Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Neurosci., 19 May 2020
Sec. Neural Technology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neurotechnologies for Human Augmentation View all 13 articles

Transcranial Static Magnetic Field Stimulation of the Motor Cortex in Children

\r\nAsha HollisAsha Hollis1Ephrem ZewdieEphrem Zewdie2Alberto Nettel-AguirreAlberto Nettel-Aguirre2Alicia HilderleyAlicia Hilderley3Hsing-Ching KuoHsing-Ching Kuo2Helen L. CarlsonHelen L. Carlson2Adam Kirton*Adam Kirton2*
  • 1Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 3Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada

Background: Non-invasive neuromodulation is an emerging therapy for children with early brain injury but is difficult to apply to preschoolers when windows of developmental plasticity are optimal. Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) decreases primary motor cortex (M1) excitability in adults but effects on the developing brain are unstudied.

Objective/Hypothesis: We aimed to determine the effects of tSMS on cortical excitability and motor learning in healthy children. We hypothesized that tSMS over right M1 would reduce cortical excitability and inhibit contralateral motor learning.

Methods: This randomized, sham-controlled, double-blinded, three-arm, cross-over trial enrolled 24 healthy children aged 10–18 years. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) assessed cortical excitability via motor-evoked potential (MEP) amplitude and paired pulse measures. Motor learning was assessed via the Purdue Pegboard Test (PPT). A tSMS magnet (677 Newtons) or sham was held over left or right M1 for 30 min while participants trained the non-dominant hand. A linear mixed effect model was used to examine intervention effects.

Results: All 72 tSMS sessions were well tolerated without serious adverse effects. Neither cortical excitability as measured by MEPs nor paired-pulse intracortical neurophysiology was altered by tSMS. Possible behavioral effects included contralateral tSMS inhibiting early motor learning (p < 0.01) and ipsilateral tSMS facilitating later stages of motor learning (p < 0.01) in the trained non-dominant hand.

Conclusion: tSMS is feasible in pediatric populations. Unlike adults, tSMS did not produce measurable changes in MEP amplitude. Possible effects of M1 tSMS on motor learning require further study. Our findings support further exploration of tSMS neuromodulation in young children with cerebral palsy.

Introduction

Early brain injury can result in cerebral palsy (CP) and lifelong disability for millions (A Kirton, 2013a; Oskoui et al., 2016). Perinatal stroke (PS) is brain damage due to a focal disruption in cerebral blood flow occurring between 20 weeks gestation and 28 days postpartum (Dunbar and Kirton, 2018). PS causes most hemiparetic cerebral palsy (HCP) with disabling weakness on one side of the body. With no known treatment or prevention strategies, improving outcomes and quality of life in PS is focused on neurorehabilitation.

Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) is an encouraging but understudied potential therapy for children with CP. Randomized trials suggest that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) (Gillick et al., 2015; Kirton et al., 2016) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) (Kirton et al., 2017; Gillick et al., 2018) may enhance motor learning in hemiparetic children. Proof-of-principle studies have demonstrated that the enhancement of motor learning seen in adults with tDCS of the primary motor cortex (M1) (Reis et al., 2009) also occurs in children (Ciechanski and Kirton, 2017; Cole et al., 2018). Although the safety of pediatric neurostimulation is becoming well established (Bikson et al., 2016; Friel et al., 2016), both rTMS and tDCS can have side effects, potentially limiting applications in younger children.

These neuromodulation approaches are based on evolving human and animal models of how the motor system develops following early unilateral injury (Eyre et al., 2007; Staudt, 2007a; Kirton, 2013b; Wen et al., 2018). Excessive preservation of motor control of the affected limb by the contralesional, ipsilateral hemisphere has led to trials trying “inhibitory” stimulation targeting contralesional M1. Animal models have also confirmed the optimal windows during which developmental motor plasticity occurs, with human equivalents occurring in infancy (Martin et al., 2011). Accordingly, a major limitation of existing neuromodulation approaches is difficulty of application in infants and toddlers, during the window in which one might expect the greatest potential therapeutic gains. There is therefore a need to find alternative forms of neuromodulation applicable at earlier stages of development.

Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) offers a potential solution. In tSMS, a strong magnet is held over the skull to generate a static magnetic field within functional cortical targets such as M1 (Oliviero et al., 2011; Kirimoto et al., 2016). Short-term application from 10 to 30 min in adults can decrease M1 excitability as assessed by the amplitude of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)-generated motor-evoked potentials (MEPs). Original tSMS results have since been replicated (Silbert et al., 2013; Nojima et al., 2015; Nojima et al., 2016; Dileone et al., 2018) with only one study reporting no physiological changes (Kufner et al., 2017). tSMS effects have also been described in the cerebellum and parietal cortex (Carrasco-López et al., 2017; Matsugi and Okada, 2017). The effects of tSMS in the developing brain are unstudied.

Previous studies in the field have identified that use of south or north polarity did not alter the measured impact on cortical excitability, although most literature in the field still indicates use of south polarity by convention (Oliviero et al., 2011). Unlike polarity, magnet strength and duration of application are significant factors: stronger magnets (e.g. 45 × 30 mm versus 30 × 15 mm in size) and application for longer time periods (e.g. 30 min versus 10 min) have been shown to have a stronger and longer-lasting effect on cortical excitability.

Few investigations have explored the behavioral effects of tSMS. Two adult studies found that visual cortex tSMS could inhibit visual search performance and reduce experimental photophobia (Gonzalez-Rosa et al., 2015; Lozano-Soto et al., 2017). One study of tSMS over M1 suggested inhibitory effects on pinch force (Nakagawa and Nakazawa, 2018) while another found improved reaction times in an implicit motor learning task (Nojima et al., 2019). Studies support favorable safety and tolerability when tSMS was administered for up to 120 min (Oliviero et al., 2015). The safety, tolerability, and behavioral effects of tSMS have not been explored in children. However, a large volume of safety evidence comes from decades of MRI use where millions of patients have been exposed to much higher doses (1-8T) and durations (hours of exposure) of static magnetic fields (to much larger areas of tissue) with no significant adverse effects (van Osch and Webb, 2014). Furthermore, guidelines on safety of static magnetic field exposure conclude the evidence does not indicate the presence of serious health effects given acute exposure to up to 8T fields (International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection, 2009).

