Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 13 September 2019
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Sensory Adaptation View all 15 articles

Global Jitter Motion of the Retinal Image Dynamically Alters the Receptive Field Properties of Retinal Ganglion Cells

Updated
  • 1Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Humanities and Sociology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Ritsumeikan Global Innovation Research Organization (R-GIRO), Ritsumeikan University, Kusatsu, Japan
  • 3Danish Research Institute of Translational Neuroscience (DANDRITE), Department of Biomedicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 4Research Organization of Science and Technology, Ritsumeikan University, Kusatsu, Japan

Fixational eye movements induce aperiodic motion of the retinal image. However, it is not yet fully understood how fixational eye movements affect retinal information processing. Here we show that global jitter motion, simulating the image motion during fixation, alters the spatiotemporal receptive field properties of retinal ganglion cells. Using multi-electrode and whole-cell recording techniques, we investigated light-evoked responses from ganglion cells in the isolated goldfish retina. Ganglion cells were classified into six groups based on the filtering property of light stimulus, the membrane properties, and the cell morphology. The spatiotemporal receptive field profiles of retinal ganglion cells were estimated by the reverse correlation method, where the dense noise stimulus was applied on the dark or random-dot background. We found that the jitter motion of the random-dot background elongated the receptive filed along the rostral-caudal axis and temporally sensitized in a specific group of ganglion cells: Fast-transient ganglion cells. At the newly emerged regions of the receptive field local light stimulation evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents with large amplitude and fast kinetics without changing the properties of inhibitory postsynaptic currents. Pharmacological experiments suggested two presynaptic mechanisms underlying the receptive field alteration: (i) electrical coupling between bipolar cells, which expands the receptive field in all directions; (ii) GABAergic presynaptic inhibition from amacrine cells, which reduces the dorsal and ventral regions of the expanded receptive field, resulting in elongation along the rostral-caudal axis. Our study demonstrates that the receptive field of Fast-transient ganglion cells is not static but dynamically altered depending on the visual inputs. The receptive field elongation during fixational eye movements may contribute to prompt firing to a target in the succeeding saccade.

Introduction

In living animals, the retina receives unstable visual inputs induced by movements of body, head, and eyes (Land, 2009). Even when an animal is fixating an object, the whole image on the retina is shifted by the presence of incessant microscopic eye movements (“fixational eye movements”) (Martinez-Conde et al., 2004; Rucci and Poletti, 2015). Once fixational eye movements are stabilized, visual perception fades rapidly (Yarbus, 1967; Murakami, 2006). It has been shown that fixational eye movements affect visual performance such as visual acuity (Keesey, 1960), contrast sensitivity (Tulunay-Keesey and Jones, 1976), and detection of visual features (Rucci et al., 2007). Physiological evidence indicates that the image motion induced by fixational eye movements prevents the adaptation of neural activity (Martinez-Conde et al., 2004; Rucci and Poletti, 2015). Involuntary small eye movements during fixation, called microsaccades, increase firing activity of primate visual cortical neurons (V1, Martinez-Conde et al., 2000; area MT, Bair and O’Keefe, 1998). Retinal ganglion cells (GCs) respond with higher firing rate to the jittering image than the static image, together with synchronization (Greschner et al., 2002) or with decorrelation (Segal et al., 2015).

In natural vision, animals repeat fixations and brief gaze shifts (saccades) (Wurtz, 2008; Land, 2009). It has been shown that firing of GCs is increased (Noda and Adey, 1974) or suppressed (Roska and Werblin, 2003; Tien et al., 2015) by saccade-like image shift depending on cell types. A burst firing is evoked by image recurrence across eye-movement-like image transitions in mouse specific GCs (Krishnamoorthy et al., 2017). Glycinergic and GABAergic inhibitory inputs from amacrine cells seem to contribute to the firing modulation during the rapid image shift (Roska and Werblin, 2003; Tien et al., 2015; Krishnamoorthy et al., 2017).

In our previous study, applying a multi-electrode array to GCs in the goldfish isolated retina, we showed that firing properties of specific GC groups were modulated by a rapid shift of a target following a period of jitter motion of a global random-dot background (Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). In particular, the response latency to a rapidly moving target was shortened in Fast-transient (Ft) GCs only when rapid motion was preceded by global jitter motion. Intriguingly, the response modulation was specific to rapid motion along the rostral-caudal axis. These results suggest that the receptive field (RF) properties of Ft GCs may have been altered during a period of global jitter motion prior to rapid motion. However, global jitter motion per se did not evoke firing in Ft GCs, and thus, it remains to be solved how global jitter motion alters the RF properties of Ft GCs and what mechanisms underlie the alteration.

Here, applying the whole-cell clamp technique as well as the multi-electrode technique to goldfish retinal GCs, we analyzed the effects of global jitter motion on the spatiotemporal RF profiles. We found that the RF of Ft GCs was spatially elongated along the rostral-caudal axis and temporally sensitized by jitter motion. At the newly emerged regions, local light stimulation frequently evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents with large amplitude and fast kinetics. Pharmacological experiments suggested that the RF alterations were mediated by activation of electrical coupling between bipolar cells and GABAergic inhibition from amacrine cells to bipolar cell terminals. Elongation of the RF of Ft GCs during jitter motion may contribute to prompt response to a rapidly moving target in the succeeding saccade.

Materials and Methods

Experimental Model and Subject Details

Goldfish (Carassius auratus; 8–12 cm; n = 62) was used for the experiments. Animals were kept in a room maintained at 23°C on a 12 h light/dark cycle. All protocols complied with “A Manual for the Conduct of Animal Experiments in The University of Tokyo” and “Guiding Principles for the Care and Use of Animals in the Field of Physiological Sciences, The Physiological Society of Japan.”

Method Details

Retinal Preparation

Goldfish were dark-adapted for more than 1 h before experiments. Under a dim red light, a goldfish was double-pithed, and eyes were enucleated. The following procedure was performed under a stereomicroscope equipped with infrared (IR) image converter (C5100, Hamamatsu photonics) and IR illuminator (HVL-IRM, Sony). After the cornea and lens were ablated, the eye cup was treated with a mixture of hyaluronidase and collagenase (4 mg/mL each, Sigma-Aldrich Corp.) for a few min. A small cut was made at the dorsal part of the eye cup as a landmark and thus the ventral retina isolated from the pigment epithelium was properly oriented and positioned on the multi-electrode array or in the recording chamber for whole-cell recordings.

