- 1Institute of Psychology, University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria
- 2Eilert-Academy, Berlin, Germany
- 3ESB Business School, Marketing & Sales Management, Reutlingen University, Reutlingen, Germany
Background: Communication apprehension (CA) is a widespread phenomenon negatively affecting communicative skills, psychological well-being, and social connectedness. This randomized controlled study investigated whether online emotion-focused ego-state coaching, based on the emTrace framework, can sustainably reduce CA.
Methods: A total of 260 German-speaking participants with elevated CA were randomly assigned to one of two ego-state coaching interventions: Core Transformation (CT) or Smart Part Lab (SPL). CA was assessed using the PRCA-24 and a bipolar scale of subjective feeling at pretest, posttest, and 2-week follow-up.
Results: Both interventions significantly reduced CA [F(2, 258.3) = 242.14, p < 0.001; pre: M = 80.7; post: M = 63.4; follow-up: M = 62.3; η2p = 0.65), with 80% of initially high-CA participants reporting moderate or low CA at follow-up. CT induced stronger self-transcendent experiences than SPL; however, this did not translate into a stronger CA reduction, suggesting self-transcendence is not the primary mechanism of change.
Conclusion: A single 80-min online ego-state coaching session within the emTrace framework sustainably reduced trait-like CA. The findings highlight the potential of resource-oriented, emotion-focused coaching as a low-threshold intervention to foster communicative confidence and psychological resilience.
1 Introduction
Communication apprehension (CA) is defined by McCroskey (1977) as “an individual's level of fear or anxiety associated with either real or anticipated communication with another person or persons”. Research demonstrated that it is a widespread phenomenon: In a large-scale study (Marcel, 2019) of over 3,000 working adults, around 20% of respondents reported high levels of CA, with a further 35–40% exhibiting moderate degrees. This distribution remained stable regardless of educational level. The high prevalence of CA was confirmed in further studies (de Araújo et al., 2022; Croucher et al., 2023; Schulenberg et al., 2023).
Young adults seem to be particularly affected (Marcel, 2019): the mean CA was highest in the 20–29 age group, indicating that it is most pronounced at the beginning of a career. With increasing age, growing professional experience, and especially with increasing presentation frequency, CA decreases significantly—an effect that was consistently observed across all age groups. Nevertheless, Marcel (2019) also showed that although CA decreases over the course of a lifetime, it does not automatically disappear completely. Rather, the decline seems to be a slow, experience-based process that often remains incomplete without targeted intervention, such as coaching.
CA not only impairs communication skills but is also closely correlated with psychological and physical wellbeing. People with high CA often avoid situations that require a high level of speaking or take on more passive roles in conversations, which can result in impaired development of communication skills in the long term (Marcel, 2019). Furthermore, CA has been shown to correlate negatively with psychological wellbeing: Students with high CA reported significantly higher stress levels and lower psychological wellbeing (Agrawal and Krishna, 2021). Of particular concern is that CA can promote social withdrawal, thereby hindering the development of stable, supportive relationships. This is significant insofar as social connectedness is a key predictor of physical health and longevity: A comprehensive meta-analysis involving over 300,000 participants showed that insufficient social relationships increase the risk of mortality more than other established risk factors such as obesity, high blood pressure, or smoking (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010).
Against this background, it seems relevant from a social and health policy perspective to identify effective ways to reduce communication apprehension. Low-threshold, resource-oriented formats such as coaching offer particular potential here, as they can also be used outside clinical contexts and can be employed both preventively and as an accompanying intervention. Because numerous studies have shown that fostering emotion regulation is transdiagnostically effective and can, for example, specifically reduce symptoms of anxiety (Sakiris and Berle, 2019; Carlucci et al., 2021; Saccaro et al., 2024), coaching methods that actively incorporate emotion regulation into the change process could be a promising approach to reducing CA. Such a coaching approach is emTrace.
emTrace is an emotion-focused coaching framework. In contrast to more cognitive oriented coaching methods (e.g., GROW Coaching Model), which demonstrated its effectiveness in behavioral change (Panchal and Riddell, 2020), emTrace focuses on the central importance of emotions for change processes (for the significance of emotions in therapeutic changes, see e.g., McNeil and Repetti, 2022) and implements common factors (following Grawe, 2004) in coaching (such as the coach-client working alliance). To maximize the accuracy of fit between intervention and client's personality and needs, emTrace integrates emotion regulation techniques in different kinds of interventions (e.g., ego-state coaching) (see Eilert, 2021). Emotion regulation is achieved during emTrace coaching through bifocal processing (see e.g., de Voogd et al., 2018; Wittfoth et al., 2020), i.e., the clients split their attention between two stimuli, e.g., they focus their gaze on a specific point in space and simultaneously observe, without evaluating what is happening internally, which mental images, thoughts, and feelings emerge (Corrigan and Grand, 2013). Other tasks that promote emotion regulation via bifocal processing can include counting backward (e.g., van den Hout et al., 2010; Engelhard et al., 2011), tapping (e.g., Nelms and Castel, 2016), or slow-paced breathing (e.g., Wells et al., 2012; You et al., 2021). A pilot study (Nachreiner, 2020) demonstrated that emTrace coaching can reduce test anxiety.
While emTrace emphasizes the role of emotions in facilitating change, it is also important to consider the specific characteristics of CA as a phenomenon. The more accurately an intervention fits the underlying structure and experiential nature of a problem, the more effective it tends to be (Cuijpers et al., 2019; Nye et al., 2023). This raises the question of how CA can be conceptualized in a way that informs the choice of a well-fitting coaching intervention.
McCroskey (1977) understands CA less as a form of state anxiety and more as a manifestation of trait anxiety, defined as a consistent tendency to respond with fear or avoidance across a variety of communication contexts. Unlike state anxiety, which is elicited by specific situations, trait-based CA reflects a stable internalized pattern of emotional responses. This conceptualization suggests that individuals who experience high levels of CA may not simply be reacting to external triggers, but rather expressing a recurring, identity-related internal state. From this perspective, CA may be associated with what ego-state theory (Watkins and Watkins, 1979, 2019) refers to as an “inner part” or “ego state” that embodies earlier experiences and coping strategies related to social evaluation, rejection, or performance pressure.
