Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 17 July 2015
Sec. Motivation and Reward
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00186

Deafferented controllers: a fundamental failure mechanism in cortical neuroprosthetic systems

  • Movement Laboratory, Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK

Brain-machine interface (BMI) research assumes that patients with disconnected neural pathways could naturally control a prosthetic device by volitionally modulating sensorimotor cortical activity usually responsible for movement coordination. However, computational approaches to motor control challenge this view. This article examines the predictions of optimal feedback control (OFC) theory on the effects that loss of motor output and sensory feedback have on the normal generation of motor commands. Example simulations of unimpaired, totally disconnected, and deafferented controllers illustrate that by neglecting the dynamic interplay between motor commands, state estimation, feedback and behavior, current BMI systems face translational challenges rooted in a debatable assumption and experimental models of limited validity.

Introduction

Current BMI research (Bensmaia and Miller, 2014) aims at extracting movement parameters believed to be encoded in motor cortical areas that would enable neuroprosthetic control after peripheral disconnection. However, despite encouraging early laboratory monkey experiments (Taylor et al., 2002; Carmena et al., 2003; Moritz et al., 2008; Velliste et al., 2008; Ethier et al., 2012), cortical neuroprosthetic control (Hochberg et al., 2012; Collinger et al., 2013) is poor compared to natural movements and highly dependent on visual feedback. Understanding the mechanisms underlying poor BMI performance is the key to clinical translation; currently funded efforts focus on identifying failure mechanisms associated with degraded neural signal acquisition from chronically implanted electrode arrays, unreliable motor decoding of such neural signals, and delivery of poor sensory feedback (Miranda et al., 2015). This article considers a fundamental failure mechanism largely underappreciated by the BMI community: the assumption that sensorimotor cortical activity engaged during unimpaired movement remains available for prosthetic control after peripheral disconnection.

Especially relevant to BMI research, the OFC theory of motor coordination (Todorov and Jordan, 2002) highlights the dynamic interplay between motor commands and sensory feedback, discouraging ideas of neural coding/decoding and emphasizing neural dynamics when interpreting sensorimotor function (Scott, 2012; for a recent review of OFC and its possible biological underpinnings see Scott et al., 2015; Shenoy et al., 2013). This is supported by work demonstrating the influence of sensory feedback on ongoing M1 activity (Herter et al., 2009; Suminski et al., 2010; Pruszynski et al., 2011), studies describing changes in neural tuning at different time scales (Sergio et al., 2005; Hatsopoulos et al., 2007; Churchland et al., 2012) and under different feedback conditions (Gaunt et al., 2013), and work showing altered EEG movement-related information in the absence of kinaesthetic feedback (Galán et al., 2015).

The OFC framework further predicts that loss of motor output and sensory feedback will have an impact on the normal generation of motor commands; although these might still be initiated, they are likely to be altered compared with those responsible for producing unimpaired movement. Such a prediction poses relevant questions to BMI research that need addressing: is it possible at all after peripheral disconnection to use sensorimotor cortical activity for reliable cortical neuroprosthetic control? If not, what is needed? And, in such a scenario, how can studies with able-bodied experimental subjects contribute to advancing the development of reliable BMI systems? The following sections illustrate the predictions of an infinite-horizon OFC model (Qian et al., 2013) in situations with different types of peripheral disconnection, and discuss the implications for the BMI field.

Peripheral Disconnections in OFC

Figure 1 illustrates the OFC framework. To achieve a goal such as an arm movement, optimal feedback controllers generate motor commands that minimize a cost function (which might include error and energy expenditure) based on their belief about the current state of the arm and the world. Such a belief is formed by integrating through an optimal estimator the state changes observed through delayed afferent feedback with those predicted by forward models, which use knowledge about the system dynamics acting on a copy of the motor commands. Note that motor commands and sensory feedback are both susceptible to corruption by noise. References (Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Scott, 2012; Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 2012; Scott et al., 2015) provide further detail on the computational and neural basis of the OFC framework in voluntary motor control.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Optimal feedback control, putative neural substrates, and peripheral disconnections. Diagram illustrating OFC framework, its possible neural basis and both simulated peripheral disconnections: complete disconnection (red) and deafferentation (gray). See section “Peripheral disconnections in OFC” for details.