Recordings

For multi-electrode recordings (Figures 1, 2), the isolated retina was placed on the electrode array (60 electrodes, electrode diameter 30 μm, electrode spacing 200 μm; 60pMEA200/30iR-Ti, Multichannel Systems) with the GC layer facing down, and light stimulation was applied from the photoreceptor side. The recorded signals were stored at 16 kHz through AD converter with 16 channels (PowerLab 16/35; AD Instruments). The retina was continuously superfused with an extracellular solution bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2 at the rate of 1 mL/min. The solution consisted of (in mM) 106 NaCl, 2.6 KCl, 28 NaHCO3, 2.5 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 1 Na-pyruvate, 10 D-glucose, 4 mg/L phenol red. For pharmacological experiments, drugs were added to the extracellular solution. Drugs were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich. Recorded spike discharges were band-pass filtered between 100 and 3,000 Hz and sorted into single unit activities by principal component analysis (PCA) and the template-matching method with custom programs using MATLAB (Lewicki, 1998; Zhang et al., 2004; Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). For further analysis, we selected up to 3 single units/electrode, showing robust light-evoked responses based on two criteria: responsibility and reliability. Responsibility was evaluated by comparing mean firing probability before and during light stimulus using two-tailed t-test. Reliability was evaluated by trial-to-trial variability in spike counts during light stimulus based on Pearson’s correlation. To verify the accuracy of sorting, we calculated the auto-correlation of the sorted spike train for each unit, and confirmed a lack of events corresponding to the refractory period of spikes (Supplementary Figure S1A) (Lewicki, 1998; Rey et al., 2015).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cell type classification based on the physiological and anatomical features. (A) Principal component analysis (PCA) of the temporal RF profile. x, centroid of each cluster. Inset, relationship between mean silhouette score and the number of clusters in k-means clustering. Red, the estimated optimal number of clusters. (B) The temporal RF profile of each GC group. Black vertical line, spike timing. Gray, individual cells. Black, mean. 20 Fast-transient (Ft), 18 Fast-sustained (Fs), 22 Medium-transient (Mt), 22 Medium-sustained (Ms), 18 Slow-transient (St), 27 Slow-sustained (Ss) GCs. (C,D) Histograms of the peak time panel (C) and the area of temporal RF profile panel (D) in each group. Gray dots, individual cells. (E) The physiological and morphological features used for a hierarchical clustering panel (F). (F) Top, dendrogram based on the hierarchical clustering. Colored circles, identity of GC groups based on the filtering property panel (A) of individual cells. Bottom, a matrix of correlation of five features between individual cells. Red square, correlation within identified six types (type I –VI). 53 GCs. (G) Examples of cell morphology (left) and membrane potential changes induced by current pulses (right, –50 and +80 pA in amplitudes, 200 ms in duration) for each type panel (F). (H) Fraction of GC groups included in each type. ∗∗∗p < 0.001. See also Supplementary Figure S1.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Global jitter motion alters the RF profiles of Fast-transient GCs. (A) A schematic of stimulus. Dense noise stimulus (orange, 1.6 × 1.6 or 0.96 × 0.96 mm) was presented on the random-dot background (4 × 4 mm, mean contrast; 45.6%, mean luminance; 6.67 cd/m2). The random-dot background was moved in four cardinal directions randomly (4 μm/50 ms; Jittered BG). (B) Examples of random-walk trajectory in the horizontal (blue) and vertical (red) directions. (C) 2D plots of the jitter motion trajectory (20 s). (D,E) Top, schematics of stimuli. Left, firing to a flash (gray band; size, 1 × 1 mm; duration, 2 s; 50% contrast increment) in Fast-transient (Ft) (D) and Fast-sustained (Fs) (E) GCs. Middle, the spatial RF profile under the Dark (gray), Static (black), and Jittered (blue) BG conditions. Right, the spatial and temporal RF profiles (upper, Ft GC; lower, Fs GC). (F,G) Expansion index panel (F) and temporal RF profile (G; upper, peak time; lower, peak amplitude): top, Static BG vs. Dark BG; bottom, Jittered BG vs. Dark BG. Fraction was calculated separately for Ft GCs (red, n = 17) and other GCs (gray, n = 73). (H) Top, a schematic for calculation of the RF orientation. The RF orientation was determined as a direction (red arrow) with the longest RF length (between × and blue dots). Middle; polar plot of the RF orientation under the Dark (gray), Static (black), and Jittered (blue) BG conditions for each GC group: 17 Ft GCs, 16 Fs GCs, 10 Medium-transient (Mt) GCs, 14 Medium-sustained (Ms) GCs, 14 Slow-transient (St) GCs, and 19 Slow-sustained (Ss) GCs. Bottom; cumulative fraction of the RF orientation. Red arrows; cumulative fraction at the horizontal axis (0 and 180 degree) under the Jittered BG condition. ∗∗∗p < 0.001. See also Supplementary Figure S1 for physiological and morphological features of each GC group, and Supplementary Figure S2 for visual stimulation.