Several studies have already demonstrated the effectiveness of ego-state interventions for various problem areas, particularly in the therapeutic field; for example, in the treatment of attachment trauma (e.g., Phillips, 2013; Spiegel, 2016), for post-traumatic stress disorder (e.g., Barabasz, 2013; Barabasz et al., 2013), narcissistic personality disorder (e.g., McNeal, 2007), specific phobias (e.g., Daharnis et al., 2021), and in depression therapy (e.g., Alladin, 2013).
This study examines the effect of two prominent ego-state interventions in the coaching literature on reducing CA: first, Core Transformation (CT), based on Andreas and Andreas (2008), and second, Smart Part Lab (SPL), based on the 6-Step-Reframing by Bandler and Grinder (1982); both modified for emTrace by integrating emotion regulation techniques according to Eilert (2023).
Braganza et al. (2019) demonstrated that CT promotes emotional stability, emotional balance (ratio of pleasant emotions to unpleasant ones), and general wellbeing. In addition, the effectiveness of CT was demonstrated in another pilot study by Braganza and Piedmont (2015). In contrast, despite extensive database searches, no research could be found on the ego-state intervention of 6-Step-Reframing according to Bandler and Grinder (1982).
Derived from the research just described, the first hypothesis is as follows:
H1: Communication apprehension differs in degree before and after the emTrace ego-state intervention; specifically, the coaching intervention reduces it.
The present study aims beyond investigating the mere effectiveness of ego-state coaching and asks what the possible mechanism of change by an ego-state intervention might be. Regarding CT, self-transcendence could be an essential factor.
According to Andreas and Andreas (2008), CT aims to achieve a self-transcendent state, a so-called core state; research speaks of the “smaller self-effect”. Zen Buddhism summarizes its transformative effect with the axiom “no self, no problem” (Niebauer, 2019). Brain research demonstrated that the smaller self-effect is even shown at a neuronal level: when a person feels self-transcendent emotions, the neuronal self-networks that are central to the person's sense of self are inhibited in their activity (Urgesi et al., 2010; van Elk et al., 2019). Consequently, core states decrease the tendency to ruminate about problems (e.g., Tarani, 2017). In addition, activating self-transcendent emotions, such as awe, promotes a resilient reaction to stress and buffers the negative effects of feedback (Atamba, 2019). Furthermore, Le et al. (2019) showed that experimental-induced awe fostered that the subjects perceived challenges ahead of them as more minor. Activating self-transcendent states also promotes the ability to think creatively and openness to new experiences (Chirico et al., 2018; Stancato and Keltner, 2019). This indicates that the activation of self-transcendent emotions could help reduce CA.
Therefore, based on these research results, the second hypothesis is:
H2: By activating a self-transcendent state, Core Transformation will reduce communication apprehension to a greater extent than an ego-state intervention that does not produce such a state.
This second main hypothesis can be divided into two sub-hypotheses:
H2a: Core Transformation results in an increased activation of a self-transcendent state compared to a conventional ego-state coaching intervention (here: Smart Part Lab).
H2b: The greater the self-transcendent state experienced by the person during the intervention, the greater the positive effect on the reduction of communication apprehension.
Finally, the question arises as to what extent the client's personality influences the extent to which an ego-state intervention can activate a self-transcendent state. The third main hypothesis relates to the client's personality. The trait of openness to experience—one of the five personality factors proposed by McCrae and Costa (1992)—seems to be of particular interest as a moderating factor. The degree of openness to experience is correlated with the frequency of experiencing self-transcendent emotional states (e.g., awe, love, compassion) (e.g., Shiota et al., 2006). This study, therefore, focuses on the moderating influence of the degree of the personality trait openness to experience in the client on the degree of activation of a self-transcendent state through an ego-state intervention.
The third hypothesis is (moderator hypothesis):
H3: The influence of the ego-state intervention on the activation of a self-transcendent state depends on the client's openness to experience.
Figure 1 presents the three hypotheses in a graphical overview.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Sample description and acquisition
This study focuses on female and male people in German-speaking countries who suffer from CA and are willing to consult a coach to reduce it. The sample was recruited via a call on social media and on the homepage of the Eilert Academy website. The incentive to participate was receiving free coaching to reduce CA. There were no other incentives.
The subjects were registered centrally using a form and then randomly allocated to the experimental and control groups under the restriction of equal group sizes. The subsequent allocation of clients to the 67 emTrace coaches who conducted the interventions was also randomized. Care was taken to ensure each coach had approximately the same number of clients per intervention group (see Table A-1; SPL per coach: M = 1.9, SD = 0.7; CT per coach: M = 2.0, SD = 0.9). The gender distribution of coaches was slightly skewed in favor of women at 64.2 %. The high number of coaches and the randomized but equal division of clients into CT and SPL groups per coach aimed to control for confounding variables related to the coach (e.g., competence, performance motivation, or personality traits). The clients' gender, age, and level of education were measured as personal variables. They were also asked whether German was their mother tongue. As the overall proportion of male subjects was significantly lower than that of female participants (22.7 % vs. 77.3 %), the gender distribution between the CT and SPL groups was parallelized.
A total of 265 subjects were recruited for the study, although five of these had to be excluded due to suspected alexithymia, which was within expectations; according to Montreuil and Pedinielli (1995), around one in ten people show difficulties perceiving their feelings and describing them in words. These clients reported considerable difficulties in describing their feelings, making it impossible to realize the interventions. This would have exceeded the planned 90-min time frame. 260 subjects were in the final sample, 133 in the CT intervention group and 127 in the SPL intervention group.
The age of the participants was between 19 and 69 years. The CT group (M = 39.1 years, SD = 11.3 years) did not differ significantly in age from the SPL group (M = 40.1 years, SD = 11.7 years), t(258) = 0.65, p = 0.52. No significant differences were shown in the gender composition (CT: 78.2 % female; SPL: 76.4 % female), = 0.12, p = 0.73, or in whether German was the native language (CT: 94.0 %; SPL: 92.1 %), = 0.35, p = 0.56. There was also no significant difference between the CT and SPL group regarding the highest educational qualification, = 3.53, p = 0.74. The most common highest educational qualification was a degree of a university of applied sciences or A-levels (CT: 31.6 %; SPL: 30.7 %) and the least common was a secondary school leaving certificate.
2.2 Study design and procedure
The experiment was conducted as a randomized controlled pre-post study with a 2-week follow-up period (see Figure 2). Participants were randomly assigned to one of two treatment conditions: Core Transformation (CT) or Smart Part Lab (SPL).
