Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 15 April 2022
Sec. Learning and Memory
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.846377

Brain Structural Correlates of Dispositional Insight and the Mediation Role of Neuroticism in Young Adults

Jiabao Lin1,2,3† Yajue Chen4† Jiushu Xie5 Qiuping Cheng2,3 Mi Zou2,3 Lei Mo2,3*
  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Public Health and Management, Guangzhou University of Chinese Medicine, Guangzhou, China
  • 2Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition and Education Sciences, South China Normal University, Ministry of Education, Guangzhou, China
  • 4School of Civil and Transportation Engineering, Guangdong University of Technology, Guangzhou, China
  • 5School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China

Studies on the neural correlates of episodic insight have made significant progress in the past decades. However, the neural mechanisms underlying dispositional insight are largely unknown. In the present study, we recruited forty-four young, healthy adults and performed several analyses to reveal the neural mechanisms of dispositional insight. Firstly, a voxel-based morphometry (VBM) technique was used to explore the structural brain mechanisms of dispositional insight. We found that dispositional insight was significantly and negatively correlated with the regional gray matter volume (rGMV) in the left thalamus (TLM.L), right temporoparietal junction (TPJ.R), and left dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC.L). Secondly, we performed a seed-based resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) analysis to complement the findings of VBM analysis further. The brain regions of TLM.L, DMPFC.L, and TPJ.R were selected as seed regions. We found that dispositional insight was associated with altered RSFC between the DMPFC.L and bilateral TPJ, between the TPJ.R and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, DMPFC.L, TPJ.L, right insula, and right cerebellum. Finally, a mediation analysis found that the personality of neuroticism partially mediated the relationship between the brain region of TLM.L and dispositional insight. These findings imply that dispositional insight has a specific functional and structural neural mechanism. The personality of neuroticism may play a pivotal role in the processes of dispositional insight.

Introduction

Creativity is the primary human function to produce work and thoughts that are both original and valuable (Sternberg and Lubart, 1996; Beaty et al., 2018; Ren et al., 2020; Tang et al., 2022). The importance of creativity has long been demonstrated (Ren et al., 2020). Insight, a form of creativity, is a sudden comprehension that reinterprets a situation and solves a problem (Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2003; Webb et al., 2016; Tik et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2020). Studies of insight properties usually focus on two dimensions: cognitive process and individual characteristics (Ash and Wiley, 2006; Ovington et al., 2016; Ogawa et al., 2018). Previous studies, treating insight as a problem’s solution process, used specialized tasks to induce insight and reveal the cognitive mechanisms under insightful solutions (Metcalfe and Wiebe, 1987; Knoblich et al., 1999; Kounios and Beeman, 2009). Overall, these studies measured episodic (state) insight. Alternatively, other studies treated insight as an individual characteristic and found reliable individual differences in solving insight problems (Jacobs and Dominowski, 1981; Ash and Wiley, 2006; Gilhooly and Fioratou, 2009; DeCaro and Wieth, 2016). For instance, a study found stable individual differences in performance across different object-use insight problems (Jacobs and Dominowski, 1981). Briefly, these studies measured dispositional (trait) insight. Studying dispositional insight can explain why some people have more insightful problem-solving success than others. Recently, many studies have utilized various paradigms, such as riddles solving, to investigate the neural mechanisms of episodic insight (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Zhao et al., 2014; Tik et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2020). However, the neural mechanisms of dispositional insight are rarely understood. In the current study, we utilized voxel-based morphometry (VBM) method to explore the structural neural mechanisms of dispositional insight.

Psychologically, episodic insight contains mental processes of set-shifting, reorienting attention, method-searching, emotional experience, and establishing new representations (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Qiu et al., 2010; Tik et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2020). For instance, when people came to an impasse in a complex problem, they needed to reconsider basic assumptions of the problem, reorient attention, and find a new method (Qiu et al., 2010; Tang et al., 2015). Moreover, insightful problem-solving can induce positive feelings (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004). Previous studies have shown that episodic and dispositional insight may share some common characteristics (Byrne and Murray, 2005; Ovington et al., 2016). For instance, similar to the findings of episodic insight, individuals with high dispositional insight had great attention switching capacity (Byrne and Murray, 2005) and positive affect (Ovington et al., 2016). In addition, compared to episodic insight, dispositional insight, a trait of personality, was found to be associated with high cognitive flexibility and tendency to engage in effortful mental activities, which may recruit more set-shifting and motivational components (Gilhooly and Murphy, 2005; Ovington et al., 2016).

Neurally, several underlying neural mechanisms in episodic insight have been described. Neuroimaging studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) indicated increased medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), temporal, and parietal activity in episodic insight (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Kounios et al., 2006; Huang et al., 2018). For example, researchers employed a compound remote associates paradigm and found neural activity in the MPFC, superior temporal gyrus (STG), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) accompanied insightful problem solving (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Kounios et al., 2006; Tik et al., 2018). The occurrence of an insight experience was related to brain activation in the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) by using riddles as experimental materials (Huang et al., 2018). Together, these findings suggest that MPFC and TPJ may be vital for episodic insight. Considering the commonalities between episodic and dispositional insight and their uniqueness, we hypothesized that dispositional insight can involve particular neural mechanisms in addition to the common neural mechanisms engaged in episodic insight.

Recently, many studies used resting-state fMRI to explore the functional connectivity in creativity (Takeuchi et al., 2012; Beaty et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2014; Sun et al., 2018). For example, researchers detected that creative performance was associated with the resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) between the inferior frontal gyrus and the default mode network (Beaty et al., 2014). Takeuchi et al. (2012) reported that creativity was related to RSFC between the MPFC and posterior cingulate cortex using the task of divergent thinking. Furthermore, a previous study has combined the VBM with RSFC to evaluate the brain structural-functional relationships in creativity. Researchers employed the creative achievement questionnaire (CAQ) to calculate individual creativity in this study. Results showed that creativity was negatively correlated with the dorsal ACC’s regional gray matter volume (rGMV). Creativity was also negatively associated with the RSFC between the dorsal ACC and medial superior frontal gyrus (MSFG) (Chen et al., 2014). However, few studies have used structural and functional analyses to explore the changed brain structure and RSFC in dispositional insight.