For whole-cell recordings (Figures 1, 3, 4), the isolated retina was placed on the recording chamber with the GC layer facing up, and light stimulation was applied from the photoreceptor side. The retina was continuously superfused with the bubbled extracellular solution. The pipette was filled with intracellular solution (in mM): 128 K gluconate, 10 KCl, 10 Hepes, 0.5 EGTA, 0.05 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 5 ATP-Na2, 0.5 GTP-Na3, and 0.08% Lucifer yellow-2K (pH 7.4 with KOH) for current-clamp recordings, and 118 CsMeSO3, 10 TEA-Cl, 10 Hepes, 0.5 EGTA, 0.05 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 5 ATP-Na2, 0.5 GTP-Na3, 5 QX314-Br, and 0.08% Lucifer yellow-2K (pH 7.4 with CsOH) for voltage-clamp recordings. ECl was calculated as −55 mV. The membrane potential was corrected for liquid junction potential which was measured before experiments. For pharmacological experiments (Figure 5), we added picrotoxin (GABA receptor blocker; 100 μM, Sigma) or mefloquine (gap junction blocker; 10 μM, Sigma) to the extracellular solution. Recordings were performed using EPC 10 (HEKA Electronik) controlled by Pactchmaster (version 2.73.5). Current and voltage records were sampled at 16 kHz and low-pass filtered at 2.9 KHz. We used a borosilicate glass electrode (CNC 1.5; Ken Enterprise), which was pulled by a puller (P97; Sutter Instrument). The resistance of recording pipettes was 6–11 MΩ. For cell group identification, light-evoked spikes were recorded in the cell-attached mode before whole-cell recording.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Synaptic inputs to Fast-transient GCs. (A) Excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) evoked by dense noise stimulation under the Dark (upper, black) and Jittered (lower, blue) BG conditions. Left, a schematic of synaptic inputs to a Ft GC. Middle, a schematic of recordings and evoked EPSCs in a Ft GC voltage-clamped at –55 mV (Vh). Right; superimposed excitatory events, in which each color indicates different peak amplitude. (B) Plot of peak amplitude and rise time for each excitatory event under the Dark (gray dots) and Jittered (blue dots) BG conditions. Data from the Ft GC shown in panel (A). (C) Mean peak amplitude (left) and rise time (right) in individual cells (circle) under the Dark and Jittered BG conditions. Black line, mean. 5 Ft GCs. (D–F) Inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) recorded from Ft GCs voltage-clamped at 0 mV. 5 Ft GCs. ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001. See also Supplementary Figure S3.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Spatial map of EPSCs evoked by local flash stimulation in Ft GCs. (A) A schematic of sparse noise stimulation on the Dark (B) and Jittered BG (C). The sparse noise was a sequence of a static flash (size, 100 × 100 μm; duration, 50 ms) randomly applied at one of the 8 × 8 grids (gold dotted lines). Stimulus location was described from the RF center estimated under the Dark BG condition (red). (B,C) Superimposed EPSCs evoked by local flash stimulation presented on the Dark (B) and Jittered (C) BGs. EPSCs are colored based on the peak amplitude. #; the RF center. (D) Top, EPSCs evoked at three locations: [1] horizontally 200 μm away from the RF center, [2] the RF center, and [3] vertically 200 μm away from the RF center. Bottom, probability of EPSC events evoked by local flash stimulation. Numbers, EPSC events/stimulation. N/A, no evoked EPSCs. D, Dark BG. J, Jittered BG. (E) Heatmaps showing the peak amplitude of EPSCs (top) and IPSCs (bottom) evoked by local flash stimulation on the Dark (left) and Jittered (center) BGs. Right, a heatmap showing differences of amplitude between the Dark and Jittered BGs. Data from the Ft GC (C). (F) Left, peak amplitude (upper) and rise time (lower) of the evoked EPSCs are plotted against the position relative to the RF center (5 Ft GCs) under the Dark (black) and Jittered (red) BG conditions. Thin lines, data from individual cells. Right, mean peak amplitude (upper) and rise time (lower) of EPSCs evoked by flash stimulation 200 μm away from the RF center and at the RF center (0 μm). ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effects of pharmacological blockade of GABAergic inhibition and electrical coupling. (A) The spatial RF profile of a Ft GC under the Dark and Jittered BG conditions in control solution. (B) The spatial RF profile in the presence of picrotoxin (PTX, 100 μM, red) and mefloquine (MFQ, 10 μM, green). Left, the estimated RF under the Jittered BG condition. Right, the spatial RF profile along the vertical (dorsal–ventral) and horizontal (rostral–caudal) axes in control (black), PTX (red), and MFQ (green) solutions. (C) Left, a schematic for calculation of the horizontal and vertical lengths of the RF (blue). Right, horizontal and vertical lengths of the RF under the Jittered (blue) and Dark (gray) BG conditions in control (gray), PTX (red) and MFQ (green) solutions. 7 Ft GCs. (D) Spatial bias index (see section “Method Details”) under the Jittered (blue) and Dark (gray) BG conditions in control (gray), PTX (red), and MFQ (green) solutions. Circle and error bars, mean ± SD. 7 Ft GCs. (E) A schematic model to explain the horizontal RF elongation. A Ft GC receives excitatory inputs from distinct BC populations: conventional BCs (dark gray) which evoke small and slow EPSCs, and electrically coupled BCs (right green) which evoke large and fast EPSCs under the Jittered BG condition. BCs receive GABAergic inhibition (orange) from local ACs which are activated by local stimulation under the Dark BG condition. The electrically coupled BCs at the dorsal and ventral sides additionally receive global GABAergic inhibition (magenta) from wide-field (or electrically coupled) ACs which are activated by the Jittered BG. p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

Light Stimulation

Light patterns were generated by Psychtoolbox3 on MATLAB (Mathworks) (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). For multi-electrode recordings, a multi-electrode array was placed on the stage of an inverted microscope (IX70; Olympus). The light stimulus was projected from a cathode-ray tube display (S501J, refresh rate 60 Hz, 1,280 × 1,024 pixels, Iiyama) to the photoreceptor layer of the retina through optics. For whole-cell recordings, a patch pipette was approached from the ganglion cell side. The light stimulus was projected from a DLP projector (L51W, refresh rate 60 Hz, 1,280 × 1,024 pixels, NEC) to the photoreceptor layer through an objective lens (4 × /0.10, Nikon), with which a condenser lens of an upright microscope (Eclipse E600-FN; Nikon) was replaced. In either recording condition, the stimulus image was not distorted because the electrode was positioned on the opposite side of the incident light.

We used a background frame (4.0 × 4.0 mm) which was either uniformly dark or a Gaussian-filtered (σ, 40 μm) random-dot pattern (51,325 dots/1,0002 pixels, 4 μm/pixel, Figure 1A, see details in Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). Background Contrast (CBK) was calculated by

C BK = ( luminance BK - luminance dark ) / luminance max

where luminanceBK, luminancedark, and luminancemax were the mean intensity of the random-dot pattern, 0.11 and 14.4 cd/m2, respectively. We introduced global jitter motion to simulate the in vivo fixational eye movements of goldfish (Figures 2A–C) (Easter et al., 1974; Mensh et al., 2004). Global jitter motion was a horizontally biased random walk, in which a shift (4 μm/50 ms) to one of the four cardinal direction occurred in each stimulus frame, and the probability of horizontal shift was two times higher than that of vertical shift.

Receptive Field Estimation

The spatiotemporal receptive field (RF) was estimated by the reverse correlation method (Meister et al., 1994; Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). The retina was stimulated with dense noise consisted of pseudorandom (M-sequence) checkerboard patterns. Each frame (32 × 32 pixels with black or white; pixel size, 50 × 50 μm or 30 × 30 μm) was updated at 30 Hz. The dense noise was placed on the center region (1.6 × 1.6 or 0.96 × 0.96 mm; Figure 2A, orange) of the background (4.0 × 4.0 mm). The checkerboard frames that preceded each spike discharge were averaged (STA, spike-triggered average).

