Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Public Health, 26 October 2016
Sec. Environmental health and Exposome

Exploratory Analysis of the Links among Food Consumption Profiles, Prenatal Androgens, and Selected Measures of Quality of Life

  • Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland

Prenatal sex steroids play a vital role in the development of the whole organism, and therefore also the brain. Exposure of the fetus to testosterone seems to be of special importance both for typical development and pathology. The key factor impacting offspring development (including prenatal androgen levels) appears to be diet, both in terms of shortage and excessive intake of certain food products. Prenatal steroid levels are measured using the ratio of the lengths of the second and fourth fingers (2D:4D). So far, the digit ratio (2D:4D) has been shown to correlate negatively with prenatal testosterone and positively with prenatal estrogen. Numerous correlational studies found relationships between the 2D:4D phenotype and differences in magnitude of many psychological traits. Certain social and demographic variables also correlate with the digit ratio. The present paper offers a preliminary analysis of correlations between diet, prenatal hormones’ levels (established based on the digit ratio), and selected social variables. One of the findings is that countries with high meat consumption present the so-called masculine digit ratio, while countries with plant-based diets – a feminine digit ratio.

Introduction

Since the ground-breaking work on organizational vs. activational effects of hormones (1), a significant amount of accumulated data confirm the relationship between sexual dimorphism (and differences in sexual characteristics within the same sex) and fetal exposure to prenatal sex steroids (prenatal estrogens and prenatal androgens) (25). Fetal testosterone level is thought to be particularly important (6, 7). Individual variations in testosterone levels tend to be associated with a number of internal and external factors. In animals producing large litters, the level of prenatal testosterone depends to a large extent on the number and position of male offspring in a given litter (8). External factors are hypothesized to be the effects on the pregnant female of a variety of substances in her environment, such as industrial by-products and compounds of e.g., plant origin (phytohormones). Some of these substances can affect the hormonal balance of the mother and the fetus (9). Multiple researchers have reported correlations between pesticides, such as DDT or DDE, and child development (10, 11). Another example is a component of commonly used plastics called biophenol A, and its effects on changes in the rate of sexual development and reproductive functions (12). Prenatal testosterone levels are also affected by maternal smoking (13). Stress and malnourishment during pregnancy can also change endogenous hormone concentration (14). Nevertheless, the key factor impacting offspring development (including the levels of prenatal androgens) appears to be diet, both in terms of shortage and excessive intake of certain products (1421). Since diet is subjected to dynamic changes resulting from increasing availability and supply of food products, it appears to be a key environmental influence on human physiology, including fetal development. Diet is also a reflection of social status (22, 23). Higher-quality diets are consumed by better educated and more affluent groups, and lower quality diets prevail in groups of lower socioeconomic status (22). Data published by Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO) highlight the key role of changes in dietary profiles in economically growing countries, consisting, among others, in a significantly higher consumption of meat – see also Ref. (24). It seems that other components of diets also affect nutritional profiles of entire nations. A probable relationship between national dietary profiles and digit ratio patterns is the focus of attention in the present article.

In order to estimate fetal hormone levels in particular groups, various indicators are taken into consideration. One widely used biomarker of prenatal sex steroids’ levels is the digit ratio (2528). The digit ratio is the relative length of the second (index) and fourth (ring) finger (2D:4D). The digit ratio has been shown to be negatively correlated with prenatal testosterone and positively with prenatal estrogen (29). Sexual dimorphism in digit ratio emerges in utero (30, 31). It is hypothesized that the genes that control urogenital development can also affect limb formation (32). On average, men have lower digit ratios than women (7). The relationship between the levels of prenatal sex steroids and digit ratio has been empirically validated, e.g., in studies involving hormonal manipulation (33, 34). More evidence comes from research on autism, Asperger’s syndrome, hyperactivity, etc. (3537). Digit ratio is related to conditions where prenatal testosterone differs from normal levels or there are differences in sensitivity to testosterone. This includes CAH, which is related to high prenatal androgen and low 2D:4D (38, 39), androgen insensitivity, which is related to high 2D:4D (40) and Klinefelter’s syndrome, which is associated with low levels of prenatal testosterone and high 2D:4D (4143). Individual differences in digit ratio are hereditary (44, 45). Although there are differences between ethnic populations, sexual dimorphism in digit ratio is universal (46, 47).