Over the past decades, many studies have explored the relationship between personality characteristics and creativity (King et al., 1996; Batey, 2007; Furnham and Bachtiar, 2008; Li et al., 2014; Sadana et al., 2021). For example, researchers used the NEO Personality Inventory and divergent thinking tests to study the linkage between personality and creativity. They found that creativity was significantly correlated with extraversion and openness to experience (King et al., 1996). Furthermore, several studies also identified that creativity potential was associated with high neuroticism (Batey, 2007; Furnham and Bachtiar, 2008). The findings mentioned above suggest that essential aspects of personality may play a pivotal role in creativity. In addition, researchers found that character was closely associated with brain structure (DeYoung et al., 2010; Kong et al., 2015b). For example, neuroticism was related to structural variations in the STG and thalamus (Kong et al., 2015b). Furthermore, a previous study has indicated that personality can mediate the relationship between brain structure and trait creativity (Li et al., 2014). Thus, we deduce that individual personality differences might mediate the relationship between dispositional insight and rGMV in particular brain regions.

Previous studies have validated that using individuals’ performance on insight problems can estimate dispositional insight (Jacobs and Dominowski, 1981; Ash and Wiley, 2006; DeCaro and Wieth, 2016; Ogawa et al., 2018). For instance, Jacobs and Dominowski (1981) used average performance (i.e., mean solution times) on several classic insight problems, such as candle problems and two-string problems, to calculate the dispositional insight of each subject. Results showed significant differences in insightful problem-solving performance between males and females. Recently, researchers utilized problem-solving success (i.e., mean correct solution rates and solution times) on insightful chunk decomposition problems (i.e., matchstick arithmetic problems) to calculate the dispositional insight of each subject. They found that working memory can predict an individual’s ability to successfully solve insightful problems (Ash and Wiley, 2006; DeCaro and Wieth, 2016). Notably, insightful chunk decomposition problems were typically regarded as insight problems (Knoblich et al., 1999; DeCaro and Wieth, 2016). For instance, in the matchstick arithmetic task (i.e., chunk decomposition task), subjects were required to transform wrong arithmetic equations, such as “XI = III+III,” into true equations, such as “VI = III + III.” To solve the problems, subjects should decompose the “X” into “V.” This process involved cognitive restructuring, the defining characteristic of insight problems (Knoblich et al., 1999). However, the number of matchstick arithmetic problems is minimal. Researchers have recently developed a new insightful chunk decomposition task (i.e., insightful Chinese character chunk decomposition task), which can provide a sufficient number of insight problems (Luo et al., 2006; Wu et al., 2009; Huang et al., 2015). Descriptions of the task can be found elsewhere (Luo et al., 2006; Wu et al., 2009; Huang et al., 2015). Thus, in the present study, we used individuals’ performance (i.e., mean correct solution rates and solution times) on insightful Chinese character chunk decomposition tasks to measure dispositional insight.

Together, we aimed to study the relevance between altered brain structure associated with RSFC and dispositional insight. Notably, the RSFC analysis was to complement the findings of VBM analysis in this study. We used multi-modal brain structural MRI and resting-state fMRI analyses to detect the structural and functional neural mechanisms underlying dispositional insight. Based on the studies mentioned above, we hypothesized that rGMV and RSFC in the MPFC and TPJ might correlate with dispositional insight. Furthermore, we predicted that subjects’ personalities might mediate the linkage between dispositional insight and rGMV in particular brain regions.

Materials and Methods

The current study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The Research Ethics Review Board of South China Normal University approved the present study. Subjects provided written informed consent before their participation in the current study.

Subjects

The sample included forty-four subjects (18 males, mean age 20.73, range 18–27). We determined the sample size according to a previous neuroimaging study (Xiang et al., 2016). All the subjects were students from South China Normal University. They were screened by questionnaire to exclude subjects who have the history of neurological or psychiatric illness. All of them reported right-handed and had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Insight Task

The present study used individuals’ performance on the insightful Chinese character chunk decomposition tasks to measure individuals’ dispositional insight. Figure 1 shows the insight task and materials in the study. Notably, the difficulty of the experimental materials might potentially affect the performance of subjects. For instance, if all the testing materials were too easy or difficult for each subject, the result would reveal ceiling or floor effects. Thus, we created two kinds of insightful Chinese character chunk decomposition which varies in difficulty, including low and high insightful Chinese character chunk decomposition conditions. Specifically, the low insight condition required subjects to transform the given character (e.g., “保”) into the target character (e.g., “口”) by discarding the to-be-removed character (e.g., “休”). Notably, each given character (e.g., “保”) in this condition is a left-right structure, which comprises a radical (e.g., “亻”) and a sub-character (e.g., “呆”). Meanwhile, each sub-character is a top-bottom structure that comprises a radical (e.g., “口”) and a character (e.g., “木”). In the macroscopic view, it is easy for subjects to decompose the radical (e.g., “亻”) from the given character (e.g., “保”) by intuition. In other words, it is easy to separate the radical (e.g., “亻”) and the sub-character (e.g., “呆”), which fulfills the expectations of subjects. Previous studies have verified that this radical-level character decomposition is easy and can be done through ordinary thinking (Luo et al., 2006; Huang et al., 2015). However, in this insight condition, subjects were required to decompose the to-be-removed character (e.g., “休”), not the radical (e.g., “亻”), from the given character (e.g., “保”) to constitute the target character (e.g., “口”). This process violates subjects’ expectations and is creative for subjects to perform. In the high insight condition, subjects were also asked to change the given character (e.g., “固”) into the target character (e.g., “十”) by discarding the to-be-removed character (e.g., “回”). However, in this insight condition, each target character (e.g., “十”) is visually embedded in the to-be-removed character (e.g., “回”). Subjects are unfamiliar with these materials. Thus, they may adopt more creative ways to perform this decomposition.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic of experimental design. (A) Experimental procedures. (B) Exemplars of each condition in insight and filler tasks.

Filler tasks (i.e., ordinary Chinese character chunk decompositions), also comprising easy and difficult conditions, were included in the present study (Figure 1). In the easy ordinary condition, subjects were asked to perform radical-level Chinese character chunk decompositions, such as decomposing given characters (e.g., “促”) into target characters (e.g., “足”) by discarding the to-be-removed parts (e.g., “亻”). In the difficult ordinary condition, subjects were asked to perform stroke-level Chinese character chunk decompositions, such as decomposing given characters (e.g., “四”) into target characters (e.g., “匹”) by discarding the to-be-removed parts (e.g., “丨”). Previous studies have proved that stroke-level Chinese character chunk decompositions were more complicated than the radical-level ones (Luo et al., 2006; Wu et al., 2013). Notably, we employed the filler tasks to prevent subjects from deliberately using strategies in performing the insight tasks. Furthermore, the filler tasks were randomly intermixed with the insight tasks to vary the charges and keep subjects’ attention on the tasks. In sum, there were four conditions in this study.