To define the orientation and size of the RF, we determined an “edge” (a position of a pixel with the intensity six times higher than the SD of the intensity in uncorrelated image) along 8 directions (0–315°, Δ45°) from the RF “center” (a pixel with maximal intensity). Eight edges were fitted by an ellipse based on the method of least squares. RF size was calculated by a length of major axis in the fitted ellipse. RF orientation (Figure 2H) was defined by the direction with the longest length among eight directions. The temporal RF profile was obtained by calculating the mean intensity of 3 × 3 pixels in the RF center region for a series of the averaged frames.

The temporal RF profile was used for classification of GC groups (Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017; Figures 1A–D and Supplementary Figure S1B). In brief, we first created an input matrix from the temporal RF profile of each GC, in which the temporal RF profiles were upsampled using interp function with a rate of 3 in MATLAB, and smoothed by a moving average filter. After the smoothing, the temporal filters were downsampled to the original rate using downsample function in MATLAB. Next, principal component analysis (PCA) was applied to visualize the features of temporal RFs (Figure 1A). Then, clustering was performed by k-means clustering using kmeans function in MATLAB. The number of clusters was determined based on the mean silhouette scores [s(i)] (Figure 1A),

s ( i ) = b ( i ) - a ( i ) / max { a ( i ) , b ( i ) } ,

where i is each cell, a(i) is average distance between i and all other points included in the same cluster, and b(i) is the smallest average distance of i to all other points (Rousseeuw, 1987; Yuan and Yang, 2019). We adopted the cluster numbers with the highest mean silhouette score for k-means clustering (Martínez et al., 2017).

To quantify the horizontal and vertical lengths of RF, we first fitted a hull to the RF (Figure 5C) using convex hull function in MATLAB. A centroid where the horizontal and vertical axes crossed was calculated in the fitted hull, and then, we measured the horizontal and vertical lengths through the centroid. To quantify the spatial bias of the RF profile, the spatial bias index (SBI) was defined by,

S B I = ( length Horizontal - length Vertical ) / ( length Horizontal + length Vertical )

where lengthHorizontal and lengthVertical are the measured RF length along the horizontal and vertical axes, respectively (Figure 5D). The positive and negative values indicate the horizontal and vertical bias of the RF, respectively.

Waveform Analysis

For whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings (Figures 3, 4), wave event was detected as the postsynaptic current when the current amplitude crossed a threshold defined by

T h r e s h o l d = | Mean c | + SD c * 3 ,

where C indicates the currents recorded during dense noise stimulation. To quantify the postsynaptic currents, we calculated the amplitude and rise time of each detected wave event. The rise time was defined as the time required for the response to reach from 63% to 100% of the peak.

Analysis of Membrane Properties

To characterize the membrane properties of GCs, we recorded the membrane potential changes induced by current pulses (duration, 200 ms; amplitude, from −50 to 100 pA; increment, 10 pA). For hierarchical clustering (Figure 1F), we used three features (Supplementary Figure S1D): the resting membrane potential, the maximum instantaneous firing rate, and the input resistance. The resting membrane potential was calculated as an average potential 100 ms before application of current pulses. The maximum firing rate was calculated as an inverse of minimum inter-spike intervals during current pulses [Supplementary Figure S1D, (i)]. The input resistance was calculated as a slope of the current-voltage function below subthreshold membrane potentials [Supplementary Figure S1D, (ii)]. We performed a hierarchical clustering based on the correlation distance in the standardized feature space using linkage and dendrogram functions in MATLAB (Baden et al., 2016).

Morphological Analysis

Lucifer yellow was introduced through a recording patch pipette to visualize the cell morphology. Using ImageJ (NIH) and customized program in MATLAB, we quantified the dendritic field, shape, and dendritic branches of each GC (Figure 1 and Supplementary Figure S1E). The dendritic tips of a stained GC were fitted to a polygon, and then its major axis and the aspect ratio of major to minor axis were used as the measure of the length and the shape, respectively. The number of branches normalized by an area of the polygon was used as the measure of the dendritic branches.

Quantification and Statistical Analysis

In Figures 2F–H, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (KS test) was used. In Figure 2H, Hodges-Ajne test was used to evaluate the bias in distribution of the RF orientation. In Figures 3C,F, 4F, paired t-test was used. In Figure 5C, Mann-Whitney U test (MWU test) with Tukey’s post hoc test was used. All measures for population data were described as mean ± SD. Error bar indicates SD. ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗p < 0.01; p < 0.05.

Results

Classification of Retinal Ganglion Cells Based on the Physiological and Anatomical Features

Applying a multi-electrode array to the goldfish isolated retina, we estimated the spatiotemporal receptive field (RF) profiles of retinal ganglion cells (GCs) by the reverse correlation method (Meister et al., 1994). The shape of the temporal RF is one of the criteria to define ganglion cell types functionally (DeVries and Baylor, 1997; Hilgen et al., 2017; Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). To dissect GC types, we performed feature detection using principal component analysis for the temporal RF (Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017), and the resulting features were clustered into six GC groups based on k-means clustering (Figures 1A,B and Supplementary Figure S1B; Rousseeuw, 1987; Yuan and Yang, 2019). The differences among the GC groups were summarized by two temporal features: peak time (Fast/Medium/Slow; Figure 1C) and kinetics (transient/sustained; Figure 1D). Thus, individual GC groups were defined as Fast-transient (Ft, group 1), Fast-sustained (Fs, group 2), Medium-transient (Mt, group 3), Medium-sustained (Ms, group 4), Slow-transient (St, group 5), and Slow-sustained (Ss, group 6) GCs (Figure 1B and Supplementary Figure S1C).

We then assessed whether each GC group could share common physiological and anatomical features. We performed whole-cell recordings from randomly selected GCs and dye loading through the recording pipette to visualize the cell morphology (Supplementary Figures S1D–F). Based on three physiological features which represent the membrane properties (Figure 1E, i–iii) and two morphological features (Figure 1E, iv, v), a hierarchical clustering (Martínez et al., 2017; Gouwens et al., 2019) revealed that GCs were branched into six types (Figure 1F, type I to VI). Intriguingly, each type defined by physiological and anatomical features was populated dominantly by a GC group defined by the filtering property (Figures 1G,H). These results highlight the correlation between the filtering property and the physiological and anatomical features to characterize GCs.