Two dependent variables were measured. CA was measured online using the PRCA-24 at three measurement points (a pretest measurement t1, 1 day before the coaching, a posttest measurement t3, 1 day after the intervention, and a follow-up measurement t4, 2 weeks after completion of the treatment). The SUD values were also measured directly before and after the coaching (t2a, b) and in the follow-up (t4). The state of self-transcendence (STS) in relation to the effect of the specific ego-state intervention was measured 1 day after the coaching (t3). In addition, the personality trait openness to experiences was measured as a moderator variable with the B5T 1 day before the coaching (t1). All coaching sessions were conducted online using the video conferencing provider Zoom, following the seven process steps subsequently outlined.
2.2.1 Measuring communication apprehension (CA)
According to McCroskey (1978), communication apprehension was measured using the Personal Report of Communication Apprehension (PRCA-24). This test was only available in English. It was translated into German and back-translated by a native English speaker to check the translation's quality and optimize it (see Appendix B). An example item is “I am tense and nervous while participating in group discussions”. The PRCA-24 asks about the degree of communication apprehension using 24 items on a 5-point Likert scale (strongly disagree – disagree – undecided – agree – strongly agree) for four contexts: group discussions, meetings, dyadic conversations, and public speaking. A total score is calculated for the PRCA-24.
The psychometric quality of the English-language PRCA-24 is considered high (see Woods, 2007): Cronbach's alpha is between 0.93 and 0.95. The retest reliability is 0.80, showing that CA measured by the PRCA-24 is stable over time. As part of a preliminary study (n = 202), the first author has checked the internal consistency of the German version of the PRCA-24, which was developed specifically for the present study. Cronbach's alpha was 0.96 and can be rated as excellent. Construct and criterion validity of the PRCA-24 are also supported by research (see also Woods, 2007).
In addition to the PRCA-24, CA was measured on a visual bipolar scale of subjective feelings (see Figure 3). When thinking about a specific stressful communication situation (individually chosen by each client), the clients assessed the intensity of their communication apprehension immediately before and after the intervention (t2a, b) and 2 weeks later (t4) on a bipolar scale of subjective feeling, ranging from −10, which represents the subjectively most unpleasant feeling, to +10, which symbolizes the subjectively most pleasant feeling; 0 represents a neutral emotional state.
Because the Subjective Units of Distress (SUD) scale (−10 to 0) measures state anxiety rather than trait anxiety, this measurement complements the PRCA-24 to additionally assess the change in subjective feeling regarding the specific communication situation, which was chosen by the client as a starting point of the ego-state intervention. Kim et al. (2008) demonstrated the validity of such a one-item scale and showed that the SUD score at the end of the first session could predict the overall therapy outcome.
2.2.2 Measurement of openness to experience
The moderator variable openness to experience was measured using the B5T according to Satow (2012b). Cronbach's alpha is between 0.76 and 0.90. Furthermore, factorial validity has been confirmed (Satow, 2012a). In the present study, only the openness scale of the B5T was used. This subscale consists of 10 items measured on a 4-point Likert scale (strongly disagree—somewhat disagree—somewhat agree—strongly agree). An example item is “I always enjoy learning new things.” The openness scale of the B5T achieves acceptable reliability with Cronbach's alpha = 0.76.
Satow (2021) reassessed the validity and reliability of the B5T as part of a re-normalization using a larger sample (n = 21048). The test's psychometric quality was confirmed.
2.2.3 Measurement of experienced self-transcendence
Self-transcendence was measured using the Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S) according to Yaden et al. (2018). It measures the state of self-transcendence via a self-report questionnaire using 30 items, measured on a 7-point Likert scale (strongly disagree—moderately disagree—somewhat disagree—neutral—somewhat agree—moderately agree—strongly agree). An example item is “I felt a sense of communion with all living things.” The AWE-S was only available in English. Therefore, the first author translated it into German using the back-translation method described above (see Appendix B).
The confirmed 6-factor structure of the AWE-S makes it a valuable measuring instrument for the present study. The presumed mechanism of change of self-transcendence, hypothesized to be activated by CT, could thus be divided into sub-dimensions. AWE-S shows high internal consistency for each of the six factors: altered time perception (α = 0.91), self-diminishment (α = 0.89), connectedness (α = 0.87), vastness (α = 0.85), physical sensations (α = 0.81) and need for accommodation (α = 0.80). Cronbach's alpha for the English total scale is α = 0.93 and thus shows a high internal consistency. The scale's validity was also confirmed empirically by Yaden et al. (2018).
2.2.4 The procedure of the ego-state interventions
The two ego-state interventions (CT and SPL) examined are standardized in their procedure based on the seven process goals of parts work according to Fritzsche (2018), as they are taught and trained in the emTrace advanced training course “Emotion coaching with ego states” (see Figure 4).
To ensure that the coaches adhered to the defined structure of the ego-state interventions, they were recruited from the participants of the emTrace advanced training course “Emotion coaching with ego states” (for a detailed description of the interventions, see also Eilert, 2023). Additionally, two online workshops of 120 min each were also conducted with the participating coaches. In these workshops, the process of the two interventions was practiced. Beforehand, the coaches received two recorded video coaching sessions for each ego-state intervention, in which the first author demonstrated the coaching process. A written process description, which was given to each coach as a digital file, aimed to additionally ensure that the procedure during the intervention was adhered to. Table 1 compares the two intervention procedures along the seven process goals. The differences are in process phases 5–7. The unique feature of CT is that it aims to activate a self-transcendent state. In contrast, SPL initiates the change via an inner creative conference.
3 Results
3.1 Descriptive statistics
3.1.1 Scales
Table 2 shows the scale means, standard deviations, and internal consistency of the scales.
The internal consistency of the openness scale of the B5T can be rated as acceptable with a Cronbach's α = 0.72. For the PRCA-24 and AWE-S scales, Cronbach's α is between 0.91 and 0.93. The internal consistency of these two questionnaires translated into German for the present study can therefore be rated as excellent. To ensure that the German versions of these two scales can be used by other researchers, the internal consistency of the subscales is shown in Tables 3 and 4.
For the subscales of the PRCA-24, Cronbach's α is between 0.79 and 0.90. The internal consistency of the subscales can, therefore, be rated as good to excellent.
For the subscales of the AWE-S (see Table 4), Cronbach's α is between 0.76 and 0.91. The subscale “physical sensations” shows the lowest value at 0.76, but this can still be rated as acceptable. All other subscales show good to excellent internal consistency.