The current experiment contained 160 trials, equally divided into two blocks, comprising 20 trials per condition. The sequence of trials was randomized. For each trial, subjects viewed a given character, a target character, and a to-be-removed portion. Firstly, the given character was presented centrally on the screen for 2 s. Then, a fixation cross was presented for 1 s. After that, the target character was presented centrally on the screen for 6 s until participants responded. During this time, subjects were informed about changing the given character into the target character and finding a solution (i.e., finding the to-be-removed portion) as quickly as they could within 6 s by pressing a key. Here, we recorded the subjects’ response times (RTs). Finally, following a fixation cross for 1s, a new portion was shown on the screen. Subjects determined whether this new portion was the same as one had previously found, then pressed the “F” or “J” key as quickly as they could in 3 s. Here, we recorded the subjects’ correct response rates (CRs).

In order to validate whether the insight task can induce an insightful experience or not, additional 20 subjects rated the creativity and “Aha!” experience of the insight and filler tasks on a 5-point scale. Results showed that the insight task (M = 3.13, SD = 0.37) was significantly higher in creativity than the filler task (M = 1.53, SD = 0.42, p < 0.001). Within the insight task, high insight trials (M = 3.44, SD = 0.37) were more creative than the low insight trials (M = 2.82, SD = 0.45, p < 0.001). In addition, the insight task (M = 3.02, SD = 0.38) induced more “Aha!” experience than the filler task (M = 1.44, SD = 0.29, p < 0.001). Within the insight task, high insight trials (M = 3.33, SD = 0.41) induced more “Aha!” experience than the low insight trials (M = 2.70, SD = 0.43, p < 0.001). Therefore, the insightful Chinese character chunk decomposition was an insight task.

After this behavioral experiment, all subjects received an MRI scan, including anatomical imaging (5 min) and resting-state imaging (8 min). During the resting-state imaging, all subjects were required to relax and stay awake, with their eyes closed.

Measures

Dispositional Insight

For each subject, we took both high and low insight tasks together to estimate the extent of dispositional insight. The mean RTs of both low and high insight tasks were calculated, then the mean CRs of low and high insight tasks were acquired. In the end, an inverse efficiency score (IES: the ratio of mean RTs over mean CRs) was computed and finally log-transformed to an approximately normal distribution (Townsend and Ashby, 1978; Bruyer and Brysbaert, 2011; Reyes and Sackur, 2014). Notably, the lower the transformed IES is, the higher the dispositional insight becomes.

Personality

In the current study, the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R), IPIP-NEO-120, was employed to estimate the personality of each subject (Costa, and McCrae, 1992). Previous studies verified that this NEO-PI-R had high reliability and validity in Chinese populations (Yang et al., 1999; Kong et al., 2015a; Xiang et al., 2016; Kajonius and Mac Giolla, 2017). In brief, the NEO-PI-R measures five different dimensions of personality: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. The NEO-PI-R comprises 120 items, with 24 items per dimension. Subjects should respond to each item on a 5-point Likert scale, which ranges from strong disagreement to strong agreement. Notably, negatively worded items were reversely coded before all analyses.

Image Acquisition

Scanning was conducted on a 3 T Siemens Trio Tim MR scanner with a 12-channel phased-array head coil in SCNU, Guangzhou, China. Briefly, the T1-weighted images were acquired with a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR/TE/FA/thickness: 1900 ms/2.52 ms/9°/1.0 mm; matrix size: 256 × 256; FOV: 256 × 256 mm2; 176 sagittal slices). The resting-state fMRI data were obtained using an Echo Planar Imaging (EPI) sequence (TR/TE/FA/thickness: 2000 ms/30 ms/90°/3.5 mm; matrix size: 64 × 64; FOV: 204 × 204 mm2; 33 axial slices).

Voxel-Based Morphometry

The computational anatomy toolbox 12 (CAT 12 toolbox1) was adopted to perform the VBM analysis of structural images. In the present study, we utilized the suggested default settings of the CAT 12 toolbox to preprocess the structural images. Detailed descriptions can be found in the manual of the CAT 12 toolbox.2 Brie?y, the preprocessing procedure comprised the following steps: (1) correcting for magnetic field inhomogeneities, (2) segmenting the brain into gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), (3) normalizing the images using the DARTEL algorithm, (4) modulating the images using the Jacobian determinants, (5) smoothing the images with an 8 mm Gaussian kernel (Besteher et al., 2017; Farokhian et al., 2017; Takeuchi et al., 2018). In addition, the total intracranial volume (TIV) was computed in the present study.

Resting-State Functional Connectivity

The functional brain data were entered into the DPABI software package3 and received preprocessing (Yan et al., 2016). Brie?y, the suggested standard procedure incorporated the following steps: (1) removing the first ten images, (2) slice-time correction, (3) head-motion correction, (4) normalizing the images into the MNI template by using the T1 image for coregistration, (5) smoothing the images by using a 4 mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter, (6) signal linear detrending, (7) removing nuisance signal (WM and CSF signal, and Friston 24-parameters of head motion) (Yan et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2019), (8) temporal filtering (0.01-0.08 HZ). Notably, this study estimated the 6-parameters of head motion, and all the subjects met the criteria (translation < 2.5 mm and rotation < 2.5°). In addition, the Friston 24-parameters of head motion were also computed, and the mean framewise displacement (FD) of head motion was acquired and subsequently used as a regressor of no interest to be regressed out in the statistical analysis (Yan et al., 2013).

In the RSFC analysis, the brain regions showing significant correlations between the rGMV and dispositional insight were selected as the seed regions to compute the voxel-wise functional connectivity. Firstly, for each seed region, the mean time course of the seed region was calculated. Secondly, we calculated Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the mean time course of the seed region and the time course of all other brain voxels. Thirdly, for normality purposes, the correlation coefficients were transformed to z-scores (i.e., Fisher’s r-to-z transformation). Finally, the seed-based z-RSFC map was acquired for each subject and subsequently entered into statistical analyses.

Statistical Analyses

Voxel-Based Morphometry

In the group-level analysis, a multiple regression model was utilized to explore the structural mechanisms of dispositional insight. In other words, based on the preprocessed structural images, we planned to identify the brain regions in which the rGMV was correlated with dispositional insight (measured by IES). In this model, the IES was used as the regressor of interest. Meanwhile, since several studies indicated that factors of sex, age, and TIV could affect the brain structure, the influence factors of sex, age, and TIV were regressed out (Malone et al., 2015; Lin et al., 2017). For multiple comparisons correction, the statistical maps were thresholded at voxel-wise uncorrected p < 0.005 and cluster-extent FWE corrected p < 0.05 (Hayasaka et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2018).