Global Jitter Motion Changes Spatiotemporal Receptive Field Profiles of a Specific Group of Retinal Ganglion Cells

To explore how the RF properties of GCs are affected by fixational eye movements, dense noise stimulus was presented on a large background (BG; 4 × 4 mm on the retina, visual angle >∼67°; Figure 2A; Macy and Easter, 1981). We used three kinds of BG patterns: a uniformly dark pattern (“Dark BG”), a static random-dot pattern (“Static BG,” a Gaussian-filtered random-dot pattern; mean luminance, 6.67 cd/m2), and a jittered random-dot pattern (“Jittered BG,” the Gaussian-filtered random-dot pattern jittered randomly) (Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). The jitter motion was a horizontally biased random walk (probability of horizontal shift/vertical shift = 2; each shift, 4 μm/50 ms; Figures 2B,C and Supplementary Figure S2C) that simulated the goldfish fixational eye movements (Easter et al., 1974; Mensh et al., 2004). Both the Static and Jittered BGs covered the area far away from the RFs of recorded GCs (Supplementary Figure S2A). Based on the responses to dense noise stimulus, we estimated the spatiotemporal RF of GCs under different BG conditions: RFDark BG (gray), RFStatic BG (black), and RFJittered BG (blue).

We found that the Jittered BG altered the spatiotemporal RF profile of Ft GCs (Figure 2D). The size of RFJittered BG (major axis of the RF; 161.9 ± 13.2 μm, 21 Ft GCs) was larger than that of either RFDark BG (112.5 ± 13.7 μm, p = 0.001; paired t-test) or RFStatic BG (115.6 ± 22.9 μm, p = 0.002). Expansion index, the ratio of the RF size under different BG conditions, was large only under the Jittered BG condition (Jittered BG vs. Dark BG, p = 8.91 × 10–9; Static BG vs. Dark BG, p = 0.38; KS test, 21 Ft GCs; Figure 2F, red). Other GC groups did not show prominent changes (Figures 2E,F, gray).

The temporal profile of RFJittered BG in Ft GCs showed faster peak time and smaller peak amplitude than those of RFDark BG and RFStatic BG (Figures 2D,G), indicating that the Jittered BG sensitized firing and input integration kinetics. These alterations of the temporal RF profile were specific to Ft GCs (peak time, Jittered BG vs. Dark BG, p = 2.35 × 10–9, Static BG vs. Dark BG, p = 0.097; peak amplitude, Jittered BG vs. Dark BG, p = 9.03 × 10–9, Static BG vs. Dark BG, p = 0.084; KS test, 21 Ft GCs; Figure 2G, red). The Jittered BG did not affect the temporal RF profile of other GC groups (Figures 2E,G, gray), indicating that the RF alterations are not ascribed to light adaptation to the random-dot background.

Intriguingly, RFJittered BG of Ft GCs was oriented along the retinal rostral-caudal (horizontal) axis (p = 0.019, Hodges-Ajne test, 21 Ft GCs; Figure 2H, blue), indicating that the RF expansion is biased along the retinal horizontal axis but not along the dorsal-ventral (vertical) axis (Figure 2H, bottom, red arrows). Since RFDark BG and RFStatic BG did not show orientation bias (RFDark BG, p = 0.7763; RFStatic BG, p = 0.7763; Hodges-Ajne test, 21 Ft GCs), the horizontal bias is not intrinsic to Ft GCs. Other GC groups showed no significant orientation bias under three BG conditions. It should be noted that the non-coherent (flickering) random noise BG did not alter the spatial RF profile of Ft GCs (RF size, Dark BG, 125.6 ± 21.1 μm; Flickering random noise BG, 121.9 ± 17.6 μm, p = 0.31; paired t-test, 17 Ft GCs; Supplementary Figure S2B). Therefore, global jitter motion is essential for the RF alterations in Ft GCs.

Synaptic Currents Evoked by the Dense Noise Stimulus Under the Jittered BG Condition in Ft GCs

Effects of the global jitter motion on the spatiotemporal RF profiles were prominent in Ft GCs. To elucidate the underlying mechanisms, we recorded postsynaptic currents from Ft GCs in the whole-cell voltage-clamp configuration (Figure 3). Ft GCs were identified based on the temporal RF profile estimated by firing to dense noise stimulus in the cell-attached configuration. We found that excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) under the Jittered BG condition were larger in amplitude (Jittered BG, –110.2 ± 56.8 pA; Dark BG, –42.8 ± 33.9 pA; p = 0.001, paired t-test, 5 Ft GCs), and faster in kinetics (Jittered BG, –110.2 ± 56.8 pA; Dark BG, –42.8 ± 33.9 pA; p = 3.85 × 10–5, paired t-test, 5 Ft GCs) than those under the Dark BG condition (Figures 3B,C). EPSCs under the Static BG condition were not significantly different in both amplitude and kinetics from those under the Dark BG condition (Static BG, –45.7 ± 20.9 pA, 10.6 ± 2.5 ms, ps > 0.3; paired t-test, 5 Ft GCs; Supplementary Figure S3), indicating that emergence of large and fast EPSCs is not ascribed to adaptation to mean contrast increment (Dark BG vs. Static BG) but to alteration induced by the global jitter motion (Static BG vs. Jittered BG).

In contrast, inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs; Figure 3D) were not affected by the Jittered BG (amplitude, Jittered BG, 99.1 ± 64.5 pA; Dark BG, 102.6 ± 66.9 pA; kinetics, Jittered BG, 28.1 ± 15.5 ms, Dark BG 27.2 ± 16.7 ms, ps > 0.3; paired t-test, 5 Ft GCs; Figures 3E,F). Therefore, it is likely that the RF alterations under the Jittered BG condition were induced not by feedforward inhibitory synaptic inputs from amacrine cells (ACs) to Ft GCs but by excitatory synaptic inputs from bipolar cells (BCs) to Ft GCs.

Alterations of the Spatial Distribution of Excitatory Inputs to Ft GCs by the Jittered BG

To examine the spatial RF profile under the Jittered BG condition in detail, we mapped the distribution of EPSCs evoked by local light stimulation (Spatial EPSC map; Figure 4). A whole-cell voltage-clamped Ft GC was stimulated by a small flash (Figure 4A; size, 100 × 100 μm; duration, 50 ms; inter-flash interval, 1 s), which was presented randomly at various locations on the Dark BG (Figure 4B) or on the Jittered BG (Figure 4C). We found that the EPSC map under the Jittered BG condition was spatially expanded along the retinal horizontal axis (Figures 4C–E), corresponding to the horizontally elongated RFJittered BG (Figure 2D).