3.1.2 Coaching duration
The mean coaching duration was M = 80.3 min (SD = 21.0 min) with a minimum of 40 min and a maximum of 140 min.
The difference between interventions (CT: M = 82.9 min, SD = 21.9 min, SPL: M = 77.6 min, SD = 19.8 min) was significant, F(1, 258) = 4.28, p = 0.04. However, this difference can be neglected in practice due to the usual coaching duration of 60–90 min.
3.2 Hypothesis testing
The three main hypotheses are tested in numerical order using a multilevel model (H1 and H2) and an ANOVA (H3). The two sub-hypotheses, H2a and H2b, are then tested using a two-sample t-test and a correlation analysis. The number of cases for the measurement times t1 to t3 was N = 260, with N = 244 in the follow-up measurement (t4).
The calculations were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 28.
3.2.1 Effect of ego-state coaching on communication apprehension (H1)
CT and SPL groups did not differ in their PRCA-24 scores (CT: M = 82.3, SD = 15.1; SPL: M = 80.6, SD = 15.2), t(258) = −0.90, p = 0.37, nor in their SUD values (CT: M = −6.8, SD = 2.4; SPL: M = −6.8, SD = 1.8), t(258) = −0.04, p = 0.97, in relation to the measured CA.
For CA, measured by PRCA-24, the main factor time was significant for CA, F(2, 258.3) = 242.14, p < 0.001, indicating differences in CA between the measuring points in time. CA decreased in both groups after the ego-state intervention (pre: M = 80.7; post: M = 63.4; follow-up: M = 62.3).
Pairwise comparisons showed that the reduction in CA was significant in relation to the estimated marginal means for the pre-post comparison (MDiff = 17.3, p < 0.001), but not for the comparison between post-measurement and follow-up (MDiff = 1.1, p = 0.07). Figure 5 shows the changes in the estimated marginal means for CA measured with PRCA-24 across the three measurement points (CT: pre = 81.3, post = 62.7, follow-up = 61.7; SPL: pre = 80.1, post = 64.1, follow-up = 62.9).

Figure 5. Change in communication apprehension (PRCA-24) over time. CA, Communication Apprehension; ***p-value < 0.001, ns, non-significant (p-value > 0.05).
There was no significant difference in CA between CT and SPL, F(1, 254) = 0.09, p = 0.76. The pairwise comparisons between the groups at showed no significant differences: pre-measurement (MDiff = −1.1; p = 0.54), post-measurement (MDiff = 1.4; p = 0.43), and follow-up (MDiff = 1.2; p = 0.53).
The shift in the distribution of subjects across the three categories of CA in the PRCA-24 showed the extent to which the ego-state interventions reduced CA. According to Woods (2007), scores of 80 to 120 are considered as high CA, while scores of 51–79 are categorized as moderate. PRCA-24 scores below 51 correspond to a low degree of CA. In the pre-test, 54.6% of the subjects (n = 142) were categorized with a high degree of CA, while 42.7% (n = 111) exhibited a moderate CA, and just 2.7% (n = 7) a low CA. After the ego-state intervention, the proportion of subjects with high CA decreased from the initial 54.6% (n = 142) to 15.8% (n = 41) in the post-measurement and to 11.9% (n = 29) in the follow-up. This means that 113 out of 142 subjects who had exhibited a high CA in the pre-measurement (this corresponds to a proportion of 80%) showed only a moderate or even low degree of CA in the follow-up instead of a high degree (see Appendix C, Figures C-1–C-3).
The main factor time was also significant for CA, measured by the bipolar scale of subjective feeling, F(2, 251.8) = 1,065.40, p < 0.001, indicating differences in CA between the measuring points in time. CA decreased in both groups after the ego-state intervention (pre: M = −6.7; post: M = 4.1; follow-up: M = 3.6). Pairwise comparisons showed that the reduction in CA was significant (MDiff = −10.7, p < 0.001). Comparison between post-measurement and follow-up was marginally non-significant (MDiff = 0.5, p = 0.05). However, the SUD values, which were clearly in the range of the subjective sense of resources (>0), only decreased minimally here, as can be seen from the mean difference. This corresponds to a consolidation of the subjective sense of resources.
Figure 6 shows the changes in the estimated marginal means for CA measured via the bipolar scale of subjective feeling across the three measurement points (CT: pre = −6.6, post = 4.4, follow-up = 3.9; SPL: pre = −6.7, post = 3.7, follow-up = 3.4).

Figure 6. Change in communication apprehension (bipolar scale of subjective feeling) over time. CA, Communication Apprehension; *** p-value < 0.001, ns, non-significant (p-value > 0.05).
The scores of the bipolar scale of subjective feeling between the CT and SPL groups did not differ significantly for CA, F(1, 250.6) = 2.80, p = 0.01. The pairwise comparisons between the groups at the three measurement points in time showed no significant differences: pre-measurement (MDiff = −0.1; p = 0.74), post-measurement (MDiff = 0.8; p = 0.08), and follow-up (MDiff = 0.5; p = 0.28).
3.2.2 Self-transcendence as a possible mechanism of change (H2)
The CT ego-state intervention (M = 126.8, SD = 24.5, n = 133) resulted in an increased experience of a self-transcendent state compared to SPL (M = 118.1, SD = 31.8, n = 127), t(237) = −2.48, p = 0.007. The effect size, according to Cohen (1988), was d = 0.28, which corresponds to a small effect.
The largest difference within the AWE-S subscales was found for connectedness (CT: M = 24.5, SD = 6.7; SPL: M = 21.2, SD = 8.0), t(245.5) = −3.57, p < 0.001. With d = 0.41, the effect size corresponds to a small to moderate effect. The subscale vastness showed a significant difference with a small effect (CT: M = 24.8, SD = 7.2; SPL: M = 22.8, SD = 8.2), t(249.9) = −2.10, p = 0.02, d = 0.24. The third subscale that showed a significant difference, albeit with just a small effect, was the altered time perception subfacet (CT: M = 22.2, SD = 6.5; SPL: M = 20.5, SD = 8.1), t(241.6) = −1.82, p = 0.04, d = 0.20. The other three subscales of the AWE-S (self-diminishment, p = 0.25; physical sensations, p = 0.26; need for accommodation, p = 0.20) did not demonstrate any significant difference and are therefore not analyzed in more detail here.