Resting-State Functional Connectivity

In the group-level analysis, a multiple regression model based on the z-RSFC maps was built. We planned to find out the brain regions showing correlations between the RSFC and dispositional insight (measured by IES). Specifically, in this model, the IES was entered as the regressor of interest; gender, age, and mean FD of head motion were entered as the regressors of no interest (Wu et al., 2019). The multiple comparisons correction here was the same as the one used in the VBM analysis.

Mediation Analysis

To examine whether the personality can mediate the linkage between the dispositional insight and brain structure or not, a mediation model was built by using the PROCESS macro (Preacher and Hayes, 2008; Xiang et al., 2016). In this model, the brain structure (i.e., rGMV value) was used as an independent variable (X), and the dispositional insight (i.e., IES value) was used as a dependent variable (Y). Then, the personality was set as a mediating variable (M). We aimed to test the effects of X on Y through M. Following previous studies (Li et al., 2014; Xiang et al., 2016), we didn’t control other personality dimensions when entering one personality dimension as M. Notably, we utilized a bootstrap procedure (n = 10000) to validate the impact of X on Y through M. A 95% confidence interval which does not incorporate 0 implies that the indirect effect is significant at the 0.05 level.

Results

Behavioral Results

Firstly, we found that the high insight condition was longer in RTs [t (43) = 5.93, p < 0.001] and lower in CRs [t (43) = -5.56, p < 0.001] than the low insight condition, reflecting the validity of our manipulations on the degree of insight. Notably, we used RTs and CRs to calculate the IES (an index of dispositional insight). Secondly, the mean (SD), range, skewness, and kurtosis of the IES and the score of each personality were computed and listed in Table 1. The kurtosis and skewness of each score varied from -1 to +1, suggesting normality of the score (Marcoulides and Hershberger, 1997; Xiang et al., 2016).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of dispositional insight (estimated by IES) and each personality.

Voxel-Based Morphometry

Figure 2 shows the findings of the whole brain VBM analysis. We found that the IES was significantly and positively correlated with rGMV in the left thalamus (TLM.L), TPJ.R, and left dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC.L). Details of these clusters are listed in Table 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Whole-brain VBM analyses showing three clusters exhibiting significant and positive correlations with the IES (an index of dispositional insight). These clusters include the TLM.L, TPJ.R, and DMPFC.L. Coordinates are shown in the MNI space. TLM.L, left thalamus; TPJ.R, right temporoparietal junction; DMPFC.L, left dorsal medial prefrontal cortex; VBM, voxel-based morphometry; IES, inverse efficiency score; MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Clusters exhibiting significant and positive correlations with the IES in the VBM analyses.

Resting-State Functional Connectivity

Figure 3 shows the results of the seed-based RSFC analysis. The brain regions of TLM.L, DMPFC.L, and TPJ.R derived from VBM analysis were selected as seed regions. Results showed that the RSFC between the seed region of DMPFC.L and the regions of TPJ.L and TPJ.R was significantly and positively correlated with the IES. Moreover, the RSFC between the seed region of TPJ.R and the regions of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC.L), left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC.L), DMPFC.L, TPJ.L, right insula (INS.R), and right cerebellum (CRBL.R) was also significantly and positively correlated with the IES. Details of these results can be found in Table 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results derived from the seed-based RSFC analysis. Significant correlations between the RSFC and the IES (an index of dispositional insight). Red color denotes the RSFC seeded from TPJ.R. The RSFC of TPJ.R-TPJ.L, TPJ.R-DLPFC.L, TPJ.R-VLPFC.L, TPJ.R-DMPFC.L, TPJ.R-INS.R, and TPJ.R-CRBL.R were significantly and positively correlated with IES. Blue color denotes the RSFC seeded from DMPFC.L. The RSFC of DMPFC.L-TPJ.R, and DMPFC.L-TPJ.L were significantly and positively correlated with IES. RSFC, resting-state functional connectivity; IES, inverse efficiency score; TPJ.R, right temporoparietal junction; DMPFC.L, left dorsal medial prefrontal cortex; DLPFC.L, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; VLPFC.L, left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; INS.R, right insula; CRBL.R, right cerebellum.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Significant and positive correlations between the RSFC and IES (an index of dispositional insight).

Mediation Analysis

Figure 4 shows that the personality of neuroticism mediates the relationship between the cluster of TLM.L and dispositional insight (estimated by IES). Specifically, we first investigated the relationship between each personality (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, or conscientiousness) and dispositional insight. We found that only neuroticism exhibited a significant correlation with IES (r = -0.47, p = 0.001, Table 1). Subsequently, we tested whether brain structures associated with dispositional insight (TLM.L, DMPFC.L, and TPJ.R) could be correlated with neuroticism. We detected that only the rGMV in the TLM.L showed a significant correlation with neuroticism (r = -0.38, p = 0.012, see Figure 4). The findings implied that neuroticism was associated with both dispositional insight and rGMV in the TLM.L. Finally, to clarify this relationship further, we conducted a mediation model to test whether neuroticism can mediate the linkage between the rGMV in the TLM.L and dispositional insight or not. Specifically, we examined the linkage between the rGMV in the TLM.L and the IES without regressing out neuroticism. We found that rGMV in the TLM.L exhibited a significant correlation with IES (β = 0.49, p = 0.001). When regressing out the neuroticism, the correlation between the TLM.L and IES was still significant but decreased a little (β = 0.37, p = 0.011). In addition, the bootstrap procedure (n = 10000) validated that neuroticism mediated the linkage between the TLM.L volume and IES (95% confidence interval = [0.049, 1.406], p < 0.05).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Results of the mediation analysis. Neuroticism mediates the association between the brain structure and IES. Paths a, b, c, and c’ denote standard regression coefficients. Path a: rGMV in the TLM.L is significantly correlated with neuroticism. Path b: neuroticism is significantly correlated with IES. Path c: rGMV in the TLM.L is significantly correlated with IES. Path c’: after regressing out neuroticism, rGMV in the TLM.L is significantly correlated with IES. IES, inverse efficiency score; rGMV, regional gray matter volume; TLM.L, left thalamus.

