Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 26 April 2022
Sec. Motivation and Reward
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.763220

Reward Dependence-Moderated Noradrenergic and Hormonal Responses During Noncompetitive and Competitive Physical Activities

Zsófia Nagy1,2* István Karsai3 Tamás Nagy1 Emese Kátai1 Attila Miseta1 Gábor Fazekas4 András Láng5 Ferenc Kocsor5 János Kállai6
  • 1Department of Laboratory Medicine, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • 2Sport and Medicine Research Group, Regenerative Science, Szentágothai Research Centre, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • 3Sports and Physical Education Center, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • 4Department of Vascular Surgery, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • 5Institute of Psychology, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • 6Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary

The aim of this study was to reveal whether increased reward dependence (RD) plays a role in the catecholamine neurotransmitter release and testosterone hormone regulation during physical activities among healthy trained participants. Twenty-two male participants (mean age: 40.27 ± 5.4 years) participated in this study. Two conditions were constructed, namely, a noncompetitive and a competitive running task (RT), which were separated by a 2-week interval. Urine and blood samples were collected prior to and following the running tasks. Noradrenaline (NA), adrenaline (A), dopamine (D), and their metabolites, vanillylmandelic acid (VMA) and homovanillic acid (HVA), were measured from urine, while testosterone levels were analyzed from blood samples. RD was assessed using the Cloninger’s Personality Inventory (PI). Mental health was evaluated using the WHO Well-Being, Beck Depression, and Perceived Life Stress Questionnaires. According to our findings, levels of NA, A, D, VMA, and testosterone released underwent an increase following physical exertion, independently from the competitive condition of the RT, while HVA levels experienced a decrease. However, we found that testosterone levels showed a significantly lower tendency to elevate in the competitive RT, compared with the noncompetitive condition (p = 0.02). In contrast, HVA values were higher in the competitive compared with the noncompetitive condition (p = 0.031), both before and after the exercise. Considering the factor RD, in noncompetitive RT, its higher values were associated with elevated NA levels (p = 0.007); however, this correlation could not be detected during the competitive condition (p = 0.233). Among male runners, the NA and testosterone levels could be predicted to the degree of RD by analyzing competitive and noncompetitive physical exercises.

Introduction

Recently, several types of research focused upon the psychological, hormonal, and neurotransmitter interactions elicited by physical exercise and sporting competitions. Earlier studies have shown that physical exercise influences neurotransmitter and hormone secretions (Kjaer, 1998; Zouhal et al., 2008; Abderrahmane et al., 2013); however, personality, temperamental traits, and social behavior patterns could also have an impact on these levels (Cohen et al., 2009; Croxson et al., 2009; Salamone and Correa, 2012; Määttänen et al., 2013; Dayan et al., 2014; Varazzani et al., 2015; Casto and Edwards, 2016; Kruk et al., 2020). Cloninger’s biosocial theory regarding personality (Cloninger, 1987; Svrakic et al., 1993) introduced, among others, a fundamental reward dependence (RD) temperament, which is associated with different components of competitive exercises. However, its relation with the activity of the catecholamine and hormonal systems is debated (Cloninger et al., 1993; Cloninger et al., 2019).

The complex molecular genetics and neurobiological basis of temperament are relatively well understood and are known to control the activity of catecholamine and monoaminergic neurotransmitter systems. The RD temperament among healthy individuals is closely related to sensitivity for social reward and is associated with the activation of certain neuroendocrine and endocrine systems, such as oxytocine, norepinephrine, and testosterone (Bell et al., 2006; Garvey et al., 1996; Määttänen et al., 2013).

Today, mental and physical health-related studies are focused on the bio-psychological motivation roots of physical and mental fitness and interactions between the personal motivations and the level of the circulating reward-related catecholamines (CATs) and hormones. Both physical and computer games, sports exercises, or social competitive activities associated with temperamental traits facilitate the assessment of these interactions within in vivo situations (Hansenne et al., 2002).

The type of reward in competitive social interaction may be material (food and/or money) or social. The social reward may originate from positive verbal or meta-communication signals, intimacy or public respect, official advancement, etc., which confirm an individual’s position in a family, among peer groups, and the capable handling in receiving support during critical problem-solving situations. Individuals with an above than average RD are motivated to be a winner in various situations whose goal is to obtain social or material rewards (Cloninger, 1987; Han et al., 2006). However, the behavioral phenotype regarding RD is determined by the interaction between neurotransmitters, hormones, and current environmental requirements (Cloninger, 1987; Rózsa et al., 2005).

Individuals with elevated RD seek tenable social territory and are prepared to cooperate with the confirming group’s members, emotionally open toward others, agreeable; however, they are defensive in the face of the concurrent competitors who are rivaling for the same rewards (Cloninger et al., 1993; De Fruyt et al., 2000). Cloninger and De Furyt suggested a behavioral phenotype that is part of the secure territory defending behavior (fight and flight syndrome) and is dominated by noradrenergic transmission. The link between RD and noradrenaline has been confirmed in numerous studies which found an association between low basal noradrenergic levels with high RD temperament (Curtin et al., 1997; Ham et al., 2005). However, noradrenaline activity is sensitive to social and physical stress, and the elevation rate of the circulating noradrenaline (NA) plays a role in the psychophysiological preparation of coping with physical and mental stress.

The most investigated psycho-neuroendocrinological agents during mental or physical stress are CATs (dopamine, adrenaline, noradrenaline) and testosterone. Catecholamines influence the secretion of testosterone and other hormones (Kumar et al., 2013; Kolb and Whishaw, 2020). The concentration of circulating CATs increases during exercise in both men and women (Henderson et al., 2007; Zouhal et al., 2008; Balas et al., 2021). Various studies have pointed out the regulatory role of CATs during the effort of physical achievement (Zouhal et al., 2008). For example, the multistage 20-m shuttle run test or other standardized exercises are effective methods to measure aerobic fitness (Leger and Lambert, 1982; Metsios et al., 2008; Paradisis et al., 2014) and trigger a well-defined catecholamine secretion. The concentration of CATs is a strong indicator regarding the individual’s stress sensitivity in trained and untrained individuals and animals as well (Ratge et al., 1986; Eto et al., 2014; Baker and Buchan, 2017).