A correlation analysis showed a significant positive correlation between the strength of experienced self-transcendence and the reduction of CA, r = 0.28, p < 0.001. According to Cohen (1988), this corresponds to a small effect. For subscales of the AWE-S, the correlations between the intensity of the feeling of the respective self-transcendence subfacet and the reduction of CA are as follows in order of their strength (see Table 5).
3.2.3 Moderator effect of “openness to experience” (H3)
The personality trait “openness to experiences” significantly influenced experienced self-transcendence in both groups, F(1, 253) = 5.77, p = 0.02. According to Cohen (1988), the effect size was r = 0.14, p = 0.01, which corresponds to a small effect. The interaction effect Group x Openness was not significant, F(1, 253) = 0.61, p = 0.43.
4 Discussion
4.1 Interpretation of results
The study's results demonstrate that an online ego-state coaching with emTrace, lasting on average 80 min, can reduce communication apprehension. Thus, the first hypothesis was confirmed. Two weeks after the intervention, the reduction in communication apprehension remained stable.
We hypothesized that one of the possible mechanisms of change is that the ego-state intervention activates a self-transcendent state. We expected that the higher the self-transcendence induced by the intervention, the greater the reduction in communication apprehension. But although the activation of a self-transcendent state was increased to a greater extent by Core Transformation than Smart Part Lab (H2a), contrary to the second hypothesis, this did not result in an increased reduction in communication apprehension. Both ego-state interventions reduced communication apprehension to the same extent. Thus, the second hypothesis could not be confirmed. The results indicate that the activation of a self-transcendent state seems not to be the mechanism of change for reducing communication apprehension.
The third hypothesis was fully confirmed: The greater the degree of openness in the client's personality, the more intensively the intervention could activate a self-transcendent state. Except for the second main hypothesis, the results confirmed all the initial assumptions.
In relation to the first hypothesis, it could be shown that the effect of communication apprehension reduction seems to be large. Eight percent of the subjects who showed a high level of communication apprehension before the coaching showed only a moderate or low degree of communication apprehension in the follow-up. The effect of the ego-state intervention was, therefore, both effective and sustainable over a period of 2 weeks after the coaching. This effect can be considered remarkable, as communication apprehension is defined as trait anxiety according to McCroskey (1977). The PRCA-24 does not just measure anxiety in a specific situation (such as an exam or presentation) but across four contexts (public speaking, group discussion, meetings, dyadic conversations). This means that the subjects' communication apprehension was seemingly not only reduced in relation to the situation focused on in the coaching, but a positive effect was found across all contexts.
For the second hypothesis, the question arises as to why the Core Transformation, contrary to the second hypothesis, did not reduce communication apprehension to a greater extent than the Smart Part Lab, and this despite (a) a more intensive activation of self-transcendence and (b) the correlation between self-transcendence and reduction of communication apprehension.
Other mechanisms besides self-transcendence may reduce communication apprehension, but these were not measured in the present study. These alternative mechanisms may explain the positive effect of Smart Part Lab. Some possible alternative mechanisms of change are listed here as examples. Mechanisms that both ego-state interventions share are, for example, (a) self-distancing (cf. process goal 1): Studies have not only shown that self-distancing—as achieved in both ego-state interventions through the dissociated perception of the ego-state—can regulate stressful feelings, but can also increase the sense of resources (see e.g., Kross and Ayduk, 2017; Dorfman et al., 2019); (b) emotion regulation of stressful feelings through bifocal processing (cf. process goals 3 and 5): Nachreiner (2020) has already demonstrated that bifocal processing as part of emTrace coaching can effectively reduce test anxiety; (c) positive reappraisal of communication apprehension (cf. process goal 4): By exploring the positive intention of the ego-state, a positive reappraisal of the communication apprehension takes place. This cognitive coping strategy changes the subjective perspective regarding a situation that the person experiences as stressful in such a way that it is perceived as valuable. It thus changes the emotional valence: the person experiences pleasant feelings instead of unpleasant ones (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000).
Also worth mentioning are common factors, such as the working alliance between coach and client (see Grawe, 2004). In addition, there are potential mechanisms of change specific to Smart Part Lab, e.g., the imagination of a creative ego-state could reduce communication apprehension. For example, Tabrizi and Yaacob (2011) showed that creative thinking can help reduce anxiety.
The third hypothesis, that there is a correlation between openness to experiences in the client's personality and the activation of a self-transcendent state, was confirmed. However, the effect size was very small; therefore, this finding has no practical meaning.
4.2 Implications
4.2.1 Theoretical implications
The results show that a single intervention can not only reduce stressful emotional states, such as test anxiety (e.g., Nachreiner, 2020), but the change can also address cross-contextual patterns of emotional stress (here, communication apprehension as trait anxiety). Especially for people who do not believe such a change is possible, the information about the study results can foster a growth mindset, which reflects the attitude that personal characteristics can be developed (e.g., Yeager and Dweck, 2020). In particular, a growth mindset regarding the changeability of anxiety correlates positively with an emotionally resilient response in relation to stressful life events (Schroder et al., 2017).
The falsification of the second main hypothesis shows that the activation of self-transcendence does not seem to be a mechanism of change in ego-state coaching for reducing communication apprehension. Despite an increased activation of self-transcendence by Core Transformation, it did not reduce communication apprehension more than the Smart Part Lab. This finding contrasts with previous research emphasizing the positive effects of self-transcendent states on emotional resilience and cognitive flexibility. For instance, self-transcendent states have been shown to reduce ruminative thinking and repetitive problem-focused thought patterns (e.g., Tarani, 2017). Moreover, eliciting self-transcendent emotions such as awe has been found to buffer individuals against the negative impact of feedback (Atamba, 2019). Experimental studies further demonstrate that awe can shift individuals' perception of upcoming challenges, making them appear less threatening or overwhelming (Le et al., 2019). In addition, activating self-transcendent states has been linked to greater creative thinking and increased openness to experience (Chirico et al., 2018; Stancato and Keltner, 2019). Thus, the Zen Buddhist axiom “no self, no problem” seems to resonate with the theoretical perspective, yet may not translate directly into measurable reductions in communication apprehension within short-term coaching interventions. One possible explanation is that the positive effects of self-transcendence may be more diffuse, general, or long-term in nature and therefore not directly reflected in trait-like constructs such as communication apprehension after a single intervention. Alternatively, the mechanisms by which self-transcendent states exert their influence may not align closely with the specific emotional and cognitive patterns that sustain communication apprehension.