Discussion

The present study aimed to study the changed brain structure and RSFC associations with dispositional insight. The VBM analysis found that IES (an index of dispositional insight) was positively correlated with the rGMV in the TLM.L, TPJ.R, and DMPFC.L. In the RSFC analysis, we found that IES was positively correlated with the RSFC of TPJ.R-TPJ.L, TPJ.R-DLPFC.L, TPJ.R-VLPFC.L, TPJ.R-DMPFC.L, TPJ.R-INS.R, TPJ.R-CRBL.R, DMPFC.L-TPJ.L, and DMPFC.L-TPJ.R. The RSFC analysis was to complement the findings of VBM analysis. Notably, the lower the IES is, the higher the dispositional insight becomes. Thus, dispositional insight is negatively correlated with the rGMV and RSFC of relevant brain regions. Finally, the mediation analysis showed that neuroticism mediated the linkage between the TLM.L volume and IES.

The TLM is a hub region widely connected with the whole brain (Van Den Heuvel and Sporns, 2011). It is mainly responsible for relaying information and involves reward processing (Gazzaniga, 2004; Boccia et al., 2015; Jung et al., 2015; Takeuchi et al., 2017; Cristofori et al., 2018; Tik et al., 2018). Previous studies on divergent thinking found that reduced dopamine receptors in the TLM were associated with reduced information filtering and reduced inhibition of prefrontal neurons, which contributed to performances requiring increased flexibility (Jung et al., 2015). A previous study indicated that expected reward can facilitate insight problem solving (Cristofori et al., 2018). In insight problem solving, problem solvers needed to perform a variety of solution attempts to find the correct solution (Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2003). Therefore, the dispositional insight correlated with TLM may indicate a relationship between dispositional insight and information searching, and reward processing. In addition, TPJ is generally believed to act as a core region involved in attention reorienting and forming novel associations (Qiu et al., 2008; Berkowitz and Ansari, 2010; Chang et al., 2013). Previous studies indicated that attentional resources should be deployed to prevent reorienting to distracting stimuli during divergent thinking (Corbetta et al., 2008; Benedek et al., 2014b). Task-fMRI studies suggested that TPJ prevented irrelevant objects from being attended and formed new associations in the insight process (Qiu et al., 2008; Tang et al., 2015). The dispositional insight correlated with TPJ may indicate a linkage between dispositional insight and attention reorienting, and forming associations. Importantly, DMPFC plays a critical role in conflict regulation and cognitive control (Egner and Hirsch, 2005; Krebs et al., 2013; Benedek et al., 2014a). Our findings were in line with previous studies on creativity (Beaty and Silvia, 2012; Benedek et al., 2013, 2014a). For example, generating creative ideas was associated with top-down control and executive processes (Benedek et al., 2013). In creative thinking, individual need to inhibit numerous competing responses before a suitable response was found (Benedek et al., 2014a). Moreover, inhibiting the dominant mental sets and forming new representations were crucial for episode insight (Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2003). Taken together, altered brain structure in the DMPFC might be associated with dispositional insight.

In addition, dispositional insight was negatively correlated with the RSFC of TPJ.R-TPJ.L, TPJ.R-DLPFC.L, TPJ.R-VLPFC.L, TPJ.R-DMPFC.L, TPJ.R-INS.R, TPJ.R-CRBL.R, DMPFC.L-TPJ.L, and DMPFC.L-TPJ.R. Lateral prefrontal cortex, including the DLPFC and VLPFC, involves inhibition of dominant but irrelevant responses and cognitive flexibility (Derrfuss et al., 2005; Volle et al., 2011). Usually, the cerebellum is linked to working memory and executive functions (Ravizza et al., 2005; Keren-Happuch et al., 2014). Previous studies noted that cognitive flexibility made it possible for problem solvers to think away from ordinary thinking, which was in line with our present study (Munakata et al., 2011; Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013). Insula is associated with subjective emotion (Quartz, 2009; Singer et al., 2009). In the insightful tasks, insight solutions often evoked “aha” feelings (Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2007; Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013). Thus, we speculate that activations in the lateral prefrontal cortex, cerebellum, and insula may imply a link between the dispositional insight, cognitive inhibition, and “aha” feelings.

Notably, previous studies indicated that the involvement of the frontal and parieto-temporal networks might be associated with creative generation tasks (Takeuchi et al., 2010; Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2020). In a task-fMRI study, an insight solution involved more robust functional connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and temporal cortex than a non-insight solution (Zhao et al., 2014). Consistent with these studies, we found that the RSFC between the frontal and parieto-temporal regions involved dispositional insight. In sum, we infer that specific brain structures associated with RSFC contribute to dispositional insight.

Finally, we detected that neuroticism mediated the link between the TLM.L and dispositional insight. Specifically, our findings highlighted that rGMV of the TLM.L was significantly correlated with neuroticism. Several studies have revealed associations between the TLM and neuroticism (Takano et al., 2007; Servaas et al., 2013; Kong et al., 2015b). For example, there was a relationship between serotonin transporter binding in the TLM and neuroticism (Takano et al., 2007). Moreover, imaging research suggested that neuroticism was related to structural variations in the TLM (Kong et al., 2015b). Previous studies revealed that individuals with higher neuroticism tended to accompany anxiety and worry emotional states (Carver et al., 2000; Leung et al., 2014). The emotional states facilitated analyses of complex problems and attainments of desired outcomes to prevent adverse effects (Andrews and Thomson, 2009; Leung et al., 2014). Besides, researches suggested that individuals with high scores on neuroticism tended to dwell on problems and recruit many rumination-related processes, such as worrying, which were helpful for creative problem-solving (Perkins et al., 2015). Individuals with higher neuroticism could generate many unusual thoughts in the idea generation tasks (Leung et al., 2014). These studies imply that neuroticism might facilitate insightful performances, in line with our findings suggested that neuroticism was positively correlated with dispositional insight. It is rational that neuroticism plays a mediation role in the relationship between TLM and dispositional insight.

Considering the important role of TLM in dispositional insight, the TML was further decoded for cognitive terms with Neurosynth toolbox4 to depict the functional validity of the TLM (Rubin et al., 2017). The top 50 terms were first selected, from which we only reported the top five terms related to specific cognitive and emotional functions in descending order. Results showed that terms were closely correlated with anticipation, monetary reward, hypoactivation, sensations, and mood. This finding is in line with previous studies, where insight could involve the processes of reward, active searching, and mood (i.e., “aha” feelings) (Cristofori et al., 2018; Tik et al., 2018). Therefore, the dispositional insight correlated with TLM may indicate a relationship between dispositional insight, reward processing, active searching, and emotional processing. However, the results from the decoding analysis were descriptive, and further studies need to clarify this issue.