Understanding the causal links between different movement phases such as preparation and execution is especially relevant to cortical neuroprosthetic control (Shenoy et al., 2011). An interesting property of the infinite-horizon OFC model utilized here (Qian et al., 2013) is the integration of movement control and postural maintenance, without artificial separations. This occurs through the application of steady-state solutions for state estimation, movement execution and postural maintenance. The state estimator and controller are computed in a preparatory phase before movement starts, but motor commands can only be determined during the movement attempt as they depend on estimated states and sensory feedback. Therefore, movement properties such as duration and trajectory, as well as the motor commands themselves, emerge from the model with inherent trial-to-trial variability. In brief, Qian et al. (2013) consider a linear dynamical system governed by stochastic differential equations:

dx=(Ax+φBu)dt+Fxdβ+Yudγ+Gdω,    (1)
dy=Cxdt+Ddξ,    (2)

where x is the state n-vector, u is the control k-vector (motor command), and y is the sensory feedback vector. β and γ are scalar Wiener processes, and ω and ξ are n- and k-vector Wiener processes modeling noise in control and sensory feedback, all independent from each other and standardized so that over a time step dt, the corresponding Gaussian white noise processes have variance dt. A, B, C, D, F, G and Y are constant matrices; A and B define the motor plant according to Newtonian mechanics and the muscle force equation, C and D define observation and observation noise, F and Y define state- and control-dependent noise (multiplicative or signal dependent noise), and G defines control-independent noise. φ is a scalar modeling efferent disconnection: if the system remains intact then φ = 1, if otherwise motor commands are blocked then φ = 0. The current state x is not available for control and is estimated according to a linear Kalman filter (Kalman, 1960):

dx^=(Ax^+Bu)dt+K(dyCx^dt),    (3)

where the first term is the prediction based on a forward internal model and an efference copy of the control signal, and the second term is the correction based on the discrepancy between observed and predicted sensory feedback.

The motor command is a linear function of x^:

u=Lx^,    (4)

and estimator gain K and controller L are determined by optimizing costs. According to Phillis's infinite-horizon formulation (Phillis, 1985), the estimator cost is defined as the steady-state variance of the estimation error x˜xx^,

J1=limtE[x˜TUx˜],    (5)

and the controller cost as the steady-state cost per unit of time,

J2=E[limt1t0t(xTQx+uTRu)dt],    (6)

where the first term measures accuracy of the reaching target, the second term measures energetic cost, and matrices U, Q, and R are constant and symmetric.

The simulations reproduced unimpaired single-joint reaching movements of the forearm at the elbow as in (Qian et al., 2013), but additionally tested two different types of peripheral disconnection (see Figure 1): complete disconnection (disconnected, K = φ = 0) and deafferentation (deafferented, K = 0). First, estimator gain K and controller L were numerically obtained from an unimpaired controller as in (Qian et al., 2013); note that they only depend on plant and cost parameters. Then, optimal steady state estimator gain K and controller L were applied to move the hand (n = 20 trials) toward a target placed 50 cm away according to the system dynamics defined by unimpaired, disconnected, and deafferented conditions. (Matlab code obtained from N. Qian upon request and modified to simulate deafferentation and complete disconnection).

Figure 2 shows time-resolved profiles of hand position, motor command, and their variance with respect to the average, generated by the system dynamics defined by unimpaired, disconnected, and deafferented controllers. The model reproduced typically-observed properties of unimpaired movements with trial-to-trial variability (Qian et al., 2013) (Figures 2A–D, unimpaired). By contrast, the disconnected controller generated (Equation 4) highly stereotyped (Figures 2B,D; disconnected) motor commands based on estimates that solely rely on the predictions of a forward model using efference copy (Wolpert and Miall, 1996; Wolpert et al., 1998) (note that K = 0 in Equation 3). Such motor commands generated no overt output, however, due to the efferent block (Figures 2A,C; disconnected).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simulations of reaching movements produced by unimpaired, disconnected, and deafferented controllers. Simulated single-joint reaching movements of the forearm (n = 20) toward a target placed 50 cm from starting position. (A) Position (B) motor command (C) position variance with respect to the average. (D) Motor command variance with respect to the average.