Many correlational studies have found correlations between the 2D:4D phenotype and differences in levels of multiple sex-dependant psychological traits (27, 4850). For example, individuals with low digit ratio score high on dominance scale (49), mainly in terms of aggressive dominance (51). This confirms the hypothesis that high level of prenatal testosterone and/or low level of prenatal estrogen is associated with dominance. Relative concentration of prenatal testosterone and prenatal estrogen is thought to affect reproduction, pubertal development, strength and fitness, aggression and social behavior (7, 25, 52, 53), as well as attractiveness for the opposite sex (54).

Certain social and demographic variables also correlate with digit ratio. For example, in societies where women have lower and men higher 2D:4D than expected for sex, there is a higher representation of women in parliament and greater female workforce participation (26). This is associated with high dominance scores in women (49). This factor may influence greater accessibility of politics and job market to women. Similar results are observed in studies on women employed in professions considered to be typically masculine. These women also have a lower than expected 2D:4D (55). Furthermore, rich societies have a negative res2D:4D (2D:4D in women is similar to 2D:4D in men). It seems that greater sex equality enables more women to work, which is particularly important in the aging Western societies. This may indicate that high testosterone in utero is associated with the formation of characteristics, preferences, and abilities required in traditionally masculine professions.

Materials and Methods

The aim of this paper is preliminary exploration of the relationships between diet, prenatal hormones (measured by digit ratio), and social functioning characteristics. In order to evaluate the possible effect of the main environmental factor, such as diet profile, in shaping the sexual dimorphism in the general populations across the countries, the multivariate approach has been adopted. The profile of the food consumption involves various food components, and therefore, all basic aspects of consumption should be taken into account. The first stage of the analysis involves clustering of countries into the homogeneous groups on the basis of their profile of food consumption (wheat, poultry, etc., consumption per capita per year). The second stage is a description of clusters, which is supposed to provide a general picture of how people utilize the food sources in their countries. This stage of analysis includes also the comparison of the groups of countries in terms of the sexual dimorphism measured by 2D:4D. Although this kind of analysis does not allow to formulate any casual explanations, it may provide a valuable hint for more detailed research on the role of diet in prenatal androgens level regulation.

Dietary profiles of selected countries were analyzed. For the analysis were selected countries for which there are reliable data on digit ratio in their populations (26). The analysis of dietary profiles was based on the most popular and common to all these countries food products such as meat, eggs, milk, wheat, vegetables, and fruits. Products consumed only in certain countries or eaten in small amounts have been excluded from the analysis. In order to extract general style of food consumption, a multivariate approach was adopted. The cluster analysis (K-means method) was administered in order to group countries presenting similar food consumption profiles.

Information on basic food products and alcohol intake was obtained from the Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations database.1 Data from the years 2000–2011 were taken into account.

Extracted in the cluster analysis groups of countries were compared in terms of the digit ratio profile, which dominates in the country’s population. Digit ratio values were sourced from Manning et al. (26). They published a chart with digit ratios of nationals of 29 countries (including non-European ones). The list presents mean digit ratios for both hands and both sexes, and residuals (res2D:4D) for either sex calculated as residuals of the regression female 2D:4D on male 2D:4D and considered to be the measure of per nation sex differences. Negative residuals indicate lower than expected 2D:4D in women compared to men, positive ones higher than expected 2D:4D in women compared to men.

The next step was to compare the extracted in the cluster analysis groups of countries in terms of demographic variables associated with the quality of life. The demographic variables previously described by Manning et al. (26) were excluding from the analysis. It seems that the quality of life level may be correlated with the nutritional profile of the country (e.g., through the relation between income and diet quality) and with its digit ratio profile (22, 26).

Demographic data were sourced from The World Factbook,2 databases published by the Central Intelligence Agency, covering fields such as history, people, government, economy, geography, communications, transportation, military, and transnational issues for 267 world entities. It is the most comprehensive and constantly updated database of demographic data. The database is used by a wide variety of folks including US Government officials, researchers, news organizations, corporations, geographers, teachers, professors, librarians, and students from all over the world. Information in The World Factbook is collected from a wide variety of US Government agencies, as well as from hundreds of published sources.

All data used for the analyses in this paper are compiled in additional materials.

Results

The data described above were analyzed using the multidimensional approach. The initial step was cluster analysis conducted on selected countries.