Given its potential ease of application in young children and therapeutically relevant effects on M1 excitability, we aimed to evaluate whether tSMS could alter M1 excitability and motor learning in typically developing children. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study of tSMS in a pediatric population. We conducted a randomized, sham-controlled, double-blinded, cross-over trial, hypothesizing that contralateral (right) M1 tSMS would decrease MEP amplitude and inhibit motor learning in the left hand.

Materials and Methods

Trial Design

The Pediatric Transcranial Static Magnetic Field Stimulation to Improve Motor Learning (PSTIM) trial was a randomized, double-blinded, sham-controlled, three-arm, cross-over interventional trial. Methods complied with the consolidated standards of reporting trials (CONSORT) guidelines including pediatric considerations (Schulz et al., 2010). The trial was registered with www.clinicaltrials.gov (NCT03949712). Methods were approved by the University of Calgary Research Ethics Board (REB 18-0178).

Population

School-aged children were recruited through the population-based, volunteer Healthy Infants and Children Clinical Research Program (HICCUP1). Inclusion criteria were (a) written informed consent/assent, (b) age 8–18 years, (c) right-handed by self-report, and (d) typical development. Potential participants with any of the following were excluded: (a) diagnosis of any neurological, psychiatric or developmental disorder, (b) taking any neuroactive medications, (c) any contraindication to brain stimulation, and (d) pregnancy.

Randomization, Blinding, and Concealment

Participants were computer-randomized into three groups which determined intervention order: (A) Sham, Right tSMS, Left tSMS; (B) Left tSMS, Sham, Right tSMS; and (C) Right tSMS, Left tSMS, Sham. A second randomization assigned the sham stimulation side, such that half of the participants in each group had sham on the left and half on the right. Participants, parents and the primary researcher conducting tSMS and analysis were blinded to randomizations. Only a research assistant was unblinded in order to apply the correct intervention. Participants were asked to guess if they received real or sham tSMS. The randomization code was broken to the primary researcher only after the final outcome and initial analysis was completed.

Outcome Measures

Neurophysiological

The primary outcome of this study (and the main neurophysiological outcome) was right M1 excitability as measured by mean MEP amplitude generated in the left first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle. This was chosen to enable comparison of M1 excitability before and after application of the tSMS intervention. TMS is an established, safe and well tolerated method in children (Zewdie and Kirton, 2016). Previously described single and paired-pulse TMS methods were applied to assess motor system neurophysiology (Zewdie and Kirton, 2016). All experiments took place in the Alberta Children’s Hospital Pediatric Neurostimulation Laboratory where children had opportunity to test procedures beforehand and watch movies when possible for distraction from the TMS stimulation and to help reduce potential fatigue.

To measure MEPs, surface electromyography (EMG) was recorded by placing Ag/AgCl electrodes on the belly of the FDI muscle. A reference electrode was placed on the second phalange, with a ground on the ulnar head. EMG signals were amplified x1000 (2024F Isolated amplifier; Intronix Technologies Corp, ON, Canada), band-pass filtered (20–2000 Hertz (Hz)), and recorded (CED1401 signal analog/digital converter; Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, United Kingdom).

First, single-pulse TMS (Magstim 200, Magstim, Cardiff, United Kingdom) used a flat iron Magstim TMS coil to locate the right and left M1 “hotspots”, defined as the location producing the largest and most consistent MEP (Zewdie and Kirton, 2016). The coil was placed at a 45-degree angle to the midline to induce a posterior-anterior current using monophasic waveforms. The identified “hotspot” was marked using neuronavigation (Brainsight2, Rogue Research, Montreal) to facilitate accurate coil replacement for serial measurements. Single-pulse TMS was then delivered to determine resting motor threshold (RMT) defined as the lowest stimulation intensity producing a 50 microvolts (μV) MEP in 5/10 stimulations. Ten suprathreshold (120% RMT) stimulations were administered to estimate cortical excitability.

Paired-pulse TMS was then completed using two connected stimulators (Magstim bi-stim, Magstim, Cardiff, United Kingdom). Consistent with other studies, pairs of pulses were delivered, which included a conditioning stimulus (CS) (80% RMT) followed by a test stimulus (TS) (120% RMT) (Zewdie and Kirton, 2016). Interstimulus intervals (ISIs) of 2 ms and 10 ms were used to evoke short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) and intracortical facilitation (ICF), respectively. A total of 30 pulses were administered in random order: 10 test single pulses, 10 paired pulses (2 ms ISI), and 10 paired pulses (10 ms ISI).

MEP signal files were imported into MATLAB R2011b (Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) for offline analysis. Visual inspection was used to identify artifacts including baseline motor activity; proportion of traces removed was less than 5%. Peak-to-peak MEP amplitude values were calculated using a custom MATLAB script, which identified maximum and minimum MEP values within 15–80 ms after TMS. Mean peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes were averaged. SICI and ICF ratios were computed by dividing the average MEP amplitude of the conditioned responses into that of the test stimuli alone.

Behavioral

The main behavioral outcome was change in the Purdue Pegboard Test (PPT) left-hand score (PPTL). This enabled assessment of motor skill performance before, during and after application of the tSMS intervention. The PPT is a validated simple motor task that requires both gross and fine motor skills, described in detail in the cited reference (Gardner and Broman, 1979). The PPT produces consistent motor learning curves in school-aged children across multiple sessions (Tiffin and Asher, 1948; Gardner and Broman, 1979; Ciechanski and Kirton, 2017; Cole et al., 2018). The PPT consists of four tasks. For the PPTL, the participant used their left hand to move as many metal pegs into holes in the pegboard as fast as possible in 30 s. Following a 1-min break, the same task was performed with the right hand (PPTR). Following another 1-min break, the task was performed using both hands at the same time (PPTLR). Finally, 1-min was given to assemble a pin-washer-collar-washer structure using alternating hands for each metal piece (PPTA). All sections were repeated three times and averaged.

Intervention

The intervention was tSMS (or sham) over left M1 (ipsilateral) or right M1 (contralateral), modeled on previous adult studies (Oliviero et al., 2011; Silbert et al., 2013; Dileone et al., 2018). A strong cylindrical Neodynium magnet (S-45-30-N, Supermagnete) or a sham magnet (MAG45s, Neurek SL, Toledo, Spain) was affixed over the M1 hotspot using a custom-designed helmet (Figure 1). The sham magnet was identical in appearance and weight but carried no magnetic properties. The custom helmet allowed for movement in the anterior-posterior, superior-inferior and medial-lateral directions. Neuronavigation was utilized to place the magnet over the previously identified hotspot. The magnet was applied with South polarity (determined using a compass), consistent with previous adult studies (Oliviero et al., 2011). Magnet dimensions were 30 mm tall × 45 mm wide with an estimated strength of 300–450 milliTesla (mT) at the cortex (Tharayil et al., 2018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Custom-Engineered tSMS Helmet. Custom designed and partially 3-D printed helmet used for application of tSMS with neuronavigation.