These results seem to suggest that not only is the motor command intact in the disconnected state, but it is actually less variable than in the unimpaired condition. Does this mean that decoding of the motor command would lead to effective movements of a neural prosthetic? Such decoding can be modeled using a deafferented controller, where efferents remain connected, revealing what movements would be generated from the stereotyped command seen in the disconnected state. This revealed highly variable movement trajectories, resulting from the accumulation of errors over time in the state estimate which must rely, in the absence of sensory feedback, on only forward model predictions from efference copy. The monotonic increase of state variance reproduced movement profiles displayed by patients suffering from sensory neuropathies without visual feedback (Sanes et al., 1984; Gordon et al., 1995) (Figures 2A,C; deafferented). Paradoxically, the generation of motor commands with trial-to-trial variability ensures successful movements (Todorov and Jordan, 2002) (Figure 2 unimpaired), whereas the lack of such variability ensures movement imprecision (Figure 2 deafferented). These examples illustrate that the lack of sensory feedback from the periphery suffered by disconnected and deafferented controllers has an impact on the generation of motor commands via poor state estimation. Execution of such motor commands (e.g., by a decoder driving a neural prosthetic) unavoidably leads to poor motor control.

Discussion

Current BMI systems aim to extract neural information from sensorimotor cortical areas to enable prosthetic control to the paralyzed. However, the above examples illustrate that these areas, considered as optimal feedback controllers (see Figure 1; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Scott, 2012), are likely to generate motor commands leading to poor prosthetic control when disconnected from the periphery. An ideal bidirectional biomimetic approach which restored both sensory and motor function would theoretically allow for feedback-corrected state estimation and restore reliable control; recent work on hand prosthetics enabling effective modulation of grasping force by conveying nearly “natural” sensory feedback (Raspopovic et al., 2014) supports this notion. However, such interfacing at higher levels of the neuraxis is beyond current technical developments (Bensmaia and Miller, 2014). More feasible might be an implementation of bidirectional interfacing based on learned non-biomimetic associations (Bensmaia and Miller, 2014). Studies with able-bodied experimental subjects demonstrating volitional control of sensorimotor cortical activity (Fetz, 1969), neural adaptation during efferent BMI control (Jarosiewicz et al., 2008; Ganguly and Carmena, 2009) and discrimination of evoked percepts by intracortical microstimulation (ICMS) to somatosensory cortex (Romo et al., 1998; London et al., 2008; O'Doherty et al., 2011) all support that this approach may have merit, although it remains unknown whether this will deliver sufficiently rich sensory feedback to enable precise cortical neuroprosthetic control. At present, deafferented patients who only rely on visual feedback appear to provide an upper bound on current cortical neuroprosthetic systems' performance.

In Figure 1 we have assigned putative neural structures to each component of the OFC model. In reality the system is likely to be distributed, with a single box in that diagram implemented by multiple neural circuits at different levels of the neuraxis. Feedback is also distributed, being received for example by both spinal and cortical circuits. One consequence of such an arrangement may be that the full motor command can never be read out from one neural structure on its own.

How do the ideas presented above fit with experimental results from the BMI field? Two FDA clinical trials involving patients with paralysis (Hochberg et al., 2012; Collinger et al., 2013) showed impoverished cortical control compared to natural movements, in agreement with these concepts. Other than these reports, the majority of BMI work involve able-bodied non-human primates (Shenoy and Carmena, 2014), with intact sensory feedback loops from the periphery. In just a few instances was peripheral disconnection modeled with nerve blocks at upper arm (Moritz et al., 2008) and elbow (Pohlmeyer et al., 2009; Ethier et al., 2012), but even in those cases feedback above the block was intact. The ability to use residual sensorimotor loops has been previously demonstrated in amputees (Kuiken et al., 2007; Raspopovic et al., 2014), where access to motor representations of the missing limb appears conditional upon the re-establishment of peripheral connections (Reilly et al., 2006). Evidence for effective neuroprosthetic systems based on extracting control signals from sensorimotor cortical areas totally disconnected from the periphery is currently lacking. We argue that neuroprostheses which seek to exploit preserved sensorimotor loops may offer the most promising control alternatives on theoretical grounds.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust.