Studies on temperamental traits showed how to reward dependency and how seeking social gratification play an essential role in the success of aerobic, endurance, and combat power sports performances (Han et al., 2006). Furthermore, the goal of social approval-oriented athletes is to gain social respect from others (Maehr and Nicholls, 1980; Feltz and Ewing, 1987). Exercise can be beneficial regarding mental health; it decreases stress hormones (Budde et al., 2015), promotes a positive mood, improves confidence, encourages social interaction, and supports weight control (Reblin and Uchino, 2008; Swift et al., 2014). Additionally, social approval-oriented athletic behavior provides a buffer against stress (Childs and de Wit, 2014).

Data from the animal and human studies demonstrated enhanced RD following testosterone administration during ongoing competitive tasks (van Honk et al., 2004). Testosterone has a diurnal variation; the release is highest in the morning and decreases over the day until the afternoon. Its level is influenced by acute or prolonged high-intensity exercise conditions. Acute intensive activities elevate levels of testosterone (Sutton et al., 1973; Cumming et al., 1986), while prolonged exercises decrease (Tanaka et al., 1986) the level of testosterone. In accounting for diurnal rhythm, a testosterone index can be used for an individual temperamental trait to assess RD (Määttänen et al., 2013); social, affiliative behavior; and the involved status competition (Daitzman et al., 1978; Dabbs, 1990; Eisenegger et al., 2011).

In the course of competitive activities, testosterone elevation promotes the successful outcome of the task challenge (Carre et al., 2013) and enhances motivation to win a social and/or monetary reward (van Honk et al., 2004; Coates and Herbert, 2008; Mehta et al., 2015; Welker et al., 2015). The testosterone hormone is linked to the regulation of catecholamines and advances certain behaviors, such as reward-seeking, status-seeking, and competitive activity (Archer, 2006). Furthermore, testosterone influences the competitive sports activity among humans; however, the effect is indeed selective and dependent upon the social and physical challenges, personal motivation factors of the individuals, and, specifically, the characteristics of a given sports activity (Casto and Edwards, 2016).

Previous research has ushered in supportive evidence regarding forms of both noradrenaline and adrenaline, influencing the emotional element of social behavior (Åstrand and Rodahl, 1986). However, other CATs, including dopamine, also participate in the regulation of social behavior and RD (Kim et al., 2006; Skuse and Gallagher, 2009; Lombardo et al., 2012). Thus, the NA action for RD cannot effectively be viewed in isolation since it is linked to dopamine functions.

The primary role of this study was to reveal associations between the psychometrically defined RD and the physical and mental stress-related testosterone and CATs responses in a group of physically trained healthy athletes. Physical and mental stress was triggered by a well-defined standard task, the intermittent running exercise, performed in both noncompetitive and competitive conditions. Blood and urinary samples for CATs, CAT’s metabolites, and testosterone were obtained immediately before and after the training.

Based on the suggestions of Zouhal et al. (2008) and Kruk et al. (2020), we examined a group of middle-aged, healthy, nonprofessionally trained male athletes actively participating in both noncompetitive and competitive running conditions. We hypothesized that the RD temperament as a biological-based heritable response pattern to physical and social stress is associated with the CATs and their metabolite levels in urine.

Additionally, we believe that the induced psychoneuroendocrinological responses are transmitted by the modulation effect of RD both in noncompetitive and competitive activities, albeit, in various CATs patterns. We hypothesized that the competition triggers CATs or testosterone response regarding participants who are sensitive to the social and peer reward. This psychoneuroendocrinological effect is manifested in higher concentrations of testosterone. However, during noncompetitive activities, lower-level CATs response patterns may be expected. Considering the noradrenaline and testosterone interactions, other CATs, adrenaline, and dopamine levels will be monitored and explored shortly.

Materials and Methods

Subjects and Experimental Design

All subjects were informed regarding the procedures including the potential risks of the experiments prior to obtaining written informed consent. All procedures were approved by the Regional Committee for Research Ethics of the Locale State University (ref. No.: 7162/2018). Twenty-two male participants (mean age: 40.27 years; SD: 5.4; range: 31–49 years) were recruited for the study, who were selected by the following criteria: a history of regular training (running, cycling, and swimming) at least three times a week for a minimum of 45 min/occasion, participation in competitions at least once a year, no known acute or chronic disease present, no regular use of prescription medication, and no obesity. All participants were examined by a Doctor of Medicine, and ECG, including monitoring blood pressure, heart rate, and a general health questionnaire regarding sports medicine were all performed and duly noted. Subjects were asked to respond to questionnaires (included the Temperament and Character Inventory – R version) online before the time of the physical activities.

The experimental setup was designed to feature two phases: In the first phase, volunteers were assessed individually by a running beep test in a noncompetitive activity, while in the second phase, subjects participated in a running beep test (RT) competition in which they were split into 2–4 individuals per team. In the second phase, other participants were invited and encouraged to support their fellow athletes. The phases were separated into 2-week periods. The main reason for the order of the tests was practical feasibility. We had to assess the physical performance/capacity in the noncompetitive condition in order to form competitive groups. Once we had the data, individuals with comparable physical performance levels were enrolled in subgroups to create a competitive situation (in which every participant had a reasonable chance to win his/her group).

During the physical phases, volunteers were asked to avoid hormone-containing menu items (e.g., soya, legumes, and milk), not to consume alcohol or use recreational drugs before the day of the experiment, and to fast for a minimum of 2 h as required before the survey.