Study Flow

The timeline and flow of the study is diagrammed in Figure 2. On visit 1, all baseline behavioral and neurophysiological measures were obtained. Participants first performed the PPTL outcome. Additional behavioral outcomes of PPTR, PPTLR and PPTA were then performed. The M1 hotspots for FDI were mapped followed by the single and paired pulse measurements, including RMT.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PSTIM protocol. Participants completed baseline PPT (all tasks: PPTL, PPTR, PPTLR, PPTA), and then underwent baseline neurophysiology testing, including determining the “hotspot”, identifying the RMT, and performing single and paired pulse protocols for test MEPs, SICI and ICF. They then received the tSMS intervention paired with motor training on the PPTL. There were 3 treatment orders, as shown. Neurophysiology measures (single and paired pulse) and PPT (all tasks) were repeated at multiple time intervals post-tSMS.

Following a short break, the magnet-holding device was affixed to the participant’s head. Size was adjusted for head shape and comfort. The magnet was positioned on the skull over the left or right M1 hotspot as identified by neuronavigation. The magnet was then held in place for 30 min. During this time, participants trained the non-dominant left hand by performing the PPTL five times (minutes 7, 12, 17, 22, and 27). The non-dominant hand was targeted to enable skill growth from baseline, given the common assumption of lower skill in the non-dominant hand. TMS studies have also suggested differences in excitability between the dominant and non-dominant hemisphere (Daligadu et al., 2013) but we could only examine one. Use of the non-dominant hand is consistent with prior motor skill learning research by our team and others (Ciechanski and Kirton, 2017; Cole et al., 2018).

After completion of tSMS, the magnet and holding device were removed. The RMT of the right M1 hotspot (identified via neuronavigation) was then re-measured and the TMS neurophysiological measurements were repeated at minutes 5, 10, 20, and 30 post-tSMS. Finally, all PPT tasks (PPTL, PPTR, PPTLR, and PPTA) were repeated at approximately 35 min post-tSMS.

On visits 2 and 3, all procedures were repeated, with the exception of varying the intervention according to the randomized group. Each visit was scheduled to occur not less than two and not more than 4 weeks (+/- 4 days) from the previous one.

Safety and Tolerability

Participants completed a pediatric tSMS and TMS safety and tolerability survey at the end of each session as previously described (Garvey and Gilbert, 2004; Cole et al., 2018). Participants were asked to rank the tolerability of the tSMS or TMS session in comparison to seven common childhood experiences (e.g. birthday party, shot at the doctor). Participants were also asked to report the presence and severity of any symptoms experienced including headaches, neck pain, unpleasant tingling or itching, fatigue, nausea, and light-headedness. All procedures were performed by trained personnel. Requests for additional breaks were accommodated.

Sample Size

Sample size was determined based on the primary outcome (MEP amplitudes) using effect sizes in adults as a guide. Based on our crossover design, an expected (conservative) decrease of MEP amplitude in the stimulated M1 from approximately 1 millivolts (mV) to 0.9 mV compared to no change in sham, power of 90%, standard deviation (SD) of 0.1, and alpha of 0.05, we estimated a sample size of 24 (8 per group).

Statistical Analysis

Given our primary neurophysiological and secondary behavioral outcomes, crossover design, and aim to explore effects both between and within subjects, we employed a linear mixed effects model. Fixed effects were considered for treatment (left tSMS, right tSMS or sham), visit (1, 2 or 3), age, and an interaction between treatment and visit (visit effects were only considered for PPT). For change in PPTL, PPTR, PPTLR, and PPTA on visit 1 only, we also employed a simple linear regression with independent variables of treatment and age. The Shapiro–Wilks test was used to assess normality of distribution of residuals, and the Breusch-Pagan/Cook-Weisberg test was used to assess heteroskedasticity of residuals. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were also utilized to compare group demographics and ensure there were no significant differences in age, sex, and baseline PPT scores between groups. Analyses were performed using Stata 14.2.

Results

Population

A total of 131 potential participants were approached. Thirty participants were recruited. Six were subsequently excluded due to self-withdrawal for scheduling conflicts (n = 2), incorrect order of intervention (n = 1), and high RMT that precluded the TMS protocol (n = 3). The final sample of participants who consented and completed the study consisted of 24 participants (13 males) with a median age of 15.9 years (range 10–18). Participant demographics and baseline motor function are summarized in Table 1. Groups were comparable with no differences in age, sex, or function.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics and baseline PPTL scores1.

Baseline Neurophysiology Measurements

TMS data were obtained from all participants. RMT ranged from 37% to 77% of maximum stimulus output (MSO) (mean 49.29, SD = 9.61). Baseline RMT was negatively correlated with age (r = −0.56, p < 0.01). Mean (SD) test MEP amplitude from all participants at baseline was 1.28 (1.1) mV. SICI and ICF were present with test MEP inhibited by the 2 ms subthreshold CS (0.57 (0.5) mV) and facilitated by the 10 ms CS (1.62 (1.29) mV). Raw MEP averages are shown in Figure 3A. Mean ratios of raw conditioned/raw test MEPs for SICI (0.47 (0.31)) and raw conditioned/raw test MEPs for ICF (1.41 (0.48)) were robust and consistent with expected SICI and ICF ratios in children (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Baseline TMS data. (A) Baseline raw MEP values for test-pulse MEPs and paired-pulse (SICI and ICF) MEPs. (B) SICI and ICF ratios at baseline, calculated as raw SICI or ICF MEPs divided by raw test MEPs. Thick lines indicate mean value.

Effects of tSMS on M1 Neurophysiology

MEP amplitudes did not change significantly between baseline and the immediate (5 min) post-tSMS measurement regardless of treatment group. Change in individual raw test MEP amplitudes are shown in Figure 4 by treatment group. MEP amplitudes 5 min post-left or -right tSMS and normalized to baseline did not change as compared to sham (left tSMS 95% confidence interval (CI) −0.19, 0.64; p = 0.29; right tSMS 95% CI −0.38, 0.44; p = 0.89). In addition to these results for the presumed maximal effect time at 5 min, no changes were seen at 10, 20 or 30 min post-tSMS either. RMT also did not change between baseline and follow-up for any treatment group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effect of sham, left and right tSMS on corticospinal excitability. (A) Raw test MEP values at baseline and 5 min post-sham tSMS. (B) Raw test MEP values at baseline and 5 min post-right tSMS. (C) Raw test MEP values at baseline and 5 min post-left tSMS. (D) Change in test MEPs from baseline to 5 min post-tSMS, normalized to baseline test MEPs. Thick lines indicate mean value.