Interestingly, we found that the properties of evoked EPSCs were not homogeneous across the RF under the Jittered BG condition: small and slow inputs at the center region (Figure 4D [2] and Figure 4F; 17.8 ± 9.87 pA, 9.54 ± 1.54 ms); large and fast inputs at the horizontally-expanded region (Figure 4D [1] and Figure 4F; 43.7 ± 19.39 pA, 7.54 ± 1.53 ms). It is likely that the emergence of large and fast EPSCs mediates the horizontally elongated RF of Ft GCs under the Jittered BG condition.

Contribution of GABAergic and Electrical Pathways to RF Elongation in Ft GCs

In the goldfish retina both GABAergic inhibition from AC to Mb1 (ON) BC terminal (Tachibana and Kaneko, 1988) and electrical coupling through connexin 36 (Cx36) between Mb1 BC dendrites contribute to global information processing (Arai et al., 2010; Tanaka and Tachibana, 2013; Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). It is possible that the horizontal RF elongation in Ft GCs may be attributable to deactivation of lateral inhibition mediated by ACs and/or activation of electrically coupled excitatory network (Bloomfield and Völgyi, 2009; Werblin, 2011).

Using a multi-electrode array, we examined the effects of pharmacological blockers on the properties of RFJittered BG (Figure 5). Application of a GABA receptor blocker picrotoxin (PTX, 100 μM) increased the RFDark BG size (horizontal length, 142.7 ± 12.4 μm in Control, 162.3 ± 18.1 μm in PTX, p = 0.03; vertical length, 133.5 ± 13.9 μm in Control, 158.6 ± 14.8 μm in PTX, p = 0.01; Mann-Whitney U (MWU) test, 7 Ft GCs; Figures 5B,C). Even in the presence of PTX, the Jittered BG increased further the RF size (RFJittered BG, horizontal length 194.5 ± 17.9 μm, p = 0.007; vertical length 208.2 ± 27.3 μm, p = 0.0012; MWU test, 7 Ft GCs, Figure 5C), although the spatial bias disappeared (Figure 5D). These results suggest that two different GABAergic mechanisms may mediate the surround inhibition (Werblin, 2011): the local inhibition which works under the dark BG condition, and the global inhibition which contributes to emphasizing the horizontal bias of the RF under the Jittered BG condition.

In the presence of gap junction blocker mefloquine (MFQ, 10 μM; Cruikshank et al., 2004), the Jittered BG did not increase the horizontal length of the RFJittered BG (horizontal length, Dark BG, 140.7 ± 16.1 μm; Jittered BG, 132.7 ± 14.8 μm, p = 0.32; MWU test, 7 Ft GCs; Figures 5B,C), but decreased the vertical length of the RFJittered BG significantly (vertical length, 130.3 ± 15.9 μm under the Dark BG, 107.3 ± 8.3 μm under the Jittered BG, p = 0.0175; MWU test, 7 Ft GCs; Figure 5C). These results suggest that electrical coupling may mediate the spatial expansion of the RFJittered BG. Indeed, spatial bias index (see section “Method Details”) indicates that the horizontal bias of the RFJittered BG was still maintained in the presence of MFQ (Figure 5D, green), whereas the horizontal bias was not observed in the presence of PTX (Figure 5D, red).

Therefore, the potential mechanism for the RF alterations may be the following: the Jittered BG expands the RF in all directions by electrical coupling, perhaps between Mb1 BCs (Arai et al., 2010); and the dorsal and ventral sides of the expanded RF are suppressed by activated GABAergic lateral inhibition (Tanaka and Tachibana, 2013), resulting in the horizontally elongated RF (Figure 5E). Lateral inhibition may be ascribed not to the feedforward input from AC to Ft GC but to the feedback input from AC to Mb1 BC terminal (Tanaka and Tachibana, 2013) because IPSCs in Ft GCs were not altered by the Jittered BG (Figures 3E,F).

Discussion

In our previous study, we showed that firing properties of specific GCs in the goldfish retina were modulated by a rapid shift of a target following a period of jitter motion of a global random-dot background (Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). Here, we examined how global jitter motion alters the RF properties of Ft GCs and what mechanisms underlies the RF alteration. We found that global jitter motion induced the RF elongation along the retinal horizontal (caudal-rostral) axis (Figure 2) in Ft GCs (Figure 1). At the elongated region, EPSCs with large amplitude and fast kinetics were dominated (Figures 3, 4). Pharmacological experiments revealed that both GABAergic lateral inhibition and electrical coupling contributed to the horizontal RF elongation (Figure 5).

Synaptic Modulation Mediated by Electrical and GABAergic Pathways

Our results indicate that the RF alteration in Ft GCs is mediated by lateral interaction pathways: electrical and GABAergic pathways (Figure 5). In the goldfish retina, excitation of an Mb1 BC spreads to neighboring Mb1 BCs through gap junction between their dendrites (Arai et al., 2010). Such lateral spread of excitation could expand the RF. It is possible that the synchronous transient glutamate release from electrically coupled BCs may evoke EPSCs with large amplitude and fast kinetics. It is conceivable that Ft GCs may receive excitatory inputs from a population of BCs under the Dark BG (“conventional BCs” in Figure 5E), and large and fast excitatory inputs from another population of electrically coupled BCs which are additionally activated by the Jittered BG (“Electrically coupled BCs” in Figure 5E).

In the goldfish retina, global stimulation activates the electrically coupled Mb1 BC network, which in turn activates wide-field ACs and/or electrically coupled ACs (Tanaka and Tachibana, 2013). Blockade of GABA receptors under the Dark BG condition increased the size of RF in all directions (Figure 5C), indicating that the local GABAergic inhibition was not biased spatially (Figure 5E). However, blockade of GABA receptors under the Jittered BG condition impaired the horizontal bias (Figure 5D). These observations indicate that the global inhibitory mechanism could affect the electrically coupled BCs at the dorsal and vertical side of the expanded RF (“Global GABAergic inhibition,” Figure 5E).