Only water consumption was allowed. All participants were asked to restrain from exercising during the day before their assessment. All RTs were executed during the evening hours. Blood and urine samples were collected immediately before and following the tests. Questionnaires were also completed within 15 min before and immediately following the RT.

Questionnaires

Before the RT, a personality test questionnaire pocket was administered. The RD is an essential factor regarding temperament traits and is measured using the Temperament and Character Inventory – R version (TCI-R, with 240 items, response range 0–4), which contains novelty seeking, harm avoidance, RD, and persistence factors (Cloninger et al., 1999; Rózsa et al., 2005).

Considering the aim of this study during our investigation, only the RD factor served as the object of the next analysis. Furthermore, the current health status of participants screened by the Perceived Stress Questionnaire, with fourteen items (response range 0–4; Cohen et al., 1983; Stauder and Konkolÿ-Thege, 2006), WHO Well-Being Questionnaire short version with five items (response range 1–5; Susánszky et al., 2006; Topp et al., 2015), and Beck’s Depression Scale short version with ten items (response range 1–4; Beck et al., 1961; Rózsa et al., 2001). All participants’ health-related questionnaire data were conferred with the national-based mean and standard deviation scores, and each subject was to be found within the healthy range of the normative sample.

Blood Samples

Before and immediately following, the RT venous blood collections were performed in suitable vacutainers; tubes containing potassium ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (K-EDTA) were used for testing cellular blood parameters. Tubes containing sodium-fluoride (NaF) were used for plasma glucose and lactate analysis, while native tubes were used to obtain serum in support of the routine laboratory blood tests.

All samples were transferred to the laboratory within 2 h, in which both plasma and serum were separated using centrifugation (15 min, room temperature, 1,500 g). Blood cell parameters were quantified in a multi-parameter automatic hematology analyzer, Sysmex XN-Series 9000 (Sysmex Corporation, Kobe, Japan). Plasma and serum parameters were measured using the Cobas 8000 Modular Analyzer (Roche Diagnostics, GmbH, Mannheim, Germany), while testosterone levels were measured using the ARCHITECT i2000SR Analyzer (Abbott Diagnostics, Abbott Park, IL, United States) in strict accordance with the manufacturer’s recommended guidelines.

Urine Samples

Middle stream urine was collected before and immediately following RT and stored in native bottles. All samples were checked by a rapid test (Cybow 10). At the laboratory, urine samples were aliquoted and frozen at −80°C until further use. Following the thaw, dopamine (D), adrenaline (A), and noradrenaline (NA) levels were detected using the Shimadzu Prominence High-Performance Liquid Chromatography (HPLC) system with Antec Decade SDCTM electrochemical detector including the Chromsystems® kit [Chromsystems® from ABL&E-JASCO Hungary, Budapest, Catecholamines in urine – HPLC kit (ref. No.: 6000)] and reverse phase column (Chromsystems® from ABL&E-JASCO Hungary, Budapest, ref. No.: 5100) in strict accordance with the manufacturer’s recommended guidelines. The flow rate was 1.3 ml/min at room temperature. Vanillylmandelic acid (VMA) and homovanillic acid (HVA) levels were detected using the same system such as the Chromsystems® kit [Chromsystems® from ABL&E-JASCO Hungary, Budapest, VMA, HVA, 5-OHIAA in urine – HPLC kit (ref. No.: 1000/B)] and reverse phase column (ref. No.: 1100/B) in strict accordance with the manufacturer’s recommended guidelines. The flow rate was 0.8 ml/min at room temperature. All data were evaluated using the LabSolution program.

Procedure

The laboratory indicators were measured using a standard clinical method regarding the participants’ blood and urinary samples, before and immediately following the noncompetitive and competitive RT. According to the health state-related anamnesis, the laboratory evidence including the physical examination substantiated and validated, all participants were indeed declared healthy. Recorded biomarkers can be found in Supplementary Figure 1, which supported the healthy stage and/or the executed RT.

In consideration of physical exertion, the multistage 20-m shuttle run test was used. The noncompetitive activity involved running a distance of 20 m, synchronized to audio sounds, which progressively increase in tempo each minute, in which time was gradually reduced. The RT lasted until total exhaustion (Leger and Lambert, 1982; Leger and Gadoury, 1989). The competitive task contains the same exercise; however, the RT was relocated to a competitive environment in which the running was performed in front of a rewarding social context. Heart rate was registered as formerly measured, during exercise, and following the activity, using the Polar Team Pro System. Volunteers were asked to don transmitters to the middle of their chest under the tip of the xiphoid process. Blood and urinary sample were immediately taken prior to and following completing noncompetitive and competitive activities.

Data Analyses

Statistical analyses were run using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 22.0). Means and standard deviations were calculated. To test the main effects of sampling time (pre- vs. post-physical exercise) and competition condition (noncompetitive vs. competitive condition), we used repeated measures ANOVAs. Both sampling time and competition are within-subject effects. No within-subject effects were tested in the repeated measures ANOVAs. Because we had only two levels in the analyses and no between-subjects effects were tested, homogeneity of variances and the assumption of sphericity are both met by default. Evidence (Stiger et al., 1998; Schmider et al., 2010) shows that ANOVAs are robust to violations of normality, and therefore, normality was not tested. The required sample size for repeated measures ANOVAs was calculated using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2009). Setting the threshold for rejecting the null hypothesis to α = 0.05, a sample size of at least 27 participants was calculated to detect medium-size effects (partial η2 = 0.06) with an adequate power of 0.80. For detecting small effects (partial η2 = 0.01), a sample size of at least 164 participants would have been required to reach a power of 0.80. Therefore, our study can be considered as underpowered in detecting small and medium effects.