Measurements of SICI and ICF ratios from baseline to immediately post-tSMS are summarized in Figure 5. Changes in intracortical physiology following right tSMS, left tSMS and sham tSMS and normalized to baseline did not appear different between groups (all p > 0.24). Although results are only shown for 5-min post-tSMS, no significant changes were observed at 10, 20 or 30 min post-tSMS either (all p > 0.1).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effect of sham, right and left tSMS on intracortical excitability. (A) Change in SICI ratio from baseline to 5 min post-tSMS, calculated as SICI ratio 5 min post-tSMS divided by the SICI ratio at baseline. (B) Change in ICF ratio from baseline to 5 min post-tSMS, calculated as ICF ratio 5 min post-tSMS divided by the ICF ratio at baseline. Thick lines indicate mean value.

Effects of tSMS on Motor Learning Behavior

All participants demonstrated motor learning curves consistent with previous pediatric studies (Ciechanski and Kirton, 2017; Cole et al., 2018). Curves of motor learning by intervention for visit 1 are shown in Figure 6A. Learning differed by intervention group. On average, participants who received sham tSMS improved by 3.21(1.70) pegs by their final PPT. Those receiving left M1 tSMS improved by 2.50(1.14) pegs by their final PPT. Participants receiving right M1 tSMS improved by 1.88(0.99) pegs on their final PPT. The linear mixed effects model conditional upon age and visit suggested the effect of left tSMS compared to sham was a 0.74 reduction in pegs moved (95% CI −1.72, 0.25;p = 0.14). The effect of receiving right tSMS compared to sham was a 1.47 reduction in pegs moved (95% CI −2.46, −0.48;p < 0.01). The Cohen’s d for sham versus right tSMS was 0.96. The same pattern of group differences was observed at the retention timepoint 30 min following completion of tSMS with a 1.36 reduction for right tSMS (95% CI −2.33, −0.39; p < 0.01) and 0.52 reduction (95% CI −1.48,0.45; p = 0.26) for left tSMS. No significant treatment group effects were seen for the other motor outcomes (Figure 6D). Greater variance in these other secondary motor outcomes is consistent with other similar studies. The Shapiro–Wilks test and the Breusch-Pagan/Cook-Weisberg test revealed normality and homoskedasticity of residuals could not be rejected, giving us confidence in our estimation methodology.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Trained left hand motor learning by intervention. (A–C) Show trained left hand motor learning on PPTL, (D) shows change in all PPT tasks. (A) Mean change in pegs moved from visit 1 baseline for PPTL was greater for sham (white diamonds) than for left (gray squares) or right tSMS (black circles). Effects were retained for all groups 30 min post-tSMS. Error bars show standard error. *p < 0.01 for right tSMS vs sham. (B) Mean change in pegs moved from visit 2 baseline for PPTL were not statistically significantly different for sham (white diamonds), left (gray squares) or right tSMS (black circles) on visit 2. (C) Mean change in pegs moved from visit 3 baseline for PPTL was greater for left (black circles) than for sham (white diamonds). Effects were retained 30 min post-tSMS. Error bars show standard error. *p < 0.01 for left tSMS vs sham. (D) Mean change in pegs moved from baseline for PPTL was greater for sham (white box) than for left (striped box) or right tSMS (gray box) at 30 min post tSMS. No statistically significant changes in pegs moved occurred for PPTR, PPTLR, or PPTA for left or right tSMS compared to sham. Thick lines indicate mean value. Error bars show standard error. *p < 0.01 for right tSMS vs sham for PPTL on visit 1.

No significant changes were seen in the PPTL task on visit 2 (Figure 6B) or any other secondary behavioral outcomes. On visit 3, treatment group specific differences were observed in motor learning curves (Figure 6C). Participants who received left tSMS experienced a greater improvement in PPTL scores compared to the sham group. Conditional on age and visit, those who received left tSMS moved 1.47 more pegs than those receiving sham (95% CI −0.48, −2.46; p < 0.01). The Cohen’s d for sham versus left tSMS was −1.22. The improvement with left tSMS compared to sham was consistent at the retention timepoint 30 min following tSMS (1.60 improvement, 95% CI 0.64, 2.58; p = 0.001). Change in pegs moved for those who received right tSMS did not differ from the other groups (p = 0.62). The sham group did not change from baseline on visit 3. Motor learning from original baseline for all participants by treatment group is shown in Figure 7.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Trained left hand motor learning by intervention for all visits. Mean change in pegs moved from visit 1 baseline for PPTL. All groups experienced motor learning from baseline. Symbols indicate intervention: sham (white diamonds), right (gray squares) or left tSMS (black circles). Lines indicate participant group: group A (dashed), group B (straight), group C (dotted), for which each group contains the same participants. Error bars show standard error.

Tolerability and Safety of tSMS

A total of 72 tSMS sessions were completed without any serious adverse events. The most common reported side effects of tSMS were headaches (Real: 19% mild, 2% moderate; Sham: 25% mild) and neck-pain (Real: 19% mild, 2% moderate; Sham: 8% mild). Other reported side effects were fatigue (Real: 8% mild, 2% moderate; Sham: 8% mild), light-headedness (Real: 2% mild; Sham 8% mild) and unpleasant tingling (Real: 2% mild; Sham 4% mild). On the pediatric brain stimulation tolerability scale, mean tSMS score was 4.06/10 (+/−1.17). This average ranked as less favorable than watching television (TV) (2.70) but more favorable than a long car ride (5.36) (Figure 8). The 144 TMS neurophysiology sessions were also well tolerated. The most common side effects were fatigue (24% total; 21% mild, 3% moderate) and headaches (14% total; 13% mild, 1% moderate). Others were neck pain (11% mild), unpleasant tingling (4% mild) and light-headedness (3% mild). Mean tolerability score was 4.03 (+/- 1.21), again falling between watching TV (2.07) and a long car ride (5.28) (Figure 8). When participants were asked if they would recommend the study to a friend, 100% said yes (n = 21; three participants were missed).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. tSMS and TMS Tolerability. Bars show tolerability score out of 8 (higher scores indicate less tolerability for various childhood experiences in comparison to TMS and tSMS. tSMS and TMS are both more tolerable than a long car ride, but less tolerable than watching TV. Error bars show standard error.