4.2.2 Practical implications
The finding that an online ego-state-intervention with emTrace can reduce communication apprehension effectively, gives emotion coaches a secure basis for offering online coaching, particularly in relation to reducing communication apprehension. The average coaching duration of around 80 min provides an orientation as to how much time should be planned for a mean coaching session with an ego-state intervention.
The results also show the effectiveness of online seminars. This is because the emotion coaches who participated in this study were all trained in relation to the conducted ego-state interventions in a 4-day emTrace online seminar. The theory was also taught via a specially developed interactive online training course. This shows that learning coaching interventions in an online setting is possible.
The confirmation of the first hypothesis, combined with the rejection of the second main hypothesis, gives coaches more freedom and flexibility in their choice of intervention. Since Core Transformation and Smart Part Lab did not differ in their effect on reducing communication apprehension, the coach is free to choose which ego-state intervention to use in practice. This shows that it seems to make more sense as a coach to think in terms of process goals of ego-state coaching than in terms of concrete ego-state interventions, which are fixed in their steps.
4.3 Limitations
Regarding the internal validity of the study, it should be positively emphasized that communication apprehension was measured with two different scales (PRCA-24 and SUD values), each of which has a different perspective on the variable. From the perspective of both scales, it was confirmed that ego-state coaching reduces communication apprehension. The SUD values measured showed that, on average, clients not only felt less stress about the anxiety-inducing situation they chose for the intervention but also rated the situation more positively and associated it with pleasant feelings.
What may also have heightened the internal validity is that the coaches were not informed about the hypotheses before conducting the coaching. This is particularly important for the second hypothesis. Because it means that the coaches did not know that the Core Transformation was expected to reduce communication apprehension to a greater extent than the Smart Part Lab. In this way, expectation effects of a different strength of the respective intervention could be reduced. This is because the coach's expectation of the intervention can also have an influence on the client's emotional change (e.g., Rosenthal and Jacobson, 1968).
One limitation of the study could be that it was not assessed how reliably the coaches adhered to the specific intervention process descriptions despite the precautions taken (e.g., detailed intervention procedures, online training in advance). This could have led to a “mixing” of the intervention process descriptions without being noticed. The intervention adherence should be investigated in further studies using two short questions after the coaching conduction: “Did you stick to the process description? At which points did you deviate from the standard process?”
All scales used in the present study showed good to excellent internal consistency. Only the openness scale of the B5T was at the lower limit of acceptable reliability. In a further study, alternative questionnaires should be used that show higher internal consistency, such as the openness scale of the NEO-PI-R with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.89 (Berth and Goldschmidt, 2006).
In relation to external validity, the high number of subjects who participated in this study should be emphasized. This increases the generalizability of the results to the focused population of female and male people in German-speaking countries who suffer from communication apprehension and are willing to consult a coach to reduce their communication apprehension. The fact that the study was a field experiment also improves the external validity.
It should be critically noted that more women than men took part in the study—both among the coaches (64.2% female) and among the clients (77.3% female). In principle, this could limit the generalizability of the results for male clients or coaches, but generally reflects the gender structure of the coaching market (Rauen, 2020). For this reason, it would have been difficult to realize an equal distribution of clients and coaches in relation to gender. This would have been at the detriment of both the number of test subjects and the number of participating coaches.
Another limitation relates to the educational background of the sample. The participants in the present study were predominantly highly educated, which raises the question of whether the intervention effects can be generalized to populations with lower educational attainment. Both ego-state interventions—Core Transformation and Smart Part Lab—require a certain degree of introspective ability, abstract thinking, and cognitive engagement. It is conceivable that individuals with lower levels of formal education, or those less familiar with inner imagery and reflective dialogue, might find it more difficult to fully access and benefit from these interventions. Future studies should therefore investigate whether and how the effectiveness of ego-state coaching varies as a function of educational background or cognitive style. This could be addressed by including a more diverse sample and by exploring potential adaptations of the intervention procedures for broader accessibility.
4.4 Outlook and conclusion
Future research could investigate the long-term stability of the observed effects beyond the 2-week follow-up period. Since communication apprehension is considered a relatively stable trait, it would be valuable to examine whether ego-state coaching can produce sustained changes in communication behavior over several months or in real-life performance contexts.
In addition, further studies could focus on identifying which process components of ego-state coaching are most relevant for reducing communication apprehension. By comparing different process elements (e.g., self-distancing, bifocal processing, or positive reappraisal) across interventions, future research could shed light on the specific active ingredients that contribute to change.
Finally, subsequent studies could systematically explore how individual characteristics, such as educational background or cognitive style, influence the effectiveness of ego-state interventions. This would help to further tailor coaching methods to different target groups and improve accessibility, especially for clients with limited experience in introspective or abstract thinking.
To summarize, the results of the present study have shown that ego-state online coaching with emTrace can effectively reduce a person's communication apprehension. In this way, these findings strengthen the empirical foundation for the use of emotion-focused interventions in coaching practice and emphasize their potential to empower individuals who experience communication apprehension to engage more openly and self-confidently in interpersonal situations. As communication apprehension affects not only communicative effectiveness but also psychological wellbeing and social connectedness, the ability to reduce it through low-threshold coaching formats may offer substantial benefits, not only for individuals but also from a broader social and public health perspective.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional Ethics Committee of the Euro-FH, University of Applied Sciences, Hamburg, Germany (protocol code EKEFHS07/24). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
DE: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PM: Project administration, Software, Writing – review & editing. MS: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the emTrace emotion coaches involved in the study who conducted the individual sessions with the participants. The Eilert Academy supported P.M.’s involvement by providing time for the project.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Correction note
31/07/2025 A correction has been made to this article. Details can be found at: 10.3389/forgp.2025.1487283.
01/08/2025 This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the scientific content of the article.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/forgp.2025.1487283/full#supplementary-material
References
Agrawal, S., and Krishna, S. M. (2021). Communication apprehension and psychological wellbeing of students in online learning. Behav. Sci. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.3390/bs11110145
Alladin, A. (2013). Healing the wounded self: combining hypnotherapy with ego state therapy. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 56, 3–22. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2013.796282
Atamba, C. (2019). Restorative effects of awe on negative affect after receiving negative performance feedback. J. Psychol. Afr. 29, 95–103. doi: 10.1080/14330237.2019.1594640
Bandler, R., and Grinder, J. (1982). Reframing—Neuro-Linguistic Programming and the Transformation of Meaning. Moab, Utah, USA: Real People Press.