Data analyses were divided into two sections. The first section controlling the validity of the measuring paradigms focused on the analyses of the effect regarding the condition and the pre- and post-measured scores on selected neurotransmitters, metabolites, and testosterone. The second part focused on the role of the RD temperament effects on detected transmitter reactivity in competitive or noncompetitive activities. The dynamical changes of the circulated neurotransmitters, their metabolites, and testosterone levels were analyzed using a stepwise regression analysis separately in noncompetitive and competitive conditions. The dependent variables were defined as the total score of the RD factor, and the pre- and post-ratios of neurotransmitters in noncompetitive and competitive conditions were included as predictors. Indeed, 22 men volunteered for the study, but only 18 individuals finished the tests completely. Four persons had to be excluded either because they missed some of the RT sessions or the blood or urine samples could not be collected.

Results

Participants’ health-related variables are in Table 1. The runners were asked to perform noncompetitive and competitive running activities in which the same neurotransmitters were analyzed in the pre-phase and post-phase regarding the RT. In all cases, in the noncompetitive condition, the within-subject analysis showed elevation between pre-phase and post-phase of the exercise-triggered neurotransmitter concentrations (the effect sizes are medium or large), except for HVA. In the case of the HVA, the concentration to the end of the RT is decreased. In the competitive condition in all cases, the level of the CATs concentration is elevated except the HVA where the concentration of the D metabolite is decreased. However, in this competitive condition, the change of the testosterone and VMA concentrations is not significant (Supplementary Figure 3).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant’s health-related variables.

Results of repeated measures ANOVAs are presented in Table 2. In addition to the significant main effect regarding the pre-RT and post-RT levels of NA, a significant interaction with large effect size (partial η2 = 0.318) was found between the pre-phase and post-phase biomarker levels and the condition of the task. This implies that the level of NA increased from pre-task to post-task; however, the increase was significantly higher during the competitive condition. Concerning A and D, the RT had a significant effect only upon their levels; both increased from pre-task to post-task.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The effect of conditioning, RT (biomarker level from pre-RT to post-RT), and their interaction (RT × condition) on neurotransmitters.

In reviewing the levels of testosterone, beyond the significant main effect of the RT, a significant interaction with large effect size (Partial η2 = 0.278) was found between pre-task and post-task value and condition. This infers that the level of testosterone increased from pre-task to post-task; however, the increase was significantly higher in the noncompetitive condition. In consideration of VMA and HVA, both the main effect of the RT and the main effect of the conditions were significant. However, no significant interaction was found. In the case of VMA, this suggests that its levels were higher in the competitive condition when compared with the noncompetitive condition, and its levels showed a significant increase from pre-task to post-task. Levels of HVA were also higher in the competitive condition, as compared with the noncompetitive condition; yet, its levels showed a significant decrease from pre-task to post-task. Results are depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Changes in CATs, their metabolites and testosterone levels measured prior to and immediately following the RT in noncompetitive and competitive conditions. SD levels are marked, p levels are numbered above the bar diagrams. NA in nmol/L, A in nmol/L, D in nmol/L, testosterone in nmol/L, VMA in μmol/L, HVA in μmol/L.

In the second step of the data analysis, the association of the RD, the examined biomarkers were accomplished. Considering the present data depicted in Table 3, the degree of the RD in men can be predicted by the catecholamine and testosterone concentration while an individual is completing various physical activities, yet differ in some respects, e.g., its circumstances.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. NA and testosterone concentrations may likely be predictors for RD.

In noncompetitive RT, the elevation of the pre-ratio and post-ratio of NA is associated with a high score in RD. In contrast, a similar association was not detected in competitive conditions. In competitive conditions, a testosterone response and RD associations were found which indicated that the testosterone pre-ratio and post-ratio are lower in an individual with a high rate of RD. In summary, among individuals with a high degree of RD and actively engaged in a competitive situation, the testosterone level in the final phase of the RT when compared with the starting phase, the level of free concentration shows a significantly attenuating tendency. Results are illustrated in Figure 2, and the nonsignificant associations are presented in Table 4.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Up: NA post/pre exercise ratio (y-axis) results in noncompetitive condition as a function of RD scores (x-axis), p = 0.007. Down: Testosterone post/pre ratio (y-axis) results in competitive condition. It is negatively associated with the RD scores (x-axis), p = 0.033.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Nonsignificant associations of measured biomarkers with RD scores.