References

Bensmaia, S. J., and Miller, L. E. (2014). Restoring sensorimotor function through intracortical interfaces: progress and looming challenges. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 313–325. doi: 10.1038/nrn3724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmena, J. M., Lebedev, M. A., Crist, R. E., O'Doherty, J. E., Santucci, D. M., Dimitrov, D. F., et al. (2003). Learning to control a brain-machine interface for reaching and grasping by primates. PLoS Biol. 1:E42. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0000042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchland, M. M., Cunningham, J. P., Kaufman, M. T., Foster, J. D., Nuyujukian, P., Ryu, S. I., et al. (2012). Neural population dynamics during reaching. Nature 487, 51–56. doi: 10.1038/nature11129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collinger, J. L., Wodlinger, B., Downey, J. E., Wang, W., Tyler-Kabara, E. C., Weber, D. J., et al. (2013). High-performance neuroprosthetic control by an individual with tetraplegia. Lancet 381, 557–564. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61816-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ethier, C., Oby, E. R., Bauman, M. J., and Miller, L. E. (2012). Restoration of grasp following paralysis through brain-controlled stimulation of muscles. Nature 485, 368–371. doi: 10.1038/nature10987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fetz, E. E. (1969). Operant conditioning of cortical unit activity. Science 163, 955–958. doi: 10.1126/science.163.3870.955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galán, F., Baker, M. R., Alter, K., and Baker, S. N. (2015). Degraded EEG decoding of wrist movements in absence of kinaesthetic feedback. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 643–654. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganguly, K., and Carmena, J. M. (2009). Emergence of a stable cortical map for neuroprosthetic control. PLoS Biol. 7:e1000153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaunt, R. A., Collinger, J. L., Wodlinger, B., Weber, D. J., and Boninger, M. L. (2013). Propioceptive Feedback enables Brain Computer Interface (BCI) Controlled Prosthetic Arm Movement in the Absence of Visual Input. Program No. 374.12. 2013 Neuroscience Meeting Planner. Washington; San Diego: Society for Neuroscience.

Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., and Ghez, C. (1995). Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 347–360.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hatsopoulos, N. G., Xu, Q., and Amit, Y. (2007). Encoding of movement fragments in the motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 5105–5114. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3570-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herter, T. M., Korbel, T., and Scott, S. H. (2009). Comparison of neural responses in primary motor cortex to transient and continuous loads during posture. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 150–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.90230.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochberg, L. R., Bacher, D., Jarosiewicz, B., Masse, N. Y., Simeral, J. D., Vogel, J., et al. (2012). Reach and grasp by people with tetraplegia using a neurally controlled robotic arm. Nature 485, 372–375. doi: 10.1038/nature11076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarosiewicz, B., Chase, S. M., Fraser, G. W., Velliste, M., Kass, R. E., and Schwartz, A. B. (2008). Functional network reorganization during learning in a brain-computer interface paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 19486–19491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808113105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalman, R. E. (1960). A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. J. Fluids Eng. 82, 35–45. doi: 10.1115/1.3662552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuiken, T. A., Miller, L. A., Lipschutz, R. D., Lock, B. A., Stubblefield, K., Marasco, P. D., et al. (2007). Targeted reinnervation for enhanced prosthetic arm function in a woman with a proximal amputation: a case study. Lancet 369, 371–380. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60193-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