Barabasz, A. (2013). Evidence Based Abreactive Ego State Therapy for PTSD. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 56, 54–65. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2013.770384
Barabasz, A., Barabasz, M., Christensen, C., French, B., and Watkins, J. G. (2013). Efficacy of single-session abreactive ego state therapy for combat stress injury, PTSD, and ASD. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 61, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/00207144.2013.729377
Berth, H., and Goldschmidt, S. (2006). NEO-PI-R: NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar nach Costa und McCrae, revidierte Fassung (Testinformation). Diagnostica 52, 95–99. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.52.2.95
Braganza, D., and Piedmont, R. L. (2015). The impact of the core transformation process on spirituality, symptom experience, and psychological maturity in a mixed age sample in India: a pilot study. J. Relig. Health 54, 888–902. doi: 10.1007/s10943-015-0049-y
Braganza, D., Piedmont, R. L., Fox, J., Fialkowski, G. M., and Gray, R. M. (2019). Examining the clinical efficacy of core transformation: a randomized clinical trial. J. Counsel. Dev. 97, 293–305. doi: 10.1002/jcad.12269
Carlucci, L., Saggino, A., and Balsamo, M. (2021). On the efficacy of the unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 87:101999. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101999
Chirico, A., Glaveanu, V. P., Cipresso, P., Riva, G., and Gaggioli, A. (2018). Awe enhances creative thinking: an experimental study. Creat. Res. J. 30, 123–131. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2018.1446491
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Corrigan, F., and Grand, D. (2013). Brainspotting: recruiting the midbrain for accessing and healing sensorimotor memories of traumatic activation. Med. Hypotheses 80, 759–766. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2013.03.005
Croucher, S. M., Kelly, S., Nguyen, T., Rocker, K., Yotes, T., Cullinane, J., et al. (2023). A longitudinal analysis of communication traits: communication apprehension, willingness to communicate, and self-perceived communication competence. Commun. Q. 72, 99–119. doi: 10.1080/01463373.2023.2292216
Cuijpers, P., Reijnders, M., and Huibers, M. J. H. (2019). The role of common factors in psychotherapy outcomes. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 15, 207–231. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095424
Daharnis, D., Ifdil, I., Amalianita, B., Zola, N., and Putri, Y. E. (2021). The effectiveness of ego-state therapy in reducing trypanophobia. Addict. Disord. Their Treat. 20, 61–65. doi: 10.1097/ADT.0000000000000210
de Araújo, D. C. S. A., Rocha, K. S. S., Cerqueira-Santos, S., dos Santos Menezes, P. W., Dos Santos, S. N. P, Dos Santos, W. M., et al. (2022). Communication apprehension among health professions students in Brazil. Am. J. Pharm. Educ. 86:8603. doi: 10.5688/ajpe8603
de Voogd, L. D., Hermans, E. J., and Phelps, E. A. (2018). Regulating defensive survival circuits through cognitive demand via large-scale network reorganization. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 24, 124–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.009
Dorfman, A., Oakes, H., Santos, H. C., and Grossmann, I. (2019). Self-distancing promotes positive emotional change after adversity: evidence from a micro-longitudinal field experiment. J. Pers. 89, 132–144. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/k4zfv
Eilert, D. W. (2021). Integratives Emotionscoaching mit emTrace: Wie emotionale Veränderung wirklich gelingt. Paderborn: Junfermann.
Eilert, D. W. (2023). Integratives Ego-State-Coaching mit emTrace: Emotionscoaching mit Persönlichkeitsanteilen. Paderborn: Junfermann.
Engelhard, I. M., van den Hout, M. A., and Smeets, M. A. (2011). Taxing working memory reduces vividness and emotional intensity of images about the Queen's Day tragedy. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiat. 42, 32–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.09.004
Folkman, S., and Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Stress, positive emotion, and coping. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 9, 115–118. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00073
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., and Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLOS Med. 7:e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316
Kim, D., Bae, H., and Chon Park, Y. (2008). Validity of the Subjective Units of Disturbance Scale in EMDR. J. EMDR Pract. Res. 2, 57–62. doi: 10.1891/1933-3196.2.1.57
Kross, E., and Ayduk, O. (2017). “Self-distancing: theory, research, and current directions,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Cambridge, USA: Academic Press), 81–136.
Le, P. Q., Saltsman, T. L., Seery, M. D., Ward, D. E., Kondrak, C. L., Lamarche, V. M., et al. (2019). When a small self means manageable obstacles: spontaneous self-distancing predicts divergent effects of awe during a subsequent performance stressor. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 80, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.07.010
Marcel, M. (2019). Communication apprehension across the career span. Int. J. Bus. Commun. 59, 506–530. doi: 10.1177/2329488419856803
Maruskin, L. A., Thrash, T. M., and Elliot, A. J. (2012). The chills as a psychological construct: content universe, factor structure, affective composition, elicitors, trait antecedents, and consequences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 135–157. doi: 10.1037/a0028117
McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (1992). Discriminant Validity of NEO-PIR Facet Scales. Educ. Psychol. Measure. 52, 229–237. doi: 10.1177/001316449205200128
McCroskey, J. C. (1977). Oral communication apprehension: a summary of recent theory and research. Human Commun. Res. 4, 78–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1977.tb00599.x
McCroskey, J. C. (1978). Validity of the PRCA as an index of oral communication apprehension. Commun. Monogr. 45, 192–203. doi: 10.1080/03637757809375965
McNeal, S. (2007). Healthy narcissism and ego state therapy. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 56, 19–36. doi: 10.1080/00207140701672987
McNeil, G. D., and Repetti, R. L. (2022). Increases in positive emotions as precursors to therapeutic change. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 29, 1113–1124. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2698
Montreuil, M., and Pedinielli, J. L. (1995). Parallel visual processing characteristics in healthy alexithymic subjects. Administration of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale and the parallel visual information test. L'Encephale 21, 589–595.
Nachreiner, L. (2020). Die Relevanz von ressourcen- und problemorientierten Interventionen für den Coachingerfolg. M.Sc. Master Thesis, Rheinische Fachhochschule Köln.
Nelms, J. A., and Castel, L. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized and nonrandomized trials of clinical emotional freedom techniques (EFT) for the treatment of depression. Explore 12, 416–426. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2016.08.001
Niebauer, C. (2019). No Self, No Problem: How Psychology and Brain Science Are Catching Up Buddhism. San Antonio, Tx: Hierophant Publishing.
