Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 22 October 2020
Sec. Neurorehabilitation
This article is part of the Research Topic Non-Pharmacological Interventions in Acquired and Age-Related Cognitive Disorders View all 8 articles

Incident Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Difficulty in Older Adults: Which Comes First? Findings From the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study

  • 1Center on Aging and Health, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 2Department of Epidemiology, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 4Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Health Sciences, La Jolla, CA, United States
  • 6Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida College of Public Health and Health Professions, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 7Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 8Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health, Pittsburgh, PA, United States

Introduction: Instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) are complex daily tasks important for independent living. Many older adults experience difficulty with IADLs as their physical and/or cognitive function begins to decline. However, it is unknown in what order IADLs become difficult.

Methods: Participants from the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) study who were free of IADL difficulty at baseline (N = 1,277) were followed up to 10 years until first reported IADL difficulty. A total of 19 IADL tasks were grouped into seven task categories. A discrete-time multiple-event process survival mixture model (MEPSUM) was used to generate hazard estimates of incident IADL difficulty in seven groups from ages 65 to 80. Hazard estimates were compared in the three intervention groups (memory, inductive reasoning, and speed of information processing) vs. the no-contact control group.

Results: A total of 887 (69.5%) participants reported incident difficulty in at least one IADL task category. Compared to individuals who remained free of IADL difficulty, those who reported incident difficulty were more likely to be older, female, and have lower Short Form 36 general health scores. The IADL task categories to first become difficult were housework, managing health care, and phone use. There were no differences by intervention group in the hazard estimates of incident IADL difficulty.

Conclusion: Managing health care and phone use are more cognitively demanding IADLs, and individuals who experience difficulty in these tasks first may be more likely to experience cognitive decline. Recognizing early difficulty in managing health care may allow for implementation of compensation strategies to minimize unintentional medication misuse, increased adverse medical events, and unnecessary hospitalization. Training of a specific cognitive domain may not influence ordering of IADL difficulty because IADL tasks require proficiency in, and integration of, multiple cognitive domains.

Introduction

Difficulty in performing daily activities increases with age (1, 2) due to declining physical and cognitive functioning. Maintaining functional independence is of great importance for older adults (3) and is associated with increased quality of life (4) and lower health care expenditures (5). Common everyday activities are categorized into two groups: instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) and basic activities of daily living (ADLs). IADLs encompass more complex tasks important for independent living (e.g., cooking, household chores, and handling money) (6), while ADLs are basic with more physical tasks of self-care necessary for independent living, and include functions such as bathing, dressing, and feeding (7). Because IADL tasks are more complex, most older adults experience difficulty with some IADLs before they experience difficulty with ADLs (8).

Prior research has evaluated the hierarchical progression of ADL difficulty. Katz et al. (7), who developed the original ADL scale, theorized that loss of ADL function mirrored the developmental achievement of ADLs in young children: skills that are obtained first in childhood such as self-feeding are lost last in older adults. Subsequent studies have generally found patterns consistent with this hypothesis, with mobility and bathing usually the first ADLs to become difficult, followed by transferring, dressing, toileting, and feeding becoming difficult last (911). However, among community-dwelling older adults, patterns of progressive difficulty are varied (12, 13).

In contrast to ADL difficulty, the relative order in which IADLs become difficult has not been well-studied. Studies of progression of IADL difficulty often consider only a limited set of IADLs (14, 15), increasing counts of difficult IADLs (1618), or using one scale that fails to distinguish IADLs from ADLs (1922). These techniques do not account for relationships among IADLs and may miss transitional patterns (23). Because IADL performance is thought to reflect underlying cognitive and physical function (24, 25), identifying early incident IADL difficulty may facilitate earlier intervention to maintain remaining function.

The primary objective of this study was to determine the relative ordering of incident difficulty across 19 IADLs representing seven task groups (preparing meals, housework, managing finances, managing health care, phone use, shopping, and travel outside of the home) in a large sample of high-functioning, community-living older adults. The secondary objective was to determine whether this relative ordering differed in individuals receiving cognitive training relative to a control group. We hypothesize that older adults, on average, first report problems with more cognitively demanding IADLs including managing finances, managing medications, and managing health care.

While previous studies within the ACTIVE cohort have shown that cognitive intervention improved ability on performance-based IADL measures (26, 27), we did not expect to observe any differences in first incident IADL difficulty in individuals receiving cognitive training compared to those not receiving cognitive training.

Materials and Methods

Study Sample

We examined data from the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) study. Methods of the ACTIVE study have been described elsewhere (27). Briefly, N = 2,802 community-living older adults aged 65 years and older were recruited from six US geographical sites beginning in 1998, randomized to a cognitive training intervention, and followed for up to 10 years with in-person visits. Participants were randomized to receive one of three cognitive interventions (memory, inductive reasoning, speed of information processing) or a no-contact control group. The primary outcome of interest in the original ACTIVE study was everyday functioning, as defined by IADLs. Secondary outcomes of interest for the original ACTIVE study included everyday processing speed and driving habits. Data collection occurred at baseline, immediately post-training at 10 weeks, and at follow-up at years 1, 2, 3, 5, and 10. For this analysis, the primary outcome was first incident IADL difficulty; only participants free of all IADL difficulty items at baseline were included (N = 1,277; 45.6% of total sample) without regard to their ADL status. Because IADLs were not assessed at the immediate post-training visit, this visit was excluded from the present study. Each study site's local institutional review board approved the ACTIVE study.