London, B. M., Jordan, L. R., Jackson, C. R., and Miller, L. E. (2008). Electrical stimulation of the proprioceptive cortex (Area 3a) used to instruct a behaving monkey. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 16, 32–36. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2007.907544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, R. A., Casebeer, W. D., Hein, A. M., Judy, J. W., Krotkov, E. P., Laabs, T. L., et al. (2015). DARPA-funded efforts in the development of novel brain-computer interface technologies. J. Neurosci. Methods 244, 52–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, C. T., Perlmutter, S. I., and Fetz, E. E. (2008). Direct control of paralysed muscles by cortical neurons. Nature 456, 639–642. doi: 10.1038/nature07418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Doherty, J. E., Lebedev, M. A., Ifft, P. J., Zhuang, K. Z., Shokur, S., Bleuler, H., et al. (2011). Active tactile exploration using a brain-machine-brain interface. Nature 479, 228–231. doi: 10.1038/nature10489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillis, Y. A. (1985). Controller design of systems with multiplicative noise. IEEE Trans. Automatic Control 30, 1017–1019. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1985.1103828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pohlmeyer, E. A., Oby, E. R., Perreault, E. J., Solla, S. A., Kilgore, K. L., Kirsch, R. F., et al. (2009). Toward the restoration of hand use to a paralyzed monkey: brain-controlled functional electrical stimulation of forearm muscles. PLoS ONE 4:e5924. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruszynski, J. A., Kurtzer, I., Nashed, J. Y., Omrani, M., Brouwer, B., and Scott, S. H. (2011). Primary motor cortex underlies multi-joint integration for fast feedback control. Nature 478, 387–390. doi: 10.1038/nature10436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qian, N., Jiang, Y., Jiang, Z. P., and Mazzoni, P. (2013). Movement duration, Fitts's law, and an infinite-horizon optimal feedback control model for biological motor systems. Neural Comput. 25, 697–724. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raspopovic, S., Capogrosso, M., Petrini, F. M., Bonizzato, M., Rigosa, J., Di Pino, G., et al. (2014). Restoring natural sensory feedback in real-time bidirectional hand prostheses. Sci. Transl. Med. 6:222ra19. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, K. T., Mercier, C., Schieber, M. H., and Sirigu, A. (2006). Persistent hand motor commands in the amputees' brain. Brain 129, 2211–2223. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romo, R., Hernandez, A., Zainos, A., and Salinas, E. (1998). Somatosensory discrimination based on cortical microstimulation. Nature 392, 387–390. doi: 10.1038/32891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanes, J. N., Mauritz, K. H., Evarts, E. V., Dalakas, M. C., and Chu, A. (1984). Motor deficits in patients with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 81, 979–982. doi: 10.1073/pnas.81.3.979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S. H. (2012). The computational and neural basis of voluntary motor control and planning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 541–549. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S. H., Cluff, T., Lowrey, C. R., and Takei, T. (2015). Feedback control during voluntary motor actions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergio, L. E., Hamel-Pâquet, C., and Kalaska, J. F. (2005). Motor cortex neural correlates of output kinematics and kinetics during isometric-force and arm-reaching tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 2353–2378. doi: 10.1152/jn.00989.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., and Krakauer, J. W. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 359–381. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1280-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., and Mussa-Ivaldi, S. (2012). Biological Learning and Control: How the Brain Builds Representations, Predicts Events, and Makes Decisions. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262016964.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenoy, K. V., Kaufman, M. T., Sahani, M., and Churchland, M. M. (2011). A dynamical systems view of motor preparation: implications for neural prosthetic system design. Prog. Brain Res. 192, 33–58. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53355-5.00003-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenoy, K. V., Sahani, M., and Churchland, M. M. (2013). Cortical control of arm movements: a dynamical systems perspective. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 337–359. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenoy, K. V., and Carmena, J. M. (2014). Combining decoder design and neural adaptation in brain-machine interfaces. Neuron 84, 665–680. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suminski, A. J., Tkach, D. C., Fagg, A. H., and Hatsopoulos, N. G. (2010). Incorporating feedback from multiple sensory modalities enhances brain-machine interface control. J. Neurosci. 30, 16777–16787. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3967-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, D. M., Tillery, S. I., and Schwartz, A. B. (2002). Direct cortical control of 3D neuroprosthetic devices. Science 296, 1829–1832. doi: 10.1126/science.1070291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, E., and Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1226–1235. doi: 10.1038/nn963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velliste, M., Perel, S., Spalding, M. C., Whitford, A. S., and Schwartz, A. B. (2008). Cortical control of a prosthetic arm for self-feeding. Nature 453, 1098–1101. doi: 10.1038/nature06996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., and Miall, R. C. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 9, 1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(96)00035-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., and Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 338–347. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01221-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain-machine interface (BMI), optimal feedback control, dynamical systems, motor control, neuroprosthetics

Citation: Galán F and Baker SN (2015) Deafferented controllers: a fundamental failure mechanism in cortical neuroprosthetic systems. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:186. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00186

Received: 20 April 2015; Accepted: 03 July 2015;
Published: 17 July 2015.

Edited by:

George W. Huntley, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, USA

Reviewed by:

Joshua C. Brumberg, City University of New York, USA
Quentin Barraud, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Switzerland

Copyright © 2015 Galán and Baker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ferran Galán, The Medical School, Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Henry Wellcome Building, Framlington Place, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE2 4HH, UK, ferran.galan@ncl.ac.uk

Download