Nye, A., Delgadillo, J., and Barkham, M. (2023). Efficacy of personalized psychological interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 91, 389–397. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000820
Panchal, S., and Riddell, P. (2020). The GROWS model: extending the GROW coaching model to support behavioural change. Coach. Psychol. 16, 12–25. doi: 10.53841/bpstcp.2020.16.2.12
Phillips, M. (2013). Mending fences: repairing boundaries through ego state therapy. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 56, 23–38. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2013.789431
Rauen, C. (2020). RAUEN Coaching-Marktanalyse 2020. Available: https://www.rauen.de/cma/ (Accessed August 01, 2024).
Rosenthal, R., and Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. Urban Rev. 3, 16–20. doi: 10.1007/BF02322211
Saccaro, L. F., Giff, A., De Rossi, M. M., and Piguet, C. (2024). Interventions targeting emotion regulation: a systematic umbrella review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 174, 263–274. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2024.04.025
Sakiris, N., and Berle, D. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the Unified Protocol as a transdiagnostic emotion regulation based intervention. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 72:101751. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101751
Satow, L. (2012a). Big-Five-Persönlichkeitstest (B5T): Test- und Skalendokumentation. Available: http://www.drsatow.de (Accessed August 01, 2024).
Satow, L. (2012b). Big-Five-Persönlichkeitstest (B5T): Testmanual und Normen. Available: http://www.drsatow.de (Accessed August 01, 2024).
Satow, L. (2021). “Validierung und Neunormierung des Big-Five-Persönlichkeitstests (B5T),” in PersonalEntwickeln (267. Erg.-Lfg., Juli 2021, S. 1-25), eds. S. Laske, A. Orthey and M.J. Schmid (Köln: Wolters Kluwer).
Schroder, H. S., Yalch, M. M., Dawood, S., Callahan, C. P., Brent Donnellan, M., Moser, J. S., et al. (2017). Growth mindset of anxiety buffers the link between stressful life events and psychological distress and coping strategies. Pers. Individ. Diff. 110, 23–26. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.01.016
Schulenberg, S., Oh, K. M., Goldberg, D. G., and Kreps, G. L. (2023). Exploring communication apprehension in nursing and healthcare education: a scoping review. Nurs. Health Sci. 25, 543–555. doi: 10.1111/nhs.13064
Shiota, M. N., Campos, B., and Keltner, D. (2003). The faces of positive emotion: prototype displays of awe, amusement, and pride. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1000, 296–299. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.029
Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., and John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style. J. Positive Psychol. 1, 61–71. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510833
Spiegel, E. B. (2016). Attachment-focused psychotherapy and the wounded self. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 59, 47–68. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2016.1163658
Stancato, D. M., and Keltner, D. (2019). Awe, ideological conviction, and perceptions of ideological opponents. Emotion. 21, 61–72. doi: 10.1037/emo0000665
Tabrizi, E. A., and Yaacob, S. (2011). Relationship between creative thinking and anxiety among adolescent boys and girls in Tehran, Iran. Int. J. Human. Soc. Sci. 1, 60–66.
Tarani, E. (2017). Affective and cognitive effects of awe in predicting hopelessness and brooding rumination. (master thesis). San José State University.
Urgesi, C., Aglioti, S. M., Skrap, M., and Fabbro, F. (2010). The spiritual brain: selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence. Neuron 65, 309–319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.026
van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Smeets, M. A., Hornsveld, H., Hoogeveen, E., de Heer, E., et al. (2010). Counting during recall: taxing of working memory and reduced vividness and emotionality of negative memories. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 303–311. doi: 10.1002/acp.1677
van Elk, M., Arciniegas Gomez, M. A., van der Zwaag, W., van Schie, H. T., and Sauter, D. (2019). The neural correlates of the awe experience: reduced default mode network activity during feelings of awe. Human Brain Mapp. 40, 3561–3574. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24616
Watkins, J. G., and Watkins, H. H. (1979). “The theory and practice of ego state therapy,” in Short-Term Approaches to Psychotherapy, ed. H. Grayson (New York, NY: Human Sciences), 176–220.
Watkins, J. G., and Watkins, H. H. (2019). Ego-States - Theorie und Therapie. Ein Handbuch. Heidelberg: Carl-Auer.
Wells, R., Outhred, T., Heathers, J. A., Quintana, D. S., and Kemp, A. H. (2012). Matter over mind: a randomised-controlled trial of single-session biofeedback training on performance anxiety and heart rate variability in musicians. PLoS ONE 7:e46597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046597
Wittfoth, D., Pfeiffer, A., Bohne, M., Lanfermann, H., and Wittfoth, M. (2020). Emotion regulation through bifocal processing of fear inducing and disgust inducing stimuli. BMC Neurosci. 21:47. doi: 10.1186/s12868-020-00597-x
Woods, R. (2007). “Personal Report of Communication Apprhension,” in Handbook of Research on Electronic Surveys and Measurements, eds. A. R. Rodney, W. Robert and D. B. Jason. (Hershey, PA, USA: IGI Global), 320–322.
Yaden, D. B., Kaufman, S. B., Hyde, E., Chirico, A., Gaggioli, A., Zhang, J. W., et al. (2018). The development of the Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S): a multifactorial measure for a complex emotion. J. Positive Psychol. 14, 474–488. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2018.1484940
Yeager, D. S., and Dweck, C. S. (2020). What can be learned from growth mindset controversies? Am. Psychol. 75, 1269–1284. doi: 10.1037/amp0000794
Keywords: ego-state coaching, emTrace, communication apprehension, online coaching, self-transcendence, emotion coaching, emotion-focused intervention
Citation: Eilert DW, Mensah P and Schmäh M (2025) Emotion-focused ego-state coaching reduces communication apprehension: evidence from a randomized controlled study. Front. Organ. Psychol. 3:1487283. doi: 10.3389/forgp.2025.1487283
Received: 01 October 2024; Accepted: 14 July 2025;
Published: 30 July 2025; Corrected: 01 August 2025.
Edited by:
Shaozhuang Ma, University Institute of Lisbon, PortugalReviewed by:
Miriam Yates, The University of Queensland, AustraliaAlex Lazarus, Lazarus and Maverick Limited, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2025 Eilert, Mensah and Schmäh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Dirk W. Eilert, ZGlyay5laWxlcnRAZWlsZXJ0LWFrYWRlbWllLmRl