Stage I. Cluster Analysis

The analysis was conducted using the K-means method of clustering. The procedure used information on the intake of basic food products in 29 countries (26). Three clusters were identified. Cluster 1 (WSS = 27.041) is made up of 4 countries (Greece, Italy, Romania, and Turkey), cluster 2 (WSS = 67.711) of 14 countries (Austria, Belgium, Bulgaria, Croatia, Czech Republic, Denmark, France, Germany, Hungary, Netherlands, Poland, Spain, Sweden, and Switzerland), cluster 3 (WSS = 76.225) of 11 countries (Argentina, Australia, Canada, Finland, Iceland, Ireland, New Zealand, Norway, Portugal, United Kingdom, and United States).

Table 1 shows the mean individual food product consumption in each of the three identified clusters. Two products included in the analysis did not discriminate between the clusters (milk and fruits). For all the remaining products, the differences in consumption between clusters were statistically significant.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Food supply (kilograms per capita per year) in each cluster.

Based on the above data, consumption profiles of basic food products were established for each cluster. Cluster 1 is characterized by high intake of wheat and vegetables (almost two times higher than the two other clusters) and the lowest consumption of meat (all types) of all clusters. Eggs consumption is low, similar to that for cluster 3.

In cluster 2, pork (pig meat) was the most consumed type. The consumption of eggs was also the highest of all three clusters. The values for foods such as poultry, fish, bovine, and wheat were in the middle, although in the case of bovine, fish, and poultry, they approximate the low values in cluster 1, while wheat consumption is similar to the lowest value in cluster 3.

Cluster 3 features a particularly high consumption of meat (bovine, poultry, and fish), with the figures significantly higher than in the remaining clusters. The consumption of wheat and vegetables is the lowest of all analyzed clusters. The statistics for eggs are also close to the lowest value of cluster 1.

Stage II. Cluster Comparisons

The next step of the analysis was to compare the three extracted clusters in terms of digit ratio and demographic values. Fisher F test was used for cluster comparisons.

Digit Ratio

The clusters were compared in terms of digit ratio (26) – Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Digit ratio values in each cluster – based on Manning et al. (26).

The clusters differed in the following parameters: left hand digit ratio in men (L2D:4D M) and women (L2D:4D F), right hand digit ratio in women (R2D:4D F), and residuals from the left hand digit ratio analysis (resL2D:4D). Mean values reported by Manning et al. (26) for the total population of participants from 29 countries were as follows: 2D:4D right hand 0.984 ± 0.003 for men and 0.994 ± 0.004 for women; 2D:4D left hand: 0.985 ± 0.002 for men and 0.993 ± 0.004 for women.

In cluster 1, L2D:4D M values were in the middle for clusters, but still higher than the mean value for men. In women, both L2D:4D and R2D:4D were the highest of all analyzed clusters and higher than the population mean. The resL2D:4D value was positive and higher than in the other clusters.

L2D:4D of men in cluster 2 was the highest and higher than the general population mean compared to the other clusters. L2D:4D F and R2D:4D F values were average relative to the remaining clusters and approximated the population means. The value of resL2D:4D was negative but close to zero, which is the intermediate value out of the three clusters.

In cluster 3, L2D:4D for men was the lowest of all clusters of interest and close to the population mean. In women, both L2D:4D and R2D:4D values were the lowest among the three clusters, lower even than the mean for men. The resL2D:4D value was negative, the lowest of all clusters.

Demographic Variables

Since previous studies have indicated that the digit ratio is related to social variables (26, 52, 55), the three clusters were analyzed for differences in terms of such variables (Table 3). Differences between clusters were found for the following variables: Sex ratio in a population (sex ratio); gender parity index (girls to boys) in primary level enrollment (GPI_primary); gender parity index (girls to boys) in secondary level enrollment (GPI_secondary); gender parity index (girls to boys) in tertiary level enrollment (GPI_tertiary); median age in male (Mdn_age_M); median age in female (Mdn_age_F); infant mortality rate (deaths/1,000 live births) in male (IMR_M); infant mortality rate (deaths/1,000 live births) in female (IMR_F); birthrate (births/1,000 population); death rate (deaths/1,000 population); total fertility rate – children born/woman (TFR); military expenditures of GDP% (Military_exp); education expenditures of GDP% (Education_exp); health expenditures of GDP% (Health_exp); alcoholic beverages supply quantity (kilograms per capita per year) (alcohol).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Demographic variables in each cluster.

