2015). “Non-parametric inference for longitudinal and repeated-measures neuroimaging data with the wild bootstrap. Poster Presented at the Organization for Human Brain. Available online at: https://scholar.google.ca/scholar?cluster=14518305275081419913&hl=en&as_sdt=0,5&sciodt=0,5. (December 09, 2022)
55. Smith, SM, and Nichols, TE. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage. (2009) 44:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061
56. Dahnke, R, Yotter, RA, and Gaser, C. Cortical thickness and central surface estimation. NeuroImage. (2013) 65:336–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.050
57. Gaser, C, Dahnke, R, Thompson, PM, Kurth, F, and Luders, E. The Alzheimer’s Disease. CAT: a computational anatomy toolbox for the analysis of structural MRI data Open Access. Neuroimaging Initiative Author Notes GigaScience. (2024). 13:giae049. doi: 10.1093/gigascience/giae049
58. Mayka, MA, Corcos, DM, Leurgans, SE, and Vaillancourt, DE. Three-dimensional locations and boundaries of motor and premotor cortices as defined by functional brain imaging: a meta-analysis. NeuroImage. (2006) 31:1453–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.004
59. Nachev, P, Kennard, C, and Husain, M. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:856–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478
60. Cai, W, George, JS, Verbruggen, F, Chambers, CD, and Aron, AR. The role of the right presupplementary motor area in stopping action: two studies with event-related transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 108:380–9. doi: 10.1152/jn.00132.2012
61. Swann, NC, Cai, W, Conner, CR, Pieters, TA, Claffey, MP, George, JS, et al. Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity. NeuroImage. (2012) 59:2860–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.049
62. Watanabe, T, Hanajima, R, Shirota, Y, Tsutsumi, R, Shimizu, T, Hayashi, T, et al. Effects of rTMS of pre-supplementary motor area on fronto basal ganglia network activity during stop-signal task. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:4813–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3761-14.2015
63. DiGuiseppi, J, and Tadi, P. Neuroanatomy, postcentral gyrus In: StatPearls [internet]. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing (2023)
64. Cavanna, AE, and Trimble, MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. (2006) 129:564–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004
65. Dadario, NB, and Sughrue, ME. The functional role of the precuneus. Brain. (2023) 146:3598–607. doi: 10.1093/brain/awad181
66. Smallwood, J, Bernhardt, BC, Leech, R, Bzdok, D, Jefferies, E, and Margulies, DS. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2021) 22:503–13. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00474-4
67. Persson, J, Lustig, C, Nelson, JK, and Reuter-Lorenz, PA. Age differences in deactivation: a link to cognitive control? J Cogn Neurosci. (2007) 19:1021–32. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.1021
68. Cramer, SC, Finklestein, SP, Schaechter, JD, Bush, G, and Rosen, BR. Activation of distinct motor cortex regions during ipsilateral and contralateral finger movements. J Neurophysiol. (1999) 81:383–7. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.1.383
69. Cross, KP, Heming, EA, Cook, DJ, and Scott, SH. Maintained representations of the ipsilateral and contralateral limbs during bimanual control in primary motor cortex. J Neurosci. (2020) 40:6732–47. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0730-20.2020
70. Strens, LH, Fogelson, N, Shanahan, P, Rothwell, JC, and Brown, P. The ipsilateral human motor cortex can functionally compensate for acute contralateral motor cortex dysfunction. Curr Biol. (2003) 13:1201–5. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00453-6
71. Cho, S, Kurokawa, R, Hagiwara, A, Gonoi, W, Mori, H, Kawahara, T, et al. Localization of the central sulcus using the distinctive high signal intensity of the paracentral lobule on T1-weighted images. Neuroradiology. (2022) 64:289–99. doi: 10.1007/s00234-021-02729-4
72. Reuter-Lorenz, PA, Jonides, J, Smith, EE, Hartley, A, Miller, A, Marshuetz, C, et al. Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. J Cogn Neurosci. (2000) 12:174–87. doi: 10.1162/089892900561814