Discussion

Our study was based on the main hypothesis in which an individual with high RD during different exercise-triggered situations responds with different CATs and hormone (testosterone) patterns. Our results indicated the elevation levels of the analyzed CATs and their metabolites (except HVA), and testosterone is relatively independent of the applied noncompetitive or competitive condition. Notably, these different response tendencies to physical stress have been confirmed by previous studies as well (Sutton et al., 1973; Wheeler et al., 1994; Määttänen et al., 2013; Casto and Edwards, 2016; McMorris, 2016; Kruk et al., 2020). However, we found that a tendency regarding testosterone is lower in the case of the competitive condition. VMA levels in the within-subject analysis showed a post-task enhancement. HVA levels following RT in both noncompetitive and competitive conditions when compared with the pre-task levels were decreased. A previous study showed that the concentration of HVA is strongly associated with dopamine levels in the brain and, in a majority of the cases, specifically in the brain stem and hypothalamus (Hasegawa et al., 2000); however, caution is recommended in this regard (Kopin et al., 1988). Therefore, this HVA level reduction deserves thoughtfulness in the interpretation of the obtained results. HVA elevated levels are frequently detected during increased stress responsiveness, as depicted in anxiety, depression, and schizophrenic cases (Breier et al., 1993; Marcelis et al., 2006; Kurita et al., 2015). In our study, HVA showed that during the noncompetitive physical activity, the stress reactivity at the end of the RT is decreased. Based on our present data, it can be stated that the RTs with the applied parameters can affect the D/NA stress reaction system among psychopathology-free healthy individuals. NA is one of the main messengers that regulates arousal, attention, emotional state, learning, memory, and stress response (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). Considering the role of NA in the neurocognitive mechanism of the salience expectancy processing, previous data (Li et al., 2017) suggest that RD can be considered as a sensitivity index to the reward-based salience when an individual approaches a goal or completes a task in which the significance of the reward may be augmented. The reward processing can be accounted as a state-dependent complex neural system that engages with the trial, trial learning in skills training and shaping of social behavior, and is controlled by the physiological state and many situational factors (Schonberg et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2009; Dayan et al., 2014). The reward is a continuum and is processed in several phases in which the upcoming event is evaluated by cost and effort balance. The undertaking for action and the execution of the goal-directed behavior involve a dual mechanism. The first is a decisive phase in which the individual evaluates the rate of the expected reward. This phase is dominated by dopaminergic reward evaluated functions. The second phase focuses on the execution of the action, controlled by the noradrenergic system in which the upcoming effort decreases the value of the expected rewards. This reward/effort trade of model (Croxson et al., 2009; Salamone and Correa, 2012; Varazzani et al., 2015) may be adapted to understand the biological and motivational bases of sports activities. The noradrenergic transmission is a dominant agent in the regulation of social behavior frequently linked to RD. However, the tendency and intensity of the association of the psychometrically defined RD and NA concentration are controversial. In an earlier study, urinary MHPG (3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol, a NA metabolite concentration) was measured, and a strong positive relationship between RD and a basic level of MHPG (Curtin et al., 1997) was found. In another study, healthy volunteers were assessed using Cloningers’ PD scales and MHPG, and over 24 h, urine was collected and assessed. This study reported a reverse association between RD and MHPG concentration (Garvey et al., 1996). In our experiments, the volunteers consisted of middle-aged, moderately physically trained men. The analysis of urine and serum samples indicated the current state-dependent amount of CATs and testosterone. Our data support the hypothesis in a relationship that exists between the RD and the NA neurotransmitter and is manifested during intensified forms of activity. However, this association diminished when the effort is larger, and the competition is facing an audience while competing with other participating members. Our data support Cloningers’ suggestion, that is, temperament is a catecholamine system-related behavioral and emotional disposition of how an individual learns, copes, and adapts to various changes in his/her environment. Additionally, we analyzed the association between RD and the pre-/post-ratios regarding the dynamic changes of the CATs and testosterone. In a stepwise regression analysis, two condition-dependent associations were revealed. In the noncompetitive condition, the rate of NA concentration change was found to be associated with RD. Additionally, the pre-test and post-test ratios of NA were indeed predictive regarding the high score of RD. In the competitive condition, we found no association between CAT’s pre- and post-ratios and RD. However, an inverse correlation was found between the dynamical changes of testosterone and RD scores. The within-subject analysis demonstrated that the testosterone concentration is strongly increased following noncompetitive conditions, and a small increase was found in competitive RT conditions. This finding is aligned with earlier results which suggest challenges that increase the level of motivation and elevate the concentration of testosterone, primarily among men (Archer, 2006; Carre et al., 2010; Wood and Stanton, 2012). However, other factors including fitness, motivation, and personality can influence the concentration of testosterone during competitive RT completion (Schultheiss, 2007; Maner et al., 2008; van der Meij et al., 2010). Despite this data, in our findings, the dynamical index (pre-ratio/post-ratio) regarding the testosterone concentration showed an attenuation tendency among those who had high RD scores. Competitive, elite sports activity is a major interest referencing stress-related neuroscientific research. Studies investigate the cost–benefit balance of competitive physical activity and explore advantageous and disadvantageous consequences. According to the current view of the psychophysiological allostasis, the competition-associated anxiety inhibits catecholamine release; the hormonal system interaction increases the perception of negative emotions and disturbs the skill automatism and the attention concentration in rivaling situations (Wheeler et al., 1994; Ong and Chua, 2020). Our present results show how testosterone concentration is increased in noncompetitive and competitive RT conditions following exercise. However, the ratio of change is dependent upon an individual temperament bias, specifically with regard to the RD. The presented data showed that high RD is associated with a low concentration of testosterone following a competitive task. The reason for this association has not yet been clarified. Suitable interpretation may be found within the neuroendocrinology of reward and behavioral dysregulation. Welker et al. (2015) suggested in a hypothesis that stress hormones and the lactate level bear a crucial role in the regulation of the interaction of reward and testosterone. According to these researchers, analysis of the cortisol and lactate levels may be an adequate means to understand the mechanism regarding the high RD and testosterone attenuation during the completion of the activity. High RD may be associated with the hemodynamical changes during physical stress which is affected by the metabolization of cortisol and lactate levels and the rate of the subject cardiovascular effort. In summary, we found evidence of how the central emotional arousal is elevated when a subject performs an activity involving physical stress. Following noncompetitive physical stress, NA levels significantly increased. However, in competitive conditions, the RD temperament is associated with the concentration of testosterone after RT. In this condition, the elevated RD is associated with a low level of serum testosterone when a runner has finished his race. Contrary, sportsmen with lower RD scores have a higher and maintained concentration testosterone after finishing their race.

Conclusion

We focused on the individual variability regarding dynamic changes between the pre-exercise and post-exercise CATs and testosterone in competitive and noncompetitive conditions. We found that RD scores were associated with NA and testosterone levels which could be indicated in that this temperamental trait affects the noradrenergic system stress response including testosterone release in the case of physical exertion in different psychical circumstances.