In terms of demographics, cluster 1 stood out due to very high (almost twice as high as the other clusters) infant mortality rate (difference significant for females), low GPI in secondary level enrollment, and the lowest education expenditures among clusters. In addition, alcoholic beverages supply was the lowest. The other variables are the middle values among the three clusters, with TFR close to the lowest value recorded in cluster 2.

Of note in cluster 2 are the highest median age in male and female of all clusters, the lowest birthrate and highest death rate, the lowest TFR and highest alcoholic beverages supply. Infant mortality rate (difference statistically significant for females) is the average for clusters, although similar to the lowest value in cluster 3.

Cluster 3 has a low (and the lowest of all clusters) infant mortality rate (difference statistically significant for females), the highest birthrate and lowest death rate, as well as the highest TFR (significantly higher than in the other two clusters). Median age in males and females is the lowest of all analyzed clusters. GPI in secondary level enrollment is the highest among clusters and greater than one, meaning that there are more women at this stage of education. Education expenditure is also the highest among the three clusters.

Discussion

By means of cluster analysis, it was possible to identify three groups of countries that differ in the consumption of basic food products, i.e., meat, wheat, etc. Cluster 1, which includes countries in the Mediterranean and Black Sea region, is characterized by a plant-based diet (wheat and vegetables) and low consumption of animal source foods (all types of meat plus eggs). Cluster 3 includes mainly Scandinavian, Northern European, and large non-European countries (such as Canada, Australia, and USA). Most of them are high-income Western countries, with wide access to sea. The diet in these countries is based on meat (mainly bovine, poultry, and fish), with low consumption of plant products (wheat and vegetables) and eggs. Cluster 2 includes Central and Western European countries. They exhibit high consumption of pig meat and eggs, which constitute the main components of the diet. However, overall meat consumption is considerably lower than in cluster 3.

The next step of the analysis revealed that the clusters differed not only in the diet type but also in terms of morphometric data, in this case, the digit ratio. The differences were seen mostly in left hand 2D:4D, since the right hand only discriminated clusters for women. Detailed analysis of clusters showed that countries with predominantly plant-based diets (cluster 1) have high digit ratio values both in males and females, i.e., low level of prenatal estrogen and/or high level of prenatal testosterone. By contrast, in countries with mainly meat-based diets (cluster 3), the male pattern of digit ratio is dominant in both sexes, suggesting high prenatal estrogen and/or low prenatal testosterone exposure in utero. Cluster 2 countries have average parameters for the clusters of interest, with sex-typical digit ratios.

These findings show an important correlation between the type of diet and digit ratio distribution in a population. A protein-rich, meat-based diet appears to be associated with masculine 2D:4D, while plant-based diet with feminine 2D:4D. Since the digit ratio is believed to be set in utero through the effects of prenatal sex steroids (2931), we can conclude that diet and levels of these hormones are indeed related. Specifically, a high-protein diet would be associated with high level of prenatal testosterone (and/or low level of prenatal estrogen), while a plant-based one – with low level of prenatal estrogen (and/or high level of prenatal testosterone). These conclusions are consistent with other findings on the effects of diet on sexual characteristics [e.g., for protein – (21); phytoestrogens – (56, 57)].

In the analysis of the relationship between digit ratio and demographic variables, Manning et al. (26) focused mainly on gender inequality indices. The analysis of differences between cluster discussed in the present paper identified additional variables that may corroborate that relationship.

The most salient figures in cluster 1 are high infant mortality rate, low TFR, the lowest participation of girls in education, and low education expenditures. These indices analyzed in the context of other variables, such as a plant-based diet and the feminine digit ratio add to the picture of this group of countries as less wealthy and underdeveloped relative to other clusters. The opposite demographic data describe cluster 3: the lowest infant mortality rate along with the highest TFR, education expenditures, and proportion of girls in education. The affluence of this group of countries is also reflected in their meat-based diets. These variables are correlated with the masculine digit ratio. Cluster 2 values for these demographic data are in the middle. On the other hand, these countries have the highest median age for both sexes and low TFR, confirming the claim that the population of Central and Western Europe is aging.