73. Barkaszi, I, Ehmann, B, Tölgyesi, B, Balázs, L, and Altbäcker, A. Are head-down tilt bedrest studies capturing the true nature of spaceflight-induced cognitive changes? A review. Front Physiol. (2022) 13:1008508. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.1008508
74. Curtis, BJ, Williams, PG, Jones, CR, and Anderson, JS. Sleep duration and resting fMRI functional connectivity: examination of short sleepers with and without perceived daytime dysfunction. Brain Behavior. (2016) 6:e00576. doi: 10.1002/brb3.576
75. Lythe, KE, Williams, SCR, Anderson, C, Libri, V, and Mehta, MA. Frontal and parietal activity after sleep deprivation is dependent on task difficulty and can be predicted by the fMRI response after normal sleep. Behav Brain Res. (2012) 233:62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.050
76. Hoenemann, JN, Moestl, S, van Herwaarden, AE, Diedrich, A, Mulder, E, Frett, T, et al. Effects of daily artificial gravity training on orthostatic tolerance following 60-day strict head-down tilt bedrest. Clin Auton Res. (2023) 33:401–410. doi: 10.1007/s10286-023-00959-5
77. Salazar, AP, Hupfeld, KE, Lee, JK, Beltran, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. Neural Working Memory Changes During a Spaceflight Analog With Elevated Carbon Dioxide: A Pilot Study. Front Syst Neurosci. (2020) 14:48. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.00048
78. Mahadevan, AD, and Hupfeld, AE, Lee, JK, De Dios, YE, Kofman, IS, Beltran, NE, et al. Head-Down-Tilt Bed Rest With Elevated CO2: Effects of a Pilot Spaceflight Analog on Neural Function and Performance During a Cognitive-Motor Dual Task. Front Physiol. (2021) 12:654906. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.654906
79. Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Reuter-Lorenz, PA, and Seidler, RD. Microgravity effects on the human brain and behavior: Dysfunction and adaptive plasticity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 122:176–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.11.017
80. Romero, JE, Coupé, P, Giraud, R, Ta, VT, Fonov, V, Park, MTM, et al. CERES: A new cerebellum lobule segmentation method. Neuroimage. (2017) 147:916–924. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.003
81. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. (2019). https://www.R-project.org/
82. Pinheiro, J, Bates, D, DebRoy, S, Sarkar, D, Eispack,, Heisterkamp, S, et al. (2022) https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
83. Tays, GD, Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Beltran, NE, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, et al. Daily artificial gravity is associated with greater neural efficiency during sensorimotor adaptation. Cereb Cortex. (2023) 33:8011–8023. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhad094
84. Smith, SM, and Nichols, TE. Statistical Challenges in Big Data Human Neuroimaging. Neuron. (2018) 97:263–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.12.018
85. Tays, GD, Hupfeld, KE, McGregor, HR, Beltran, NE, De Dios, YE, Mulder, E, et al. Daily artificial gravity partially mitigates vestibular processing changes associated with head-down tilt bedrest. NPJ Microgravity. (2024) 10:27. doi: 10.1038/s41526-024-00367-7
Keywords: spaceflight, cognition, artificial gravity, head down tilt (HDT) bed rest, fMRI
Citation: Tays GD, McGregor HR, De Dios YE, Mulder E, Bloomberg JJ, Mulavara AP, Wood SJ and Seidler RD (2025) Thirty minutes of daily artificial gravity does not mitigate head down tilt induced brain activity changes during cognitive task performance. Front. Neurol. 16:1602104. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2025.1602104
Edited by:
Tatyana Yakusheva, Washington University in St. Louis, United StatesReviewed by:
Olivier White, INSERM U1093 Cognition, Action et Plasticité Sensomotrice, FranceIrén Barkaszi, Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA), Hungary
Copyright © 2025 Tays, McGregor, De Dios, Mulder, Bloomberg, Mulavara, Wood and Seidler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Rachael D. Seidler, cmFjaGFlbHNlaWRsZXJAdWZsLmVkdQ==