Study Limitations

In this study, only male participants were enrolled. The interpretation of the results concerned with the psychological functions and their associations with testosterone and the main catecholamines; however, the complex biochemical interaction between several catecholamines and other temperament factors has not been evaluated. The interaction between temperament and character factors was not the objective of this study. The role of the found association in the participants’ sociobiological adaptation has not been tested. The limited number of participants did not allow to reveal the interaction and the role of the dopaminergic transmission in RD. Due to the rigorous selection criteria, despite the relatively small number of persons available, the reported conclusions are tenable based on the found partial η2 large effect sizes. However, further examinations are recommended to confirm the results.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Regional Committee for Research Ethics of the Locale State University (ref. No.: 7162/2018). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

ZN was involved in conceptualization, methodology, investigation, resources, project administration, writing the original draft, editing and supervision. IK was involved in conceptualization, methodology, funding acquisition, writing and supervision. TN, AM, and GF were involved in writing and supervision. EK was involved in investigation and supervision. AL and FK were involved in the statistical analysis. JK was involved in the conceptualization, statistical analysis, funding acquisition, writing the original draft, editing and supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by NKFI K-120334 and GINOP-2.3.2-15-2016-00047 grants.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank all of the athletes for willfully participating in the research. Furthermore, we wish to mention the colleagues, namely, Laura Takács, Ildikó Bock-Marquette, MD & Ph.D., István Német, Anikó Lajtai, Ph.D., Heléna Halász, Jon Eugene Marquette, and assistants aligned to the Department of Laboratory Medicine of the University of Pécs for their selfless contribution.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.763220/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

RD, reward dependence; CAT, catecholamine; A, adrenaline; NA, noradrenaline; D, dopamine; VMA, vanillylmandelic acid; HVA, homovanillic acid; RT, running beep test; MHPG, 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol.

References

Abderrahmane, A. B., Prioux, J., Mrizek, I., Chamari, K., Tabka, Z., Bouslama, A., et al. (2013). Recovery (passive vs. active) during interval training and plasma catecholamine responses. Int. J. Sports Med. 34, 742–747. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1327697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 319–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Åstrand, P. O., and Rodahl, K. (1986). Textbook Of Work Physiology: Physiological Bases Of Exercise. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Baker, J. S., and Buchan, D. (2017). Metabolic stress and high-intensity exercise. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Res. 2, 1–2. doi: 10.15761/PMRR.1000136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balas, J., Gajdosik, J., Krupkova, D., Chrastinova, L., Hlavackova, A., Bacakova, R., et al. (2021). Psychophysiological responses to treadwall and indoor wall climbing in adult female climbers. Sci. Rep. 11:2639. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82184-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, C. J., Nicholson, H., Mulder, R. T., Luty, S. E., and Joyce, P. R. (2006). Plasma oxytocin levels in depression and their correlation with the temperament dimension of reward dependence. J. Psychopharmacol. 20, 656–660. doi: 10.1177/0269881106060512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breier, A., Davis, O. R., Buchanan, R. W., Moricle, L. A., and Munson, R. C. (1993). Effects of metabolic perturbation on plasma homovanillic acid in schizophrenia. relationship to prefrontal cortex volume. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 541–550. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820190043005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budde, H., Machado, S., Ribeiro, P., and Wegner, M. (2015). The cortisol response to exercise in young adults. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:13. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carre, J. M., Campbell, J. A., Lozoya, E., Goetz, S. M., and Welker, K. M. (2013). Changes in testosterone mediate the effect of winning on subsequent aggressive behaviour. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 2034–2041. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carre, J. M., Gilchrist, J. D., Morrissey, M. D., and McCormick, C. M. (2010). Motivational and situational factors and the relationship between testosterone dynamics and human aggression during competition. Biol. Psychol. 84, 346–353. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casto, K. V., and Edwards, D. A. (2016). Testosterone, cortisol, and human competition. Horm. Behav. 82, 21–37. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Childs, E., and de Wit, H. (2014). Regular exercise is associated with emotional resilience to acute stress in healthy adults. Front. Physiol. 5:161. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 573–588. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R., Cloninger, K. M., Zwir, I., and Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2019). The complex genetics and biology of human temperament: a review of traditional concepts in relation to new molecular findings. Transl. Psychiatry 9:290. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0621-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., and Svrakic, D. M. (1999). The Temperament and Character Inventory-Revised. St. Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry.

Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., and Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, J. M., and Herbert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial risk-taking on a London trading floor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 6167–6172. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704025105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X., Schoene-Bake, J. C., Elger, C. E., and Weber, B. (2009). Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 32–34. doi: 10.1038/nn.2228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croxson, P. L., Walton, M. E., O’Reilly, J. X., Behrens, T. E., and Rushworth, M. F. (2009). Effort-based cost-benefit valuation and the human brain. J. Neurosci. 29, 4531–4541. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4515-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, D. C., Brunsting, L. A. III, Strich, G., Ries, A. L., and Rebar, R. W. (1986). Reproductive hormone increases in response to acute exercise in men. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 18, 369–373.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Curtin, F., Walker, J. P., Peyrin, L., Soulier, V., Badan, M., and Schulz, P. (1997). Reward dependence is positively related to urinary monoamines in normal men. Biol. Psychiatry 42, 275–281. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00364-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dabbs, J. M. Jr. (1990). Salivary testosterone measurements: reliability across hours, days, and weeks. Physiol Behav 48, 83–86. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90265-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daitzman, R. J., Zuckerman, M., Sammelwitz, P., and Ganjam, V. (1978). Sensation seeking and gonadal hormones. J. Biosoc. Sci. 10, 401–408. doi: 10.1017/s0021932000011895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, E., Hamann, J. M., Averbeck, B. B., and Cohen, L. G. (2014). Brain structural substrates of reward dependence during behavioral performance. J. Neurosci. 34, 16433–16441. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3141-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Fruyt, F., Van De Wiele, L., and Van Heeringenb, C. (2000). Cloninger’s psychobiological model of temperament and character and the five-factor model of personality. Pers. Individ. Diff. 29, 441–452. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenegger, C., Haushofer, J., and Fehr, E. (2011). The role of testosterone in social interaction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eto, K., Mazilu-Brown, J. K., Henderson-MacLennan, N., Dipple, K. M., and McCabe, E. R. (2014). Development of catecholamine and cortisol stress responses in zebrafish. Mol. Genet. Metab. Rep. 1, 373–377. doi: 10.1016/j.ymgmr.2014.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feltz, D. L., and Ewing, M. E. (1987). Psychological characteristics of elite young athletes. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 19 (5 Suppl), S98–S105.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garvey, M. J., Noyes, R., Jr., Cook, B., and Blum, N. (1996). Preliminary confirmation of the proposed link between reward-dependence traits and norepinephrine. Psychiatry Res. 65, 61–64. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(96)02954-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ham, B. J., Choi, M. J., Lee, H. J., Kang, R. H., and Lee, M. S. (2005). Reward dependence is related to norepinephrine transporter T-182C gene polymorphism in a Korean population. Psychiatr. Genet. 15, 145–147. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200506000-00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, D. H., Kim, J. H., Lee, Y. S., Bae, S. J., Bae, S. J., Kim, H. J., et al. (2006). Influence of temperament and anxiety on athletic performance. J. Sports Sci. Med. 5, 381–389.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hansenne, M., Pinto, E., Pitchot, W., Reggers, J., Scantumburlo, G., Moor, M., et al. (2002). Further evidence on the relationship between dopamine and novelty seeking: a neuroendocrine study. Pers. Individ. Diff. 33, 967–977. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00021-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasegawa, H., Yazawa, T., Yasumatsu, M., Otokawa, M., and Aihara, Y. (2000). Alteration in dopamine metabolism in the thermoregulatory center of exercising rats. Neurosci. Lett. 289, 161–164. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01276-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, G. C., Fattor, J. A., Horning, M. A., Faghihnia, N., Johnson, M. L., Mau, T. L., et al. (2007). Lipolysis and fatty acid metabolism in men and women during the postexercise recovery period. J. Physiol. 584(Pt 3), 963–981. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.137331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. J., Kim, Y. S., Lee, H. S., Kim, S. Y., and Kim, C. H. (2006). An interaction between the serotonin transporter promoter region and dopamine transporter polymorphisms contributes to harm avoidance and reward dependence traits in normal healthy subjects. J. Neural Transm. (Vienna) 113, 877–886. doi: 10.1007/s00702-006-0444-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjaer, M. (1998). Adrenal medulla and exercise training. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 77, 195–199. doi: 10.1007/s004210050321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolb, B., and Whishaw, I. Q. (2020). An Introduction To Brain and Behavior, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Worth Publishers.

Google Scholar

Kopin, I. J., White, J. H., and Bankiewicz, K. (1988). A new approach to biochemical evaluation of brain dopamine metabolism. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 8, 171–179. doi: 10.1007/BF00711243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruk, J., Kotarska, K., and Aboul-Enein, B. H. (2020). Physical exercise and catecholamines response: benefits and health risk: possible mechanisms. Free Radic. Res. 54, 105–125. doi: 10.1080/10715762.2020.1726343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Rinwa, P., Kaur, G., and Machawal, L. (2013). Stress: neurobiology, consequences and management. J. Pharm. Bioallied Sci. 5, 91–97. doi: 10.4103/0975-7406.111818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurita, M., Nishino, S., Numata, Y., Okubo, Y., and Sato, T. (2015). The noradrenaline metabolite MHPG is a candidate biomarker between the depressive, remission, and manic states in bipolar disorder I: two long-term naturalistic case reports. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 11, 353–358. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S74550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leger, L. A., and Lambert, J. (1982). A maximal multistage 20-m shuttle run test to predict VO2 max. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 49, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/BF00428958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leger, L., and Gadoury, C. (1989). Validity of the 20 m shuttle run test with 1 min stages to predict VO2max in adults. Can. J. Sport Sci. 14, 21–26.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Li, S., Demenescu, L. R., Sweeney-Reed, C. M., Krause, A. L., Metzger, C. D., and Walter, M. (2017). Novelty seeking and reward dependence-related large-scale brain networks functional connectivity variation during salience expectancy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4064–4077. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lombardo, M. V., Ashwin, E., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., Lai, M. C., Taylor, K., et al. (2012). Fetal programming effects of testosterone on the reward system and behavioral approach tendencies in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 839–847. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Määttänen, I., Jokela, M., Hintsa, T., Firtser, S., Känene, M., Jula, A., et al. (2013). Testosterone and temperament traits in men: longitudinal analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 2243–2248. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maehr, M. L., and Nicholls, J. G. (1980). “Culture and achievement motivation: a second look,” in Studies in Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol. 2, ed. N. Warren (London: Academic Press), 221–267.

Google Scholar

Maner, J. K., Miller, S. L., Schmidt, N. B., and Eckel, L. A. (2008). Submitting to defeat: social anxiety, dominance threat, and decrements in testosterone. Psychol. Sci. 19, 764–768. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcelis, M., Suckling, J., Hofman, P., Woodruff, P., Bullmore, E., and van Os, J. (2006). Evidence that brain tissue volumes are associated with HVA reactivity to metabolic stress in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 86, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMorris, T. (2016). Developing the catecholamines hypothesis for the acute exercise-cognition interaction in humans: lessons from animal studies. Physiol. Behav. 165, 291–299. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehta, P. H., Snyder, N. A., Knight, E. L., and Lassetter, B. (2015). Close versus decisive victory moderates the effect of testosterone change on competitive decisions and task enjoyment. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 1, 291–311. doi: 10.1007/s40750-014-0014-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metsios, G. S., Flouris, A. D., Koutedakis, Y., and Nevill, A. (2008). Criterion-related validity and test-retest reliability of the 20m square shuttle test. J. Sci. Med. Sport 11, 214–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2006.12.120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ong, N. C. H., and Chua, J. (2020). Effects of psychological interventions on competitive anxiety in sport: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 52:101836. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradisis, G. P., Zacharogiannis, E., Mandila, D., Smirtiotou, A., Argeitaki, P., and Cooke, C. B. (2014). Multi-stage 20-m shuttle run fitness test, maximal oxygen uptake and velocity at maximal oxygen uptake. J. Hum. Kinet. 41, 81–87. doi: 10.2478/hukin-2014-0035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratge, D., Gehrke, A., Melzner, I., and Wisser, H. (1986). Free and conjugated catecholamines in human plasma during physical exercise. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 13, 543–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.1986.tb00937.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reblin, M., and Uchino, B. N. (2008). Social and emotional support and its implication for health. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 21, 201–205. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3282f3ad89

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rózsa, S., Kállai, J., Osváth, A., and Bánki, M. C. S. (2005). Temperament and Character: Cloninger’s Psychobiological Model. Budapest: Medicina KRT.