Conclusion

This paper presents preliminary analyses of correlations between population diet profile (a macro-environmental factor) and sexual dimorphism measured by 2D:4D ratio. In countries with high consumption of meat, male-like pattern of digit ratio is observed in both sexes. It may suggest that, in these countries, a meat-based diet is followed by males and females. The same countries appear to be the most developed and wealthy. It must be noted, however, that it is not possible, on the basis of this type of data, to pinpoint the mechanisms underlying these relationships. Especially, that all analyzed variables are interlinked on multiple levels.

The existing data support the presence of the processes running at various levels that have a significant impact on the evolution of contemporary human societies. In the light of the findings confirming the fast pace of evolutionary changes in Homo sapiens (58), the role of diet, as one of the factors affecting the fitness of the human population, should probably be reevaluated from a multidisciplinary and multigenerational perspective.

Author Contributions

Both authors contributed to study design, data analysis, and manuscript writing.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer KS and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Funding

This paper has been supported by National Science Center in Poland, grant #UMO-2015/17/B/HS6/04206.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpubh.2016.00240/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

1. Pheoenix CH, Goy RW, Gerall AA, Young WC. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology (1959) 65(5):369–82. doi: 10.1210/endo-65-3-369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Fitch RH, Denenberg VH. A role for ovarian hormones in sexual differentiation of the brain. Behav Brain Sci (1998) 21(03):311–52. doi:10.1017/S0140525X98001216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hines M. Gender development and the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci (2011) 34:69–88. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sisk CL, Zehr JL. Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol (2005) 26:163–74. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Thornton J, Zehr JL, Loose MD. Effects of prenatal androgens on rhesus monkeys: a model system to explore the organizational hypothesis in primates. Horm Behav (2009) 55:633–45. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Clemens L, Gladue B, Coniglio L. Prenatal endogenous androgenic influences on masculine sexual behavior and genital morphology in male and female rats. Horm Behav (1978) 10(1):40–53. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(78)90023-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Manning JT. Resolving the role of prenatal sex steroids in the development of digit ratio. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2011) 108(39):16143–4. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113312108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zielinski WJ, vom Saal FS, Vandenbergh JG. The effect of intrauterine position on the survival, and home range size of female house mice (Mus musculus) reproduction. Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2012) 30(3):185–91. doi:10.1007/BF00166702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bigsby R, Chapin RE, Daston GP, Davis BJ, Gorski J, Gray LE, et al. Evaluating the effects of endocrine disruptors on endocrine function during development. Environ Health Perspect (1999) 107(Suppl 4):613–8. doi:10.1289/ehp.99107s4613

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ribas-Fitó N, Torrent M, Carrizo D, Muñoz-Ortiz L, Júlvez J, Grimalt JO, et al. In utero exposure to background concentrations of DDT and cognitive functioning among preschoolers. Am J Epidemiol (2006) 164(10):955–62. doi:10.1093/aje/kwj299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rogan WJ, Gladen BC, McKinney JD, Carreras N, Hardy P, Thullen J, et al. Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and dichlorodiphenyl dichloroethene (DDE) in human milk: effects on growth, morbidity, and duration of lactation. Am J Public Health (1987) 77(10):1294–7. doi:10.2105/AJPH.77.10.1294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Howdeshell KL, Hotchkiss AK, Thayer KA, Vandenbergh JG, vom Saal FS. Exposure to bisphenol A advances puberty. Nature (1999) 401(6755):763–4. doi:10.1038/44517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Rizwan S, Manning JT, Brabin BJ. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and possible effects of in utero testosterone: evidence from the 2D:4D finger length ratio. Early Hum Dev (2007) 83(2):87–90. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Prado EL, Dewey KG. Nutrition and brain development in early life. Nutr Rev (2014) 72(4):267–84. doi:10.1111/nure.12102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Anderson KE, Rosner W, Khan MS, New MI, Pang SY, Wissel PS, et al. Diet-hormone interactions: protein/carbohydrate ratio alters reciprocally the plasma levels of testosterone and cortisol and their respective binding globulins in man. Life Sci (1987) 40(18):1761–8. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(87)90086-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Férézou-Viala J, Roy A-F, Sérougne C, Gripois D, Parquet M, Bailleux V, et al. Long-term consequences of maternal high-fat feeding on hypothalamic leptin sensitivity and diet-induced obesity in the offspring. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol (2007) 293(3):R1056–62. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00117.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Passos MC, da Fonte Ramos C, Dutra SCP, Mouço T, de Moura EG. Long-term effects of malnutrition during lactation on the thyroid function of offspring. Horm Metab Res (2002) 34(1):40–3. doi:10.1055/s-2002-19966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Pisula W, Pisula E. Autism prevalence and meat consumption – a hypothesis that needs to be tested. Med Hypotheses (2014) 83(4):488–93. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2014.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sloboda DM, Hickey M, Hart R. Reproduction in females: the role of the early life environment. Hum Reprod Update (2011) 17(2):210–27. doi:10.1093/humupd/dmq048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Snoeck A, Remacle C, Reusens B, Hoet JJ. Effect of a low protein diet during pregnancy on the fetal rat endocrine pancreas. Biol Neonate (1990) 57(2):107–18. doi:10.1159/000243170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Zambrano E, Bautista CJ, Deás M, Martínez-Samayoa PM, González-Zamorano M, Ledesma H, et al. A low maternal protein diet during pregnancy and lactation has sex- and window of exposure-specific effects on offspring growth and food intake, glucose metabolism and serum leptin in the rat. J Physiol (2006) 571(Pt 1):221–30. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2005.100313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Does social class predict diet quality? Am J Clin Nutr (2008) 87(5):1107–17.