On the other hand, the horizontal bias remained even after blockade of electrical coupling (Figure 5D). This result indicates that the inputs from electrically coupled BCs may not be the unique resource to drive the global inhibitory mechanism. It is possible that the continuous jitter motion may prevent the BCs from adaptation, resulting in augmentation of the synaptic inputs to the wide-field ACs. Another possible resource may be the ACs electrically coupled through non-Cx36 (Mark et al., 1988; Völgyi et al., 2013), which are also activated by the Jittered BG. This mechanism could explain the observed horizontal bias in the presence of a gap junction blocker. Since AC circuits in the goldfish retina are not yet fully understood, further studies are needed to clarify the circuit mechanism.

Diverse Features of Fixational Eye Movements

In this study we used randomly jittering motion to mimic the retinal image during fixation, which was similar to the random walk used in the previous studies (Ölveczky et al., 2003; Baccus et al., 2008; Segal et al., 2015). The stimulus was summarized by oscillatory image motion with small amplitude, which approximates to an animal’s tremor (e.g., archer fish, 12 μm in mean amplitude, 5 Hz in frequency, Segev et al., 2007). Nevertheless, the features of fixational eye movements are more complicated in amplitude, speed, and frequency: drift, slow motion occurring together with tremor; microsaccedes, small but rapid and involuntary shifts of eyes (Martinez-Conde et al., 2004; Rucci and Poletti, 2015). Global jitter motion used in this study may be similar to tremor and drift in periodicity and slow frequency components (0.5–8 Hz; Supplementary Figure S2C). On the other hand, our stimulus did not include the small and rapid displacements corresponding to microsaccades. Furthermore, it is not yet clear how three components of fixational eye movements interact with one another and affect the retinal circuit. It is likely that that the RF alterations shown in this study may be a part of the effects induced by fixational eye movements. Further studies are required to dissect the relationship between the diverse features of fixational eye movements and the retinal information processing.

Functional Relevance of RF Elongation

Goldfish makes horizontally biased saccades spontaneously (Salas et al., 1997). Thus, the horizontal elongation of RF in Ft GCs during global jitter motion seems to be advantageous to responding quickly to a target in a succeeding rapid gaze shift (saccade). Indeed, we have shown that horizontal, but not vertical, rapid shift of a target following jitter motion evoked firing in Ft GCs before arrival of the target to the RF estimated under the Dark BG condition (Matsumoto and Tachibana, 2017). In some visual neurons in the brain, their RFs spatially shift or expand toward the future position of eyes before saccade initiation (frontal eye field; Umeno and Goldberg, 1997, prefrontal cortex; Zirnsak et al., 2014). These dynamic RF changes in visual neurons may contribute to fast and efficient processing of visual information during repetitive sequence of eye movements (Hosoya et al., 2005; Gollisch and Meister, 2008; Wurtz, 2008; Zirnsak et al., 2014). It is possible that, in natural environment, the RF profiles of specific GCs are tuned to global image motion induced by eye movements to facilitate processing in the visual system.

Data Availability

The data and codes that used in this research are available at the corresponding author, MT (mstchbn@fc.ritsumei.ac.jp) upon reasonable requests.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Committee of Animal Experiments in The University of Tokyo.

Author Contributions

Both authors designed the study, performed all experiments, and wrote the manuscript. AM performed the data analysis.

Funding

This work was supported by Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology, Japan Science and Technology Agency (CREST, JST) and JST KAKENHI Grant Number JP19K06915 to MT and by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research, Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Fellows to AM.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2019.00979/full#supplementary-material

References

Arai, I., Tanaka, M., and Tachibana, M. (2010). Active role of electrical coupled bipolar cell network in the adult retina. J. Neurosci. 30, 9260–9270. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baccus, S. A., Ölveczky, B. P., Manu, M., and Meister, M. (2008). A retinal circuit that computes object motion. J. Neurosci. 28, 6807–6817. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4206-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baden, T., Berens, P., Franke, K., Román Rosón, M., Bethge, M., and Euler, T. (2016). The functional diversity of retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. Nature 529, 345–350. doi: 10.1038/nature16468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bair, W., and O’Keefe, L. P. (1998). the influence of fixational eye movements on the response of neurons in area mt of the macaque. Vis. Neurosci. 5, 779–786. doi: 10.1017/s0952523898154160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloomfield, S., and Völgyi, B. (2009). The diverse functional roles and regulation of neuronal gap junctions in the retina. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 495–506. doi: 10.1038/nrn2636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainard, D. (1997). The psychophysics tool box. Spat. Vis. 10, 443–446.

Google Scholar

Cruikshank, S., Hopperstad, M., Younger, M., Connors, B., Spray, D., and Srinivas, M. (2004). Potent block of Cx36 and Cx50 gap junction channels by mefloquine. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 12364–12369. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402044101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVries, S. H., and Baylor, D. A. (1997). Mosaic arrangement of ganglion cell receptive fields in rabbit retina. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 2048–2060. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.4.2048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easter, S., Pamela, R., and Heckenlively, D. (1974). Horizontal compensatory eye movements in goldfish (Carassius auratus). J. Comp. Physiol. 92, 23–35. doi: 10.1007/bf00696524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollisch, T., and Meister, M. (2008). Rapid neural coding in the retina with relative spike latencies. Science 319, 1108–1111. doi: 10.1126/science.1149639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouwens, N. W., Sorensen, S. A., Berg, J., Lee, C., Jarsky, T., Ting, J., et al. (2019). Classification of electrophysiological and morphological neuron types in the mouse visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1182–1195. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0417-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greschner, M., Bongard, M., Rujan, P., and Ammermüller, J. (2002). Retinal ganglion cell synchronization by fixational eye movements improves feature estimation. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 341–347. doi: 10.1038/nn821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilgen, G., Pirmoradian, S., Pamplona, D., Komprobst, P., Cessac, B., Hennig, M. H., et al. (2017). Pan-retinal characterization of light responses from ganglion cells in the developing mouse retina. Sci. Rep. 7:42330.