Google Scholar

Rózsa, S., Szádóczky, E., and Füredi, J. (2001). Beck depression questionnaire: a national adaptation. Psychiatr. Hung. 16, 384–402.

Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., and Correa, M. (2012). The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron 76, 470–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmider, E., Ziegler, M., Danay, E., Beyer, L., and Bühner, M. (2010). Is it really robust? Reinvestigating the robustness of ANOVA against violations of the normal distribution assumption. Methodology 6, 147–151. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241/a000016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonberg, T., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. J. Neurosci. 27, 12860–12867. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). “A biobehavioral model of implicit power motivation arousal, reward, and frustration,” in Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological And Psychological Explanations Of Social Behavior, eds P. Winkielman and E. Harmon-Jones (New York, NY: Guilford), 176–196.

Google Scholar

Skuse, D. H., and Gallagher, L. (2009). Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stauder, A., and Konkolÿ-Thege, B. (2006). Characteristics of the hungarian version of the perceived stress scale (PSS). Mentálhigiéné Pszichoszomatika 7, 203–216.

Google Scholar

Stiger, T. R., Kosinski, A. S., Barnhart, H. X., and Kleinbaum, D. G. (1998). Anova for repeated ordinal data with small sample size? a comparison of anova, manova, wls and gee methods by simulation. Commun. Statist. Simul. Comput. 27, 357–375. doi: 10.1080/03610919808813485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Susánszky, É, Konkolÿ-Thege, B., Stauder, A., and Kopp, M. (2006). Validation of the short (5-item) version of the WHO well-being scale based on a Hungarian representative health survey, (Hungarostudy 2002). Mentálhigiéné Pszichoszomatika 7, 247–255.

Google Scholar

Sutton, J. R., Coleman, M. J., Casey, J., and Lazarus, L. (1973). Androgen responses during physical exercise. Br. Med. J. 1, 520–522. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.5852.520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svrakic, D. M., Whitehead, C., Przybeck, T. R., and Cloninger, C. R. (1993). Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 991–999. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240075009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swift, D. L., Johannsen, N. M., Lavie, C. J., Earnest, C. P., and Church, T. S. (2014). The role of exercise and physical activity in weight loss and maintenance. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 56, 441–447. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2013.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, H., Cleroux, J., de Champlain, J., Ducharme, J. R., and Collu, R. (1986). Persistent effects of a marathon run on the pituitary-testicular axis. J. Endocrinol. Invest. 9, 97–101. doi: 10.1007/BF03348075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Topp, C. W., Ostergaard, S. D., Sondergaard, S., and Bech, P. (2015). The WHO-5 Well-being index: a systematic review of the literature. Psychother. Psychosom. 84, 167–176. doi: 10.1159/000376585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Meij, L., Buunk, A. P., Almela, M., and Salvador, A. (2010). Testosterone responses to competition: the opponent’s psychological state makes it challenging. Biol. Psychol. 84, 330–335. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Hermans, E. J., Putman, P., Tuiten, A., and Koppeschaar, H. (2004). Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and reward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 937–943. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varazzani, C., San-Galli, A., Gilardeau, S., and Bouret, S. (2015). Noradrenaline and dopamine neurons in the reward/effort trade-off: a direct electrophysiological comparison in behaving monkeys. J. Neurosci. 35, 7866–7877. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0454-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welker, K. M., Gruber, J., and Mehta, P. H. (2015). A positive affective neuroendocrinology approach to reward and behavioral dysregulation. Front. Psychiatry 6:93. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler, G., Cumming, D., Burnham, R., Maclean, I., Sloley, B. D., Bhambhani, Y., et al. (1994). Testosterone, cortisol and catecholamine responses to exercise stress and autonomic dysreflexia in elite quadriplegic athletes. Paraplegia 32, 292–299. doi: 10.1038/sc.1994.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, R. I., and Stanton, S. J. (2012). Testosterone and sport: current perspectives. Horm. Behav. 61, 147–155. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zouhal, H., Jacob, C., Delamarche, P., and Gratas-Delamarche, A. (2008). Catecholamines and the effects of exercise, training and gender. Sports Med. 38, 401–423. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200838050-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reward dependence, temperament and character inventory - revised (TCI-R), neuroendocrinological response, beep running test, competitive condition

Citation: Nagy Z, Karsai I, Nagy T, Kátai E, Miseta A, Fazekas G, Láng A, Kocsor F and Kállai J (2022) Reward Dependence-Moderated Noradrenergic and Hormonal Responses During Noncompetitive and Competitive Physical Activities. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:763220. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.763220

Received: 23 August 2021; Accepted: 15 March 2022;
Published: 26 April 2022.

Edited by:

Raúl G. Paredes, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Jan Rodriguez, Maj Institute of Pharmacology (PAS), Poland
Wendy Portillo, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2022 Nagy, Karsai, Nagy, Kátai, Miseta, Fazekas, Láng, Kocsor and Kállai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zsófia Nagy, nagyzsofia.pteaok@gmail.com

Download