Variables

Participants' self-reported IADL ability for 19 tasks at each study visit (Table 2). For each task, participants were asked, (1) “In the last 7 days, how much of the activity did you do on your own?” and (2) “How difficult was it (or would it have been) to do on your own?” Possible responses for (1) included 1—Did all on own; 2—Some help some of the time; 3—Help all of the time; 4—Fully performed by others; and 5—Activity not performed by you or others. Possible responses for (2) included 1—Not difficult; 2—Some help needed or I am slow, or I became tired; 3—Great difficulty. Participants' response to (2) was the primary outcome of this analysis, and a response of 2 or 3 constituted having difficulty with the specific task.

Adjustment Variables

Both unadjusted and adjusted analyses were performed. Adjustment variables included age at study entry, sex, race (white, black, or other), years of education, and baseline self-reported global health. The global health score was calculated using the MOS Short Form 36 (28) (SF-36) general health subscale and ranges from 0 (worst) to 100 (best).

Analysis Plan

Means and proportions were used to describe baseline demographic and health characteristics, and t-tests and chi-square tests were used to evaluate for differences between those who ever reported any incident IADL difficulty during the study and those who never did. Participants were followed until first incident difficulty with any IADL; more than one IADL could become newly difficult at the same study visit. The proportion of participants reporting incident IADL difficulty at each follow-up visit was also calculated.

The 19 IADLs assessed in ACTIVE were a priori grouped into seven categories of related tasks for model estimation and ease of interpretation of findings (Table 2). Patterns of relative worsening in IADL groups were evaluated using a discrete-time multiple-event process survival mixture (MEPSUM) model (29). To use a biologically relevant timescale, we aligned participant follow-up by chronological age instead of study follow-up time and incorporated left-hand censoring for individuals (30). Advantages of the MEPSUM model over traditional proportional hazards survival models include accommodation of multiple non-repeated events that may be reported simultaneously during discrete study visits, thereby accounting for ties between different categories of IADLs. Results from the MEPSUM model are provided as the probability of incident IADL difficulty for each task at each year of age, which may be interpreted equivalently to hazard estimates derived using standard univariate discrete-time survival analysis methods (29, 31). All models were estimated using ages 66–80 years to aid in model fitting.

To statistically test which IADLs were more likely to occur first, McNemar's test (32) was applied to each possible pairing of IADL task, for each age from 66 to 80 years. For each pairing, the number of participants who experienced difficulty in one task, both tasks, or none was calculated. Discordant pairs (difficulty in one task only) represent individuals for whom only one IADL was newly difficult (and thus became difficult first). For each pairing, a task in which a significantly larger percentage of participants reported difficulty in only that task indicates that a specific task was more likely to become difficult first relative to the other tasks.

A second MEPSUM model was constructed to evaluate differences in the probability of incident IADL difficulty comparing the intervention groups (memory, inductive reasoning, and speed of processing) separately relative to the control group. Because the absolute number of incident IADL difficulties within each intervention group was small, age was grouped into intervals of 5 years to facilitate model fitting (ages 66–70, 71–75, and 76–80). Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals comparing the probability (hazard) estimates between each of the three intervention groups relative to the control group were generated automatically using the cinterval command in Mplus.

Descriptive analyses and McNemar's tests were performed using Stata version 15.1 (StataCorp 2017, College Station, TX, USA). MEPSUM analyses were estimated in Mplus version 8.2 (Muthén & Muthén 2018, Los Angeles, CA, USA) using a maximum likelihood estimator with 300 initial stage random starts and 60 final stage optimizations. Results with p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Of the N = 1,277 participants included who had no prevalent IADL difficulty, most were white (73.9%), female (77.0%), and the average age at baseline was 72.8 ± 5.5 years. The majority of participants were either married (36.0%) or widowed (40.6%). The average years of education completed was 13.67 ± 2.63 years, and the average SF-36 general health score at baseline was 72.27 ± 18.20. The median time to a first incident IADL difficulty was 5 years. Compared to ACTIVE participants who entered the study reporting difficulty with at least one IADL (N = 1,525, 54.4%), participants free of IADL difficulty at baseline were, on average, younger (72.8 vs. 74.3 years; p < 0.001), had higher education (13.67 vs. 13.41 years; p = 0.014), higher SF-36 general health scores (74.07 vs. 64.66; p < 0.001), and were more likely to be white (71.1 vs. 54.8%; p = 0.032). No difference in marital status (p = 0.314) or sex (77.0 vs. 75.0% female; p = 0.212) between groups was observed.