Google Scholar

23. Gossard MH, York R. Social structural influences on meat consumption. Hum Ecol Rev (2003) 10:1–9.

Google Scholar

24. Verbeke W, Pérez-Cueto FJA, Barcellos MD, Krystallis A, Grunert KG. European citizen and consumer attitudes and preferences regarding beef and pork. Meat Sci (2010) 84(2):284–92. doi:10.1016/j.meatsci.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Nelson E, Shultz S. Finger length ratios (2D:4D) in anthropoids implicate reduced prenatal androgens in social bonding. Am J Phys Anthropol (2010) 141(3):395–405. doi:10.1002/ajpa.21157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Manning J, Fink B, Trivers R. Digit ratio (2D: 4D) and gender inequalities across nations. Evol Psychol (2014) 12(4):757–68. doi:10.1177/147470491401200406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wakabayashi A, Nakazawa Y. On relationships between digit ratio (2D:4D) and two fundamental cognitive drives, empathizing and systemizing, in Japanese sample. Pers Individ Dif (2010) 49(8):928–31. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. McIntyre MH. The use of digit ratios as markers for perinatal androgen action. Reprod Biol Endocrinol (2006) 4:10. doi:10.1186/1477-7827-4-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Zheng Z, Cohn MJ. Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit ratios. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2011) 108(39):16289–94. doi:10.1073/pnas.1108312108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Galis F, Ten Broek CMA, Van Dongen S, Wijnaendts LCD. Sexual dimorphism in the prenatal digit ratio (2D:4D). Arch Sex Behav (2010) 39(1):57–62. doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9485-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Malas MA, Dogan S, Evcil EH, Desdicioglu K. Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D). Early Hum Dev (2006) 82(7):469–75. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod (1998) 13(11):3000–4. doi:10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Saino N, Rubolini D, Romano M, Boncoraglio G. Increased egg estradiol concentration feminizes digit ratios of male pheasants (Phasianus colchicus). Naturwissenschaften (2007) 94(3):207–12. doi:10.1007/s00114-006-0188-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Talarovicová A, Krsková L, Blazeková J. Testosterone enhancement during pregnancy influences the 2D:4D ratio and open field motor activity of rat siblings in adulthood. Horm Behav (2009) 55(1):235–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. de Bruin EI, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand RF. Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol (2006) 48(12):962. doi:10.1017/S0012162206002118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Manning JT, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Dev Med Child Neurol (2001) 43(3):160–4. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2001.tb00181.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Stevenson JC, Everson PM, Williams DC, Hipskind G, Grimes M, Mahoney ER. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and digit ratios in a college sample. Am J Hum Biol (2007) 19(1):41–50. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Brown WM, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM. Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav (2002) 42(4):380–6. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ökten A, Kalyoncu M, Yariş N. The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Early Hum Dev (2002) 70(1–2):47–54. doi:10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00073-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Berenbaum SA, Bryk KK, Nowak N, Quigley CA, Moffat S. Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure. Endocrinology (2009) 150(11):5119–24. doi:10.1210/en.2009-0774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Manning JT, Kilduff LP, Trivers R. Digit ratio (2D:4D) in Klinefelter’s syndrome. Andrology (2013) 1(1):94–9. doi:10.1111/j.2047-2927.2012.00013.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Savic I. Asymmetry of cerebral gray and white matter and structural volumes in relation to sex hormones and chromosomes. Front Neurosci (2014) 8:329. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Chang S, Skakkebaek A, Trolle C, Bojesen A, Hertz JM, Cohen A, et al. Anthropometry in Klinefelter syndrome – multifactorial influences due to CAG length, testosterone treatment and possibly intrauterine hypogonadism. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2015) 100(3):E508–17. doi:10.1210/jc.2014-2834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Forstmeier W. Quantitative genetics and behavioural correlates of digit ratio in the zebra finch. Proc Biol Sci (2005) 272(1581):2641–9. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Nelson E, Voracek M. Heritability of digit ratio (2D:4D) in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Primates (2010) 51(1):1–5. doi:10.1007/s10329-009-0173-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Fink B, Thanzami V, Seydel H, Manning JT. Digit ratio and hand-grip strength in German and Mizos men: cross-cultural evidence for an organizing effect of prenatal testosterone on strength. Am J Hum Biol (2006) 18(6):776–82. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Manning JT, Churchill AJG, Peters M. The effects of sex, ethnicity, and sexual orientation on self-measured digit ratio (2D:4D). Arch Sex Behav (2007) 36(2):223–33. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9171-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Chai XJ, Jacobs LF. Digit ratio predicts sense of direction in women. PLoS One (2012) 7(2):e32816. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Manning JT, Fink B. Digit ratio (2D:4D), dominance, reproductive success, asymmetry, and sociosexuality in the BBC Internet Study. Am J Hum Biol (2008) 20(4):451–61. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Manning JT, Fink B. Digit ratio (2D:4D) and aggregate personality scores across nations: data from the BBC internet study. Pers Individ Dif (2011) 51(4):387–91. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. van der Meij L, Almela M, Buunk AP, Dubbs S, Salvador A. 2D:4D in men is related to aggressive dominance but not to sociable dominance. Aggress Behav (2012) 38(3):208–12. doi:10.1002/ab.21422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hönekopp J, Voracek M, Manning JT. 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and number of sex partners: evidence for effects of prenatal testosterone in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2006) 31(1):30–7. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Manning JT, Fink B. Is low digit ratio linked with late menarche? Evidence from the BBC internet study. Am J Hum Biol (2011) 23(4):527–33. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Roney JR, Maestripieri D. Relative digit lengths predict men’s behavior and attractiveness during social interactions with women. Hum Nat (2004) 15(3):271–82. doi:10.1007/s12110-004-1009-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Manning JT, Reimers S, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Fink B. Sexually dimorphic traits (digit ratio, body height, systemizing-empathizing scores) and gender segregation between occupations: evidence from the BBC internet study. Pers Individ Dif (2010) 49(5):511–5. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kanno J, Kato H, Iwata T, Inoue T. Phytoestrogen-low diet for endocrine disruptor studies. J Agric Food Chem (2002) 50(13):3883–5. doi:10.1021/jf020235p

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Whitten PL, Naftolin F. Effects of a phytoestrogen diet on estrogen-dependent reproductive processes in immature female rats. Steroids (1992) 57(2):56–61. doi:10.1016/0039-128X(92)90029-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Templeton A. Has human evolution stopped? Rambam Maimonides Med J (2010) 1(1):1–10. doi:10.5041/RMMJ.10006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: diet, digit ratio, 2D:4D, prenatal sex steroids, meat consumption

Citation: Modlinska K and Pisula W (2016) Exploratory Analysis of the Links among Food Consumption Profiles, Prenatal Androgens, and Selected Measures of Quality of Life. Front. Public Health 4:240. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00240

Received: 14 June 2016; Accepted: 13 October 2016;
Published: 26 October 2016

Edited by:

Efstratios Vogiannis, Evangeliki Model School of Smyrna – Athens, Greece

Reviewed by:

David Paige Gilkey, Colorado State University, USA
Konstantina Svoli, Evangeliki Model School of Smyrna – Athens, Greece

Copyright: © 2016 Modlinska and Pisula. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wojciech Pisula, wojciech.pisula@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.