The present study found that dispositional insight was negatively correlated with the rGMV and RSFC of specific brain regions. Consistently, evidence from previous studies supported the association (Chen et al., 2014; Li et al., 2014). For instance, Chen et al. (2014) found that trait creativity was negatively correlated with the rGMV in the dorsal ACC and the RSFC between the dorsal ACC and MSFC. However, there were contradictory studies indicating an inconsistent association (Gansler et al., 2011; Takeuchi et al., 2012; Beaty et al., 2014; Li et al., 2014; Zhao et al., 2014). For instance, Li et al. (2014) reported that creative subjects had increased rGMV in the middle temporal gyrus. Takeuchi et al. (2012) showed that trait creativity was positively related to the RSFC between the MPFC and posterior cingulate cortex. In fact, it is hard to explain the relationship between decreased or increased rGMV and creativity. Previous studies indicated that increased rGMV reflects neuroplasticity, which may be helpful for task performance (Draganski et al., 2006; Chen et al., 2014). However, other studies showed that decreased rGMV reflects neuronal pruning processes, which may be related to better cognitive performance (Kanai and Rees, 2011; Duan et al., 2012). Thus, we speculated that neuronal pruning processes might be the reason for decreased rGMV, contributing to high dispositional insight in this study. In addition, previous studies suggested a consistent relationship between gray matter alteration and RSFC (Chen et al., 2014; Niu et al., 2018). For instance, in a trait creativity study, researchers found that decreased rGMV in the dorsal ACC appears to result in the reduced RSFC within the salience network (Chen et al., 2014). Therefore, we deduced that decreased rGMV might be the reason for decreased RSFC, contributing to high dispositional insight in this study. Finally, we didn’t estimate Chinese language abilities of subjects in the present study. However, we recruited the student subjects randomly, who have passed the college entrance examination. We assumed that they have similar language abilities.

Conclusion

The present study utilized a multi-modal imaging method to reveal the structural and functional mechanisms underlying dispositional insight. We found that rGMV in the TLM, TPJ, and DMPFC was related to dispositional insight. Altered RSFC of the frontal and parieto-temporal regions also involved dispositional insight. In addition, neuroticism was proved to be a mediational mechanism explaining the link between the TLM volume and dispositional insight. Our study provides a new understanding of the brain structure and functions underlying dispositional insight.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Research Ethics Review Board of South China Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JL and LM developed the study concept. QC, MZ, and YC performed testing and data collection. JL and YC performed the data analysis and interpretation. JL, JX, and YC drafted the manuscript. JL, JX, and LM provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Funding

This work was supported by funding from the National Social Science Foundation of China (Grant Number: 14ZDB159), the Project of Key Institute of Humanities and Social Sciences, MOE (Grant Number: 16JJD880025), the Philosophy and Social Science Project of Guangdong Province (Grant Number: GD20YXL01), the Guangdong Basic and Applied Basic Research Foundation (Grant Number: 2020A1515110737), the Project of Guangzhou Philosophies and Social Sciences (Grant Number: 2020GZQN43), and the Humanities and Social Sciences Project of Guangzhou University of Chinese Medicine (Grant Number: 2020SKYB07).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/
  2. ^ http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat12/CAT12-Manual.pdf
  3. ^ http://rfmri.org/dpabi
  4. ^ https://www.neurosynth.org/

References

Andrews, P. W., and Thomson, J. A. Jr. (2009). The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems. Psychol. Rev. 116:620. doi: 10.1037/a0016242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ash, I. K., and Wiley, J. (2006). The nature of restructuring in insight: an individual-differences approach. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 66–73. doi: 10.3758/bf03193814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batey, M. D. (2007). A Psychometric Investigation Of Everyday Creativity. London: University of London.

Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., and Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 6:309.

Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R. W., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., et al. (2014). Creativity and the default network: a functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia 64, 92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Benedek, M., Chen, Q., et al. (2018). Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 1087–1092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713532115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., et al. (2014b). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage 88, 125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Beaty, R., Jauk, E., Koschutnig, K., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., et al. (2014a). Creating metaphors: the neural basis of figurative language production. NeuroImage 90, 99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Mühlmann, C., Jauk, E., and Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Assessment of divergent thinking by means of the subjective top-scoring method: effects of the number of top-ideas and time-on-task on reliability and validity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 7:341. doi: 10.1037/a0033644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, A. L., and Ansari, D. (2010). Expertise-related deactivation of the right temporoparietal junction during musical improvisation. Neuroimage 49, 712–719. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besteher, B., Squarcina, L., Spalthoff, R., Bellani, M., Gaser, C., Brambilla, P., et al. (2017). Brain structural correlates of irritability: findings in a large healthy cohort. Hum. Brain Mapping 38, 6230–6238. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boccia, M., Piccardi, L., Palermo, L., Nori, R., and Palmiero, M. (2015). Where do bright ideas occur in our brain? Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies of domain-specific creativity. Front. Psychol. 6:1195. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowden, E. M., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Aha! Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 730–737. doi: 10.3758/bf03196539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowden, E. M., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2007). Methods for investigating the neural components of insight. Methods 42, 87–99. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruyer, R., and Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychol. Belg. 51, 5–13.

Google Scholar

Byrne, R. M., and Murray, M. A. (2005). Attention and working memory in insight problem-solving. Proc. Annu. Meet. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 27, 1571–1575.

Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., and Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: emerging conceptual integration. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 741–751.

Google Scholar

Chang, C. F., Hsu, T. Y., Tseng, P., Liang, W. K., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L., et al. (2013). Right temporoparietal junction and attentional reorienting. Hum. Brain Mapping 34, 869–877. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., Yang, W., Li, W., Wei, D., Li, H., Lei, Q., et al. (2014). Association of creative achievement with cognitive flexibility by a combined voxel-based morphometry and resting-state functional connectivity study. Neuroimage 102, 474–483. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Patel, G., and Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. Jr., and McCrae, R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor (NEO-FFI) Inventory Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: PAR.