Of the N = 1,277 participants free of IADL difficulty at baseline, 887 (69.5%) developed incident difficulty with any IADL task over 10 years of follow-up. Figure 1 shows the distribution of people contributing data by age. For most (59.6%), the first incident IADL difficulty occurred alone, and for another 19.1% the first incident difficulty occurred with two tasks at the same visit. For the remainder of participants (21.3%), the first incident difficulty occurred with more than two tasks at the same visit. Compared to participants who remained free of incident IADL difficulty throughout the study, participants with incident IADL difficulty were older (73.52 vs. 71.18 years; p < 0.001), more likely to be female (78.8 vs. 72.8%; p = 0.019), had worse SF-36 global health scores at baseline (69.92 vs. 77.75; p < 0.001), and had lower total years of education (13.54 vs. 13.96; p = 0.007). Those who experienced incident IADL difficulty were also more likely to be widowed at baseline (43.4 vs. 34.1%; p = 0.010) (Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Number of people contributing data at each year of age: results from Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) (N = 1,277).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) sample free of instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) difficulty at baseline.

Table 2 shows the number of incident difficult IADLs by study visit. Although most participants developed incident difficulty at some point during the study, no one task accounted for the majority of incident cases. The first task to become difficult was quite heterogenous in this population. The most common IADLs to become difficult first were giving self-injections, applying ointments, and changing bandages, representing 13.76% of all new incident cases. The second most common IADL to become difficult first was doing dishes, dusting, making beds, and tidying up (13.37%), and the third most common IADL to become difficult was remembering often-called numbers without having to look them up (11.05%). The IADL task least likely to become difficult first was hanging up at the end of a phone call (0.77%), followed by answering the phone (1.22%), and keeping household expenses balanced (2.93%).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Number (%) of incident difficult IADLS, by study visit: descriptive results from ACTIVE (N = 1,277).

Main Findings

Figures 2, 3 show the estimated probability of incident IADL difficulty at each year of age, for each IADL task group from the adjusted model. Both unadjusted and adjusted MEPSUM models containing one latent class were estimated. For the unadjusted model, the number of free parameters was 103, the log likelihood (LL) was −4,611.281, the AIC was 9,428.562, and the BIC was 9,949.082. For the adjusted model, the number of free parameters was 138, the LL was −4,384.630, the AIC was 9,045.260, and the BIC was 9,737.923. The estimated probability may be interpreted as the hazard of incident IADL difficulty, which is the probability of an individual experiencing difficulty in a specific IADL at a specific age given that they have remained free of any IADL difficulty up until that age. Findings of the adjusted model are as follows: Although the majority of participants experienced incident difficulty at some point during the study, the probability of experiencing new difficulty at any 1 year of age was low due to the wide dispersion of age at baseline. The hazard of experiencing difficulty with managing finances and traveling was both low and constant across all age groups. Difficulty with preparing meals and shopping increased beginning around the age of 75. Most notable is the steady increase in the probability of incident difficulty with housework, managing health care, and phone use with age. In the unadjusted model, managing health care was the IADL with the highest hazard of becoming difficult first, followed by housework, and phone use (see Supplemental Material). In the adjusted model, housework was the IADL with the highest hazard of becoming difficult first, followed by managing health care and phone use.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Hazard of incident IADL difficulty by task group, adjusted model: results from ACTIVE (N = 1,277). Results of discrete-time multiple-event process survival mixture model (MEPSUM) model, as described in the Methods section. Figure shows the hazard estimates of incident difficulty in seven Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) task groups as a function of age. The hazards are estimated from a model with age 65 years as the time origin.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Probability of remaining free of IADL difficulty by task group, adjusted model: results from ACTIVE (N = 1,277). Results of MEPSUM model, as described in the Methods section. Figure shows the probability of remaining free of difficulty in seven Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) task groups as a function of age. This survival probability is derived using model hazard estimates in the standard discrete time survival formula [see Ref. (28)].

Results from McNemar's test to statistically test which tasks occurred earlier are in Table 3. Difficulty preparing meals was significantly less likely to occur before difficulty performing housework [ratio range (0.19–0.67)], managing health care [ratio range (0.11–0.46)], or using the phone [ratio range (0.12–1.000)]. Difficulty performing housework was significantly more likely to occur before difficulty managing money [ratio range (0.88–3.86)], shopping [ratio range (1.27–4.20)], and traveling [ratio range (1.43–8.00)], but less likely to become difficult before managing health care [ratio range (0.37–1.13)]. Difficulty managing money was less likely to occur before managing health care [ratio range (0.25–0.82)] or using the phone [ratio range (0.30–2.25)], while managing health care was more likely to become difficult before using the phone [ratio range (0.90–3.57)], shopping [ratio range (1.91–12.00)], or traveling [ratio range (3.00–13.50)]. Finally, phone use was more likely to become difficult before shopping [ratio range (1.00–8.50)] and traveling [ratio range (1.00–10.00)].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Odds ratio of each IADL becoming difficult first: results from McNemar's test.

Effect