Google Scholar

Hosoya, T., Baccus, S. A., and Meister, M. (2005). Dynamic predictive coding by the retina. Nature 436, 71–77. doi: 10.1038/nature03689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keesey, U. T. (1960). Effects of involuntary eye movements on visual acuity. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 50:769. doi: 10.1364/josa.50.000769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnamoorthy, V., Weick, M., and Gollisch, T. (2017). Sensitivity to image recurrence across eye-movement-like image transitions through local serial inhibition in the retina. eLife 6:e22431. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, M. F. (2009). Vision, eye movements, and natural behavior. Vis. Neurosci. 26, 51–62. doi: 10.1017/S0952523808080899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicki, M. S. (1998). A review of methods for spike sorting: the detection and classification of neural action potentials. Network 9, R53–R78.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Macy, A., and Easter, S. (1981). Growth-related changes in the size of receptive field centers of retinal ganglion cells in goldfish. Vis. Res. 21, 1497–1504. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(81)90221-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mark, R. E., Liu, W. L., and Muller, J. E. (1988). Gap junctions in the inner plexiform layer of the goldfish retina. Vis. Res. 28, 9–24. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90003-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez, J. J., Rahsepar, B., and White, J. A. (2017). Anatomical and electrophysiological clustering of superficial medial entorhinal cortex interneurons. eNeuro 4:ENEURO.0263-16.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0263-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S., and Hubel, D. (2004). The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 229–240. doi: 10.1038/nrn1348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., and Hubel, D. H. (2000). Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 251–258. doi: 10.1038/72961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, A., and Tachibana, M. (2017). Rapid and coordinated processing of global motion images by local clusters of retinal ganglion cells. Proc. JPN. Acad. Ser. B Phys. Biol. Sci. 93, 234–249. doi: 10.2183/pjab.93.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meister, M., Pine, J., and Baylor, D. A. (1994). Multi-neuronal signals from the retina: acquisition and analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 51, 95–106. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(94)90030-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mensh, B., Aksay, E., Lee, D., Seung, H., and Tank, D. (2004). Spontaneous eye movements in goldfish: oculomotor integrator performance, plasticity, and dependence on visual feedback. Vis. Res. 44, 711–726. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murakami, I. (2006). Fixational eye movements and motion perception. Prog. Brain Res. 154, 193–209. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)54010-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noda, H., and Adey, W. R. (1974). Retinal ganglion cells of the cat transfer information on saccadic eye movement and quick target motion. Brain Res. 70, 340–345. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90323-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ölveczky, B. P., Baccus, S. A., and Meister, M. (2003). Segregation of object and background motion in the retina. Nature 423, 401–408. doi: 10.1038/nature01652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelli, D. (1997). The videotoolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897x00366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, H. G., Pedreira, C., and Quian Quiroga, R. (2015). Past, present and future of spike sorting techniques. Brain Res. Bull. 119, 106–117. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2015.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roska, B., and Werblin, F. (2003). Rapid global shifts in natural scenes block spiking in specific ganglion cells types. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 600–608. doi: 10.1038/nn1061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseeuw, P. J. (1987). Silhouettes: a graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster anslysis. J. Comput. Appl. Math. 20, 53–65. doi: 10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucci, M., Iovin, R., Poletti, M., and Santini, F. (2007). Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail. Nature 447, 852–855. doi: 10.1038/nature05866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucci, M., and Poletti, M. (2015). Control and functions of fixational eye movements. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 1, 499–518. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salas, C., Herrero, L., Rodriguez, F., and Torres, B. (1997). Tectal codification of eye movements in goldfish studied by electrical microstimulation. Neuroscience 78, 271–288. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)83048-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segal, I. Y., Giladi, C., Gedalin, M., Rucci, M., Ben-Tov, M., Kushinsky, Y., et al. (2015). Decorrelation of retinal response to natural scenes by fixational eye movements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 3110–3115. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1412059112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segev, R., Schneidman, E., Goodhouse, J., and Berry, M. J. (2007). Role of eye movements in the retinal code for a size discrimination task. J. Neurophyiol. 98, 1380–1391. doi: 10.1152/jn.00395.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tachibana, M., and Kaneko, A. (1988). Retinal bipolar cells receive negative feedback input from GABAergic amacrine cells. Vis. Neurosci. 1, 297–305. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800001954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, M., and Tachibana, M. (2013). Independent control of reciprocal and lateral inhibition at the axon terminal of retinal bipolar cells. J. Physiol. 591, 3833–3851. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.253179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tien, N. W., Pearson, J. T., Heller, C. R., Demas, J., and Kerschensteiner, D. (2015). Genetically identified suppressed-by-contrast retinal ganglion cells reliably signal self-generated visual stimuli. J. Neurosci. 35, 10815–10820. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1521-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulunay-Keesey, U., and Jones, R. M. (1976). The effects of micromovements of the eye and exposure duration on contrast sensitivity. Vis. Res. 16, 481–488. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(76)90026-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umeno, M. M., and Goldberg, M. E. (1997). Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. predictive visual responses. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 1373–1383. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Völgyi, B., Kovács-Oller, T., Atlasz, T., Wilhelm, M., and Gábriel, R. (2013). Gap junctional coupling in the vertebrate retina: variations on the theme? Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 34, 1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werblin, F. (2011). The retinal hypercircuit: a repeating synaptic interactive motif underlying visual function. J. Physiol. 589, 3691–3702. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.210617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wurtz, R. (2008). Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vis. Res. 48, 2070–2089. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye Movements and Vision. New York, NY: Plenum.

Google Scholar

Yuan, C., and Yang, H. (2019). Research on K-value selection method of K-means clustering algorithm. J. Multidiscip. Sci. 2, 226–235. doi: 10.3390/j2020016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, P. M., Wu, J. Y., Zhou, Y., Liang, P. J., and Yuan, J. Q. (2004). Spike sorting based on automatic template reconstruction with a partial solution to the overlapping problem. J. Neurosci. Methods 135, 55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zirnsak, M., Steinmetz, N. A., Noudoost, B., Xu, K. Z., and Moore, T. (2014). Visual space is compressed in prefrontal cortex before eye movements. Nature 507, 504–507. doi: 10.1038/nature13149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: retina, retinal ganglion cells, receptive field, eye movements, gap junctions

Citation: Matsumoto A and Tachibana M (2019) Global Jitter Motion of the Retinal Image Dynamically Alters the Receptive Field Properties of Retinal Ganglion Cells. Front. Neurosci. 13:979. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00979

Received: 01 July 2019; Accepted: 30 August 2019;
Published: 13 September 2019.

Edited by:

Mehdi Adibi, University of New South Wales, Australia

Reviewed by:

Masoud Ghodrati, Monash University, Australia
William Martin Connelly, University of Tasmania, Australia

Copyright © 2019 Matsumoto and Tachibana. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Masao Tachibana, mstchbn@fc.ritsumei.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.