Google Scholar

Cristofori, I., Salvi, C., Beeman, M., and Grafman, J. (2018). The effects of expected reward on creative problem solving. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 925–931. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0613-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeCaro, M. S., and Wieth, M. B. (2016). When higher working memory capacity hinders insight. J. Exp. Psychol. 42:39. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derrfuss, J., Brass, M., Neumann, J., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Hum. Brain Mapping 25, 22–34. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., and Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience: brain structure and the big five. Psychol. Sci. 21, 820–828. doi: 10.1177/0956797610370159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., Winkler, J., Büchel, C., et al. (2006). Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J. Neurosci. 26, 6314–6317. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4628-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, X., He, S., Liao, W., Liang, D., Qiu, L., Wei, L., et al. (2012). Reduced caudate volume and enhanced striatal-DMN integration in chess experts. Neuroimage 60, 1280–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nat. Neurosci. 8:1784. doi: 10.1038/nn1594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farokhian, F., Beheshti, I., Sone, D., and Matsuda, H. (2017). Comparing CAT12 and VBM8 for detecting brain morphological abnormalities in temporal lobe epilepsy. Front. Neurol. 8:428. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Bachtiar, V. (2008). Personality and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Pers. Individ. Diff. 45, 613–617.

Google Scholar

Gansler, D. A., Moore, D. W., Susmaras, T. M., Jerram, M. W., Sousa, J., and Heilman, K. M. (2011). Cortical morphology of visual creativity. Neuropsychologia 49, 2527–2532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzaniga, M. S. (2004). The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Gilhooly, K. J., and Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non-insight problems. Think. Reason. 11, 279–302.

Google Scholar

Gilhooly, K., and Fioratou, E. (2009). Executive functions in insight versus non-insight problem solving: an individual differences approach. Think. Reason. 15, 355–376.

Google Scholar

Gonen-Yaacovi, G., De Souza, L. C., Levy, R., Urbanski, M., Josse, G., and Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal contribution to creativity: a meta-analysis of functional imaging data. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:465. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayasaka, S., Phan, K. L., Liberzon, I., Worsley, K. J., and Nichols, T. E. (2004). Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage 22, 676–687. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, F., Fan, J., and Luo, J. (2015). The neural basis of novelty and appropriateness in processing of creative chunk decomposition. Neuroimage 113, 122–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, F., Tang, S., Sun, P., and Luo, J. (2018). Neural correlates of novelty and appropriateness processing in externally induced constraint relaxation. NeuroImage 172, 381–389. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, M. K., and Dominowski, R. L. (1981). Learning to solve insight problems. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 17, 171–174.

Google Scholar

Jung, R. E., Wertz, C. J., Meadows, C. A., Ryman, S. G., Vakhtin, A. A., and Flores, R. A. (2015). Quantity yields quality when it comes to creativity: a brain and behavioral test of the equal-odds rule. Front. Psychol. 6:864. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., et al. (2004). Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biol. 2:e97. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kajonius, P., and Mac Giolla, E. (2017). Personality traits across countries: Support for similarities rather than differences. PLoS One 12:e0179646. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., and Rees, G. (2011). The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 231. doi: 10.1038/nrn3000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keren-Happuch, E., Chen, S.-H. A., Ho, M.-H. R., and Desmond, J. E. (2014). A meta-analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Hum. Brain Mapping 35, 593. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, L. A., Walker, L. M., and Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity and the five-factor model. J. Res. Pers. 30, 189–203.

Google Scholar

Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., and Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. J. Exp. Psychol. 25:1534.

Google Scholar

Kong, F., Liu, L., Wang, X., Hu, S., Song, Y., and Liu, J. (2015b). Different neural pathways linking personality traits and eudaimonic well-being: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 299–309. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0328-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, F., Hu, S., Xue, S., Song, Y., and Liu, J. (2015a). Extraversion mediates the relationship between structural variations in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and social well-being. Neuroimage 105, 269–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kounios, J., and Beeman, M. (2009). The Aha! moment: the cognitive neuroscience of insight. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 210–216.

Google Scholar

Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T. B., et al. (2006). The prepared mind: neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychol. Sci. 17, 882–890. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01798.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krebs, R. M., Boehler, C. N., De Belder, M., and Egner, T. (2013). Neural conflict–control mechanisms improve memory for target stimuli. Cereb. Cortex 25, 833–843. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, A. K.-Y., Liou, S., Qiu, L., Kwan, L. Y.-Y., Chiu, C.-Y., and Yong, J. C. (2014). The role of instrumental emotion regulation in the emotions–creativity link: how worries render individuals with high neuroticism more creative. Emotion 14:846. doi: 10.1037/a0036965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Li, X., Huang, L., Kong, X., Yang, W., Wei, D., et al. (2014). Brain structure links trait creativity to openness to experience. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 191–198. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J., Lv, X., Niu, M., Liu, L., Chen, J., Xie, F., et al. (2017). Radiation-induced abnormal cortical thickness in patients with nasopharyngeal carcinoma after radiotherapy. Neuroimage Clin. 14, 610–621. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J., Wen, X., Cui, X., Xiang, Y., Xie, J., Chen, Y., et al. (2020). Common and specific neural correlates underlying insight and ordinary problem solving. Brain Imaging Behav. 15, 1374–1387. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00337-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, J., Niki, K., and Knoblich, G. (2006). Perceptual contributions to problem solving: chunk decomposition of Chinese characters. Brain Res. Bull. 70, 430–443. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, I. B., Leung, K. K., Clegg, S., Barnes, J., Whitwell, J. L., Ashburner, J., et al. (2015). Accurate automatic estimation of total intracranial volume: a nuisance variable with less nuisance. Neuroimage 104, 366–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcoulides, G. A., and Hershberger, S. L. (1997). Multivariate Statistical Methods: A First Course. Hove: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Metcalfe, J., and Wiebe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Mem. Cogn. 15, 238–246. doi: 10.3758/bf03197722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., and O’Reilly, R. C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 453–459.

Google Scholar

Niu, C., Tan, X., Liu, X., Han, K., Niu, M., Xu, J., et al. (2018). Cortical thickness reductions associate with abnormal resting-state functional connectivity in non-neuropsychiatric systemic lupus erythematosus. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 674–684. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9729-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogawa, T., Aihara, T., Shimokawa, T., and Yamashita, O. (2018). Large-scale brain network associated with creative insight: combined voxel-based morphometry and resting-state functional connectivity analyses. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24981-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ovington, L. A., Saliba, A. J., and Goldring, J. (2016). Dispositional insight scale: development and validation of a tool that measures propensity toward insight in problem solving. Creat. Res. J. 28, 342–347.

Google Scholar

Perkins, A. M., Arnone, D., Smallwood, J., and Mobbs, D. (2015). Thinking too much: self-generated thought as the engine of neuroticism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 492–498. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891.