Discussion

In this trial, we evaluated the effects of tSMS on cortical neurophysiology and motor learning in a pediatric population. We show that tSMS is feasible, well tolerated and safe in school-aged children. Our results suggest that contralateral tSMS may have inhibitory effects on motor learning while stimulation of the ipsilateral hemisphere may enhance later stages of learning, although this requires additional study. We were not able to replicate the neurophysiological effects of tSMS reported in most adult studies.

Since the introduction of tSMS (Oliviero et al., 2011), numerous studies have tested the effect of contralateral tSMS on cortical neurophysiology over M1 and other cortical brain regions in healthy adults. The most consistent net tSMS effects have been inhibitory in nature, often demonstrating reduced excitability such as in the motor cortex where TMS-evoked MEP amplitudes are reduced (Oliviero et al., 2011; Silbert et al., 2013; Dileone et al., 2018). The mechanism behind tSMS is not yet known, but it is thought that tSMS may act by indirectly altering ion channels in cell membranes (Rosen, 2003). Surprisingly, contralateral tSMS in children did not generate similar results. Although we hypothesized there would be an inhibition of MEP amplitudes, we found no evidence of consistent effects of contralateral (or ipsilateral) tSMS on any of our neurophysiological outcomes. This included our measures of intracortical motor neurophysiology (SICI and ICF) where again no effects were observed.

Multiple potential contributing factors may account for this discrepancy from the tSMS effects described in adults. Our study tested tSMS for the first time in a pediatric population where the many known differences of the developing brain may have been a factor. Many other studies of different forms of NIBS (TMS, tDCS) have identified distinct differences in effects in children as compared to adults (Moliadze et al., 2015; Ciechanski et al., 2018). It may also be more difficult to discern changes in intracortical neurophysiology in children compared to adults. For example, SICI may be more difficult to elicit in children, and can be differentially affected by practice- or use-dependent plasticity (Garvey and Mall, 2008). The combination in our study of both a pediatric population and concomitant motor training and neuromodulation by tSMS may have further complicated our ability to detect changes in TMS measures of M1 neurophysiology.

TMS data are also intrinsically noisy. TMS neurophysiology outcomes depend on a variety of factors, such as muscle contraction, fatigue, and attention (Darling et al., 2006; Li et al., 2015). Additional factors influencing TMS neurophysiology established in adults such as gender, sleep, medications, and genetics have not been well defined in children (Ridding and Ziemann, 2010). Reliability of the measures themselves also varies. Studies of test-retest reliability in adults have established the natural variability in the data and also that this variability changes for different measures. For example, reliability of ICF and SICI measures are lower than for RMTs (Schambra et al., 2015; Hermsen et al., 2016). Even simple test stimuli demonstrate greater variability when participants are relaxed as compared to holding an active contraction (Darling et al., 2006). While the same reliability studies have not been completed in children, there are reasons to expect the same issues are at least as relevant, if not potentially more so.

Another factor unique to our study design was that TMS measures of neurophysiology had to be acquired in conjunction with the execution and measurement of motor training. This not only complicates the measurements themselves but introduces potential noise from the effects of motor learning. Previous literature assessing motor learning has shown effects on cortical neurophysiology. For example, multiple studies have shown increases in MEP amplitudes measured from hand muscles following hand motor training (Muellbacher et al., 2001; Cirillo et al., 2010). Furthermore, pharmacological studies have shown that plastic changes associated with motor learning may share mechanistic similarities with neurostimulation such as long-term potentiation (Butefisch et al., 2000). It is therefore possible that our pairing of motor training with tSMS may have altered the potential neurophysiological effects of tSMS (Foffani and Dileone, 2017). Further studies comparing tSMS alone versus tSMS combined with motor training in children would be required to determine this.

Behaviorally, we were able to demonstrate that tSMS over M1 may modulate motor learning in children. Effects appeared to be specific to both the side of stimulation and timing across multiple motor learning sessions. Consistent with our clinical hypothesis, right contralateral M1 tSMS significantly inhibited motor learning in the trained, left hand as assessed by the PPTL. Effects were consistent across the learning curve which itself was comparable to previously described single day PPT learning curves in children (Ciechanski and Kirton, 2017; Cole et al., 2018). Specificity of effect to the trained hand was further suggested by the absence of other significant changes in any of the other secondary motor function outcomes (PPTR, PPTLR, PPTA). While our observed behavioral effects require replication, they would appear to be of similar magnitude and effect size as described for more studied forms of M1 non-invasive neuromodulation.

In contrast to the inhibitory effects of contralateral (right) tSMS on left hand motor learning, we found that after multiple days of motor training, ipsilateral (left) tSMS had a facilitatory effect on left hand motor learning as measured by the PPTL. This potential effect was hypothesized a priori based on previous studies of the effects of ipsilateral M1 rTMS and tDCS on hand motor learning. Though many exceptions are now recognized, anodal tDCS has often been suggested to increase cortical excitability while cathodal-tDCS may decrease cortical excitability (Batsikadze et al., 2013; Monte-Silva et al., 2013). In keeping with this simple model, previous adult studies have shown that anodal tDCS can facilitate motor learning when applied to the contralateral M1 (Vines et al., 2006; Reis and Fritsch, 2011). In addition, cathodal tDCS applied to the opposite, ipsilateral M1 has also been shown to facilitate motor learning (Reis and Fritsch, 2011). One study comparing these effects of M1 tDCS on motor learning directly (Vines et al., 2006) found that cathodal tDCS applied ipsilaterally improved motor learning, contralateral cathodal tDCS inhibited it, and anodal tDCS had the opposite effects (contralateral improvement, and ipsilateral inhibition). This body of adult evidence supports the concept that cathodal tDCS may improve motor learning via modulation of well-established inhibitory transcallosal pathways (interhemispheric inhibition (IHI)) which itself is associated with motor function in adults (Williams et al., 2010). Such “disinhibition” by ipsilateral cathodal stimulation might enable relative “excitation” of the opposite motor cortex, in turn facilitating motor learning (Vines et al., 2006). That TMS measures of IHI appear to be similar in school-aged children as compared to adults (Ciechanski et al., 2017) further supports this premise.

Translationally, these behavioral effects of M1 tSMS may be relevant to stroke rehabilitation. A theory of IHI imbalance has dominated early approaches to non-invasive neuromodulation of the contralesional hemisphere, though this model has more recently been questioned. Neuromodulation strategies aiming to reduce cortical excitability in the contralesional hemisphere have been associated with improved motor performance in chronic stroke (Hsu et al., 2012; Elsner et al., 2017).

Although the underlying models are different, a smaller but significant body of evidence has supported the same approach of inhibiting the contralesional motor cortex in children with PS and HCP (Kirton, 2013b). Substantial preclinical (Martin et al., 2007; Friel et al., 2013; Friel et al., 2014; Wen et al., 2018) and human (Eyre, 2007; Staudt, 2007b) evidence following early brain injury supports a negative association between the relative preservation of ipsilateral corticospinal projections from the contralesional hemisphere to the affected hand and clinical motor function. Targeting these ipsilateral tracts on the contralesional side thus has the potential to improve motor function. Multiple translational trials show both contralesional low frequency rTMS (Gillick et al., 2014; Kirton et al., 2016) and cathodal tDCS (Kirton et al., 2017; Gillick et al., 2018) may enhance therapy-induced gains in clinical function.

Our results here provide preliminary evidence that tSMS might provide an alternative application to achieve similar M1 inhibitory effects, although additional research exploring this is needed. The simplicity of tSMS, including potential ease of application to very young children, possibly paired with infant therapy in the home environment, is particularly appealing.

Our study has also established the tolerability and feasibility of tSMS in children. Rankings of tolerability were comparable with pediatric studies of motor cortex TMS and tDCS (Kirton et al., 2016; Cole et al., 2018; Zewdie et al., 2018). The most common side effects were mild headaches and neck pain. By both our observations and subject report, we believe many of these effects were largely due to the weight of the magnet itself. While our tSMS magnet weighed only 360 g (less than 1 pound), its mass was relatively highly concentrated on a small area of the skull due to its small diameter. Additional modifications to either better support the weight of the magnet (or otherwise redistribute the weight) may be helpful in improving tolerability further.

Several important limitations are noted. Our study was limited by an informed but modest sample size of 24 participants. Given the variability of TMS outcomes discussed above, larger sample sizes would certainly have been beneficial. Furthermore, with only 8 participants per treatment group, our ability to detect specific differences may have been reduced, emphasizing the need for our crossover design to be replicated in future studies. The crossover design did increase power for our neurophysiological outcomes but posed challenges for our clinical outcomes for which there may have been carry-over effects. Motor training on the PPT does not fully wash-out and can reach a plateau. As such, effect sizes on visits two and three were limited by previous motor training, and potentially by the intervention(s) received on prior visits. Therefore, potential motor learning effects of tSMS require additional studies designed primarily to assess behavioral outcomes. The 3-h study visits were tiring, especially for younger participants, although we tried to mitigate this with a snack break midway through each visit. We were not able to account for all factors that might have influenced our outcomes including fatigue, genetics, and attention (Li et al., 2015).

Ultimately, our data suggests that tSMS over M1 may modulate motor learning in children with specific effects of location and timing but this finding would benefit from further research. Our results also suggest that neurophysiological changes may differ in children compared to adults, and further research to determine neurophysiological effects of tSMS is required. Translationally, this study opens new opportunities for exploration into clinical trials of tSMS as a simple, non-invasive method to modulate motor learning in children with CP, with the ultimate goal of home-based, personalized, neuromodulation therapy during optimal windows of developmental plasticity.

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data generated and analyzed for this study is included in the article.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of Calgary Research Ethics Board. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants or participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

AHo contributed to the study design, recruitment of participants, data collection and analysis, and drafting and revising the manuscript. EZ contributed to the study design, data collection, data analysis, and revising the manuscript. HC, AHi, and H-CK contributed to the data collection, drafting and revising the manuscript. AN-A contributed to the data analysis and revising the manuscript. AK contributed to the obtaining funding, study design, and revising the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final version for submission.

Funding

This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research [grant number FDN-143294].

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Support from Adrianna Giuffre, Brandon Craig, Zahra Ofoghi, Dion Kelly, Anju Hollis, Shaelene Standing, Daria Merrikh, Julia Batycky, Tanaeem Rehman, Hana Osman and Susana Puche-Saud for their generous assistance with study sessions is gratefully acknowledged. We thank Ion Robu for all of his hard work assisting with designing and creating the helmet used to apply tSMS. We also thank Dr. Oliviero and his team for kindly supplying the sham magnet.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.hiccupkids.ca

References

Batsikadze, G., Moliadze, V., Paulus, W., Kuo, M.-F., and Nitsche, M. A. (2013). Partially non-linear stimulation intensity-dependent effects of direct current stimulation on motor cortex excitability in humans. J. Physiol. 591(Pt 7), 1987–2000. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.249730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bikson, M., Grossman, P., Thomas, C., Zannou, A. L., Jiang, J., Adnan, T., et al. (2016). Safety of transcranial direct current stimulation: evidence based update 2016. Brain Stimulat. 9, 641–661. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butefisch, C. M., Davis, B. C., Wise, S. P., Sawaki, L., Kopylev, L., Classen, J., et al. (2000). Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex.(Brief Article). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97:3661. doi: 10.1073/pnas.050350297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco-López, C., Soto-León, V., Céspedes, V., Profice, P., Strange, B. A., Foffani, G., et al. (2017). Static magnetic field stimulation over parietal cortex enhances somatosensory detection in humans. J. Neurosci. 37:3840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2123-16.2017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciechanski, P., Carlson, H. L., Yu, S. S., and Kirton, A. (2018). Modeling transcranial direct-current stimulation-induced electric fields in children and adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:268. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciechanski, P., and Kirton, A. (2017). Transcranial direct-current stimulation can enhance motor learning in children. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2758–2767. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciechanski, P., Zewdie, E., and Kirton, A. (2017). Developmental profile of motor cortex transcallosal inhibition in children and adolescents. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 140–148. doi: 10.1152/jn.00076.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirillo, J., Rogasch, N., and Semmler, J. (2010). Hemispheric differences in use-dependent corticomotor plasticity in young and old adults. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 57–68. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2332-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, L., Giuffre, A., Ciechanski, P., Carlson, H. L., Zewdie, E., Kuo, H.-C., et al. (2018). Effects of high-definition and conventional transcranial direct-current stimulation on motor learning in children. Front. Neurosci. 12:787. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daligadu, J., Murphy, B., Brown, J., Rae, B., and Yielder, P. (2013). TMS stimulus–response asymmetry in left- and right-handed individuals. Exp. Brain Res. 224, 411–416. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3320-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darling, W. G., Wolf, S. L., and Butler, A. J. (2006). Variability of motor potentials evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation depends on muscle activation. Exp. Brain Res. 174, 376–385. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0468-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dileone, M., Mordillo-Mateos, L., Oliviero, A., and Foffani, G. (2018). Long-lasting effects of transcranial static magnetic field stimulation on motor cortex excitability. Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodul. 11, 676–688. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, M., and Kirton, A. (2018). Perinatal stroke: mechanisms, management, and outcomes of early cerebrovascular brain injury. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2, 666–676. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30173-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsner, B., Kwakkel, G., Kugler, J., and Mehrholz, J. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving capacity in activities and arm function after stroke: a network meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 14:95. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0301-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyre, J. A. (2007). Corticospinal tract development and its plasticity after perinatal injury. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 1136–1149. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyre, J. A., Smith, M., Dabydeen, L., Clowry, G. J., Petacchi, E., Battini, R., et al. (2007). Is hemiplegic cerebral palsy equivalent to amblyopia of the corticospinal system? Ann. Neurol. 62, 493–503. doi: 10.1002/ana.21108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foffani, G., and Dileone, M. (2017). No modulatory effects by tSMS when delivered during a cognitive task. Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodul. 10:867. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.04.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friel, K. M., Chakrabarty, S., and Martin, J. H. (2013). Pathophysiological mechanisms of impaired limb use and repair strategies for motor systems after unilateral injury of the developing brain. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 55, 27–31. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friel, K. M., Gordon, A. M., Carmel, J. B., Kirton, A., and Gillick, B. T. (2016). “Pediatric issues in neuromodulation: safety, tolerability and ethical considerations,” in Pediatric Brain Stimulation: Mapping and Modulating the Developing Brain, eds A. Kirton and D. L. Gilbert (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 475.

Google Scholar

Friel, K. M., Williams, P. T. J. A., Serradj, N., Chakrabarty, S., and Martin, J. H. (2014). Activity-based therapies for repair of the corticospinal system injured during development. Front. Neurol. 5:229. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, R. A., and Broman, M. (1979). The purdue pegboard: normative data on 1334 school children. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 8, 156–162. doi: 10.1080/15374417909532912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garvey, M. A., and Gilbert, D. L. (2004). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in children. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 8, 7–19.

Google Scholar

Garvey, M. A., and Mall, V. (2008). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in children. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 973–984. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.11.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillick, B., Rich, T., Nemanich, S., Chen, C.-Y., Menk, J., Mueller, B., et al. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation and constraint-induced therapy in cerebral palsy: a randomized, blinded, sham-controlled clinical trial. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. EJPN Off. J. Eur. Paediatr. Neurol. Soc. 22, 358–368. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2018.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillick, B. T., Krach, L. E., Feyma, T., Rich, T. L., Moberg, K., Menk, J., et al. (2015). Safety of primed repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and modified constraint-induced movement therapy in a randomized controlled trial in pediatric hemiparesis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 96(4 Suppl.), S104–S113. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillick, B. T., Krach, L. E., Feyma, T., Rich, T. L., Moberg, K., Thomas, W., et al. (2014). Primed low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and constraint-induced movement therapy in pediatric hemiparesis: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 56, 44–52. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Rosa, J. J., Soto-Leon, V., Real, P., Carrasco-Lopez, C., Foffani, G., Strange, B. A., et al. (2015). Static magnetic field stimulation over the visual cortex increases alpha oscillations and slows visual search in humans. J. Neurosci. 35:9182. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4232-14.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermsen, A. M., Haag, A., Duddek, C., Balkenhol, K., Bugiel, H., Bauer, S., et al. (2016). Test–retest reliability of single and paired pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation parameters in healthy subjects. J. Neurol. Sci. 362, 209–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2016.01.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, W. Y., Cheng, C. H., Liao, K. K., Lee, I. H., and Lin, Y. Y. (2012). Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on motor functions in patients with stroke: a meta-analysis. Stroke 43, 1849–1857. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.649756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection (2009). Guidelines on limits of exposure to static magnetic fields. Health Phys. 96, 505–511. doi: 10.1097/01.HP.0000343164.27920.4a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirimoto, H., Asao, A., Tamaki, H., and Onishi, H. (2016). Non-invasive modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials by the application of static magnetic fields over the primary and supplementary motor cortices. Sci. Rep. 6:34509. doi: 10.1038/srep34509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirton, A. (2013a). Life after perinatal stroke. Stroke 44, 3265–3271. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.000739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirton, A. (2013b). Modeling developmental plasticity after perinatal stroke: defining central therapeutic targets in cerebral palsy. Pediatr. Neurol. 48, 81–94. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirton, A., Andersen, J., Herrero, M., Nettel-Aguirre, A., Carsolio, L., Damji, O., et al. (2016). Brain stimulation and constraint for perinatal stroke hemiparesis: the PLASTIC CHAMPS trial. Neurology 86, 1659–1667. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirton, A., Ciechanski, P., Zewdie, E., Andersen, J., Nettel-Aguirre, A., Carlson, H., et al. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation for children with perinatal stroke and hemiparesis. Neurology 88, 259–267. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kufner, M., Brückner, S., and Kammer, T. (2017). No modulatory effects by transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of human motor and somatosensory cortex. Brain Stimulat. 10, 703–710. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L. M., Uehara, K., and Hanakawa, T. (2015). The contribution of interindividual factors to variability of response in transcranial direct current stimulation studies. Front. Cell Neurosci. 9:181. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lozano-Soto, E., Soto-León, V., Sabbarese, S., Ruiz-Alvarez, L., Sanchez-del-Rio, M., Aguilar, J., et al. (2017). Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) of the visual cortex decreases experimental photophobia. Cephalalgia 38, 1493–1497. doi: 10.1177/0333102417736899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, J. H., Chakrabarty, S., and Friel, K. M. (2011). Harnessing activity-dependent plasticity to repair the damaged corticospinal tract in an animal model of cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 53(Suppl. 4), 9–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04055.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, J. H., Friel, K. M., Salimi, I., and Chakrabarty, S. (2007). Activity- and use-dependent plasticity of the developing corticospinal system. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 1125–1135. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsugi, A., and Okada, Y. (2017). Cerebellar transcranial static magnetic field stimulation transiently reduces cerebellar brain inhibition. Funct. Neurol. 32, 77–82. doi: 10.11138/fneur/2017.32.2.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moliadze, V., Schmanke, T., Andreas, S., Lyzhko, E., Freitag, C. M., and Siniatchkin, M. (2015). Stimulation intensities of transcranial direct current stimulation have to be adjusted in children and adolescents. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1392–1399. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.10.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monte-Silva, K., Kuo, M.-F., Hessenthaler, S., Fresnoza, S., Liebetanz, D., Paulus, W., et al. (2013). Induction of late LTP-Like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimulat. 6, 424–432. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muellbacher, W., Ziemann, U., Boroojerdi, B., Cohen, L., and Hallett, M. (2001). Role of the human motor cortex in rapid motor learning. Exp. Brain Res. 136, 431–438. doi: 10.1007/s002210000614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakagawa, K., and Nakazawa, K. (2018). Static magnetic field stimulation applied over the cervical spinal cord can decrease corticospinal excitability in finger muscle. Clin. Neurophysiol. Pract. 3, 49–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cnp.2018.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nojima, I., Koganemaru, S., Fukuyama, H., and Mima, T. (2015). Static magnetic field can transiently alter the human intracortical inhibitory system. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 2314–2319. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nojima, I., Koganemaru, S., and Mima, T. (2016). Combination of static magnetic fields and peripheral nerve stimulation can alter focal cortical excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:598. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nojima, I., Watanabe, T., Gyoda, T., Sugata, H., Ikeda, T., and Mima, T. (2019). Transcranial static magnetic stimulation over the primary motor cortex alters sequential implicit motor learning. Neurosci. Lett. 696, 33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliviero, A., Carrasco-López, M. C., Campolo, M., Perez-Borrego, Y. A., Soto-León, V., Gonzalez-Rosa, J. J., et al. (2015). Safety study of transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) of the human cortex. Brain Stimulat. 8, 481–485. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliviero, A., Mordillo-Mateos, L., Arias, P., Panyavin, I., Foffani, G., and Aguilar, J. (2011). Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 589(Pt 20), 4949–4958. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.211953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oskoui, M., Messerlian, C., Blair, A., Gamache, P., and Shevell, M. (2016). Variation in cerebral palsy profile by socio-economic status. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 58, 160–166. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, J., and Fritsch, B. (2011). Modulation of motor performance and motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 24, 590–596. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834c3db0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, J., Schambra, H. M., Cohen, L. G., Buch, E. R., Fritsch, B., Zarahn, E., et al. (2009). Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1590–1595. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805413106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridding, M. C., and Ziemann, U. (2010). Determinants of the induction of cortical plasticity by non-invasive brain stimulation in healthy subjects. J. Physiol. 588, 2291–2304. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.190314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, A. D. (2003). Mechanism of action of moderate-intensity static magnetic fields on biological systems. Cell Biochem. Biophys. 39, 163–173. doi: 10.1385/CBB:39:2:163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schambra, H. M., Ogden, R. T., Martínez-Hernández, I. E., Lin, X., Chang, Y. B., Rahman, A., et al. (2015). The reliability of repeated TMS measures in older adults and in patients with subacute and chronic stroke. Front. Cell Neurosci. 9:335. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. F., Altman, D. G., Moher, D., and CONSORT Group (2010). CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ 340:c332. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silbert, B. I., Pevcic, D. D., Patterson, H. I., Windnagel, K. A., and Thickbroom, G. W. (2013). Inverse correlation between resting motor threshold and corticomotor excitability after static magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Brain Stimulat. 6, 817–820. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staudt, M. (2007a). Reorganization of the developing human brain after early lesions. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49:564. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00564.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staudt, M. (2007b). (Re-)organization of the developing human brain following periventricular white matter lesions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 1150–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tharayil, J. J., Goetz, S. M., Bernabei, J. M., and Peterchev, A. V. (2018). Field distribution of transcranial static magnetic stimulation in realistic human head model. Neuromodul. Technol. Neural Interf. 21, 340–347. doi: 10.1111/ner.12876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiffin, J., and Asher, E. J. (1948). The Purdue pegboard; norms and studies of reliability and validity. J. Appl. Psychol. 32, 234–247.

Google Scholar

van Osch, M. J. P., and Webb, A. G. (2014). Safety of ultra-high field mri: what are the specific risks? Curr. Radiol. Rep. 2:61. doi: 10.1007/s40134-014-0061-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vines, B. W., Nair, D. G., and Schlaug, G. (2006). Contralateral and ipsilateral motor effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport 17, 671–674. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200604240-00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, T.-C., Lall, S., Pagnotta, C., Markward, J., Gupta, D., Ratnadurai-Giridharan, S., et al. (2018). Plasticity in one hemisphere, control from two: adaptation in descending motor pathways after unilateral corticospinal injury in neonatal rats. Front. Neural Circ. 12:28. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fregni, F. (2010). Interhemispheric modulation induced by cortical stimulation and motor training. Phys. Ther. 90, 398–410. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zewdie, E., Ciechanski, P., Kuo, H.-C., Giuffre, A., Cole, L., Seeger, T., et al. (2018). F150 Non-invasive brain stimulation is safe in children: evidence from 3 million stimulations. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, e123–e124. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.04.313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zewdie, E., and Kirton, A. (2016). “TMS basics: single and paired pulse neurophysiology,” in Pediatric Brain Stimulation: Mapping and Modulating the Developing Brain, eds A. Kirton and D. L. Gilbert (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 475.

Google Scholar

Keywords: non-invasive brain stimulation, neurophysiology, pediatrics, neuromodulation, motor learning, tSMS

Citation: Hollis A, Zewdie E, Nettel-Aguirre A, Hilderley A, Kuo H-C, Carlson HL and Kirton A (2020) Transcranial Static Magnetic Field Stimulation of the Motor Cortex in Children. Front. Neurosci. 14:464. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00464

Received: 10 September 2019; Accepted: 15 April 2020;
Published: 19 May 2020.

Edited by:

Davide Valeriani, Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Wei-Peng Teo, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Isabelle Buard, University of Colorado Denver, United States

Copyright © 2020 Hollis, Zewdie, Nettel-Aguirre, Hilderley, Kuo, Carlson and Kirton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adam Kirton, adam.kirton@albertahealthservices.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.