Google Scholar

Qiu, J., Li, H., Jou, J., Liu, J., Luo, Y., Feng, T., et al. (2010). Neural correlates of the “Aha” experiences: evidence from an fMRI study of insight problem solving. Cortex 46, 397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, J., Li, H., Yang, D., Luo, Y., Li, Y., Wu, Z., et al. (2008). The neural basis of insight problem solving: an event-related potential study. Brain Cogn. 68, 100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quartz, S. R. (2009). Reason, emotion and decision-making: risk and reward computation with feeling. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravizza, S. M., McCormick, C. A., Schlerf, J. E., Justus, T., Ivry, R. B., and Fiez, J. A. (2005). Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working memory. Brain 129, 306–320. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, J., Huang, F., Zhou, Y., Zhuang, L., Xu, J., Gao, C., et al. (2020). The function of the hippocampus and middle temporal gyrus in forming new associations and concepts during the processing of novelty and usefulness features in creative designs. Neuroimage 214, 116751.

Google Scholar

Reyes, G., and Sackur, J. (2014). Introspection during visual search. Conscious. Cogn. 29, 212–229. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, T. N., Koyejo, O., Gorgolewski, K. J., Jones, M. N., Poldrack, R. A., and Yarkoni, T. (2017). Decoding brain activity using a large-scale probabilistic functional-anatomical atlas of human cognition. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13:e1005649. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadana, D., Gupta, R. K., Jain, S., Kumaran, S. S., and Rajeswaran, J. (2021). Correlates of creativity: an association between creativity, personality, and intelligence. PsyArXiv [Preprint] doi: 10.31234/osf.io/qdby6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Servaas, M. N., Van Der Velde, J., Costafreda, S. G., Horton, P., Ormel, J., Riese, H., et al. (2013). Neuroticism and the brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies investigating emotion processing. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1518–1529. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., and Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 334–340. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., and Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. Am. Psychol. 51:677.

Google Scholar

Sun, J., Liu, Z., Rolls, E. T., Chen, Q., Yao, Y., Yang, W., et al. (2018). Verbal creativity correlates with the temporal variability of brain networks during the resting state. Cereb. Cortex 29, 1047–1058. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takano, A., Arakawa, R., Hayashi, M., Takahashi, H., Ito, H., and Suhara, T. (2007). Relationship between neuroticism personality trait and serotonin transporter binding. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 588–592. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., et al. (2012). The association between resting functional connectivity and creativity. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2921–2929.

Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., et al. (2010). White matter structures associated with creativity: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 51, 11–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sekiguchi, A., Nouchi, R., Kotozaki, Y., Nakagawa, S., et al. (2017). Mean diffusivity of basal ganglia and thalamus specifically associated with motivational states among mood states. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 1027–1037. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1262-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Tsurumi, K., Murao, T., Mizuta, H., Murai, T., and Takahashi, H. (2018). Amygdala volume is associated with risky probability cognition in gambling disorder. Addict. Biol. 24, 802–810. doi: 10.1111/adb.12640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, S., Jia, L., Ren, J., Li, F., Luo, J., and Huang, F. (2022). Reactive control in evaluating appropriately or inappropriately novel ideas: evidence from electrophysiological measures. Psychophysiology e14010. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, X., Pang, J., Nie, Q.-Y., Conci, M., Luo, J., and Luo, J. (2015). Probing the cognitive mechanism of mental representational change during chunk decomposition: a parametric fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 26, 2991–2999. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tik, M., Sladky, R., Luft, C. D. B., Willinger, D., Hoffmann, A., Banissy, M. J., et al. (2018). Ultra-high-field fMRI insights on insight: neural correlates of the Aha!-moment. Hum. Brain Mapping 39, 3241–3252. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, J. T., and Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. Cogn. Theory 3, 200–239.

Google Scholar

Van Den Heuvel, M. P., and Sporns, O. (2011). Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J. Neurosci. 31, 15775–15786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volle, E., de Lacy Costello, A., Coates, L. M., McGuire, C., Towgood, K., Gilbert, S., et al. (2011). Dissociation between verbal response initiation and suppression after prefrontal lesions. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2428–2440. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M., Zhang, A., Zhang, J., Lu, H., Xu, S., Qin, Z., et al. (2018). Morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study in children with primary monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis. Front. Pediatr. 6:103. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, M. E., Little, D. R., and Cropper, S. J. (2016). Insight is not in the problem: investigating insight in problem solving across task types. Front. Psychol. 7:1424. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, H., Zhou, R., Zhao, L., Qiu, J., and Guo, C. (2019). Neural bases underlying the association between balanced time perspective and trait anxiety. Behav. Brain Res. 359, 206–214. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.10.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, L., Knoblich, G., and Luo, J. (2013). The role of chunk tightness and chunk familiarity in problem solving: evidence from ERPs and fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapping 34, 1173–1186. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, L., Knoblich, G., Wei, G., and Luo, J. (2009). How perceptual processes help to generate new meaning: an EEG study of chunk decomposition in Chinese characters. Brain Res. 1296, 104–112. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, Y., Kong, F., Wen, X., Wu, Q., and Mo, L. (2016). Neural correlates of envy: Regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity predicts dispositional envy. NeuroImage 142, 225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, C.-G., Cheung, B., Kelly, C., Colcombe, S., Craddock, R. C., Di Martino, A., et al. (2013). A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional connectomics. Neuroimage 76, 183–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, C.-G., Wang, X.-D., Zuo, X.-N., and Zang, Y.-F. (2016). DPABI: data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics 14, 339–351.

Google Scholar

Yang, J., McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., Dai, X., Yao, S., Cai, T., et al. (1999). Cross-cultural personality assessment in psychiatric populations: the NEO-PI—R in the People’s Republic of China. Psychol. Assess. 11:359.

Google Scholar

Zhao, Q., Zhou, Z., Xu, H., Fan, W., and Han, L. (2014). Neural pathway in the right hemisphere underlies verbal insight problem solving. Neuroscience 256, 334–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dispositional insight, structural brain mechanism, voxel-based morphometry, functional connectivity, mediation analysis

Citation: Lin J, Chen Y, Xie J, Cheng Q, Zou M and Mo L (2022) Brain Structural Correlates of Dispositional Insight and the Mediation Role of Neuroticism in Young Adults. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:846377. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.846377

Received: 24 January 2022; Accepted: 24 March 2022;
Published: 15 April 2022.

Edited by:

Edward F. Pace-Schott, Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Junyi Yang, Xinyang Normal University, China
Ruibin Zhang, Southern Medical University, China

Copyright © 2022 Lin, Chen, Xie, Cheng, Zou and Mo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lei Mo, molei@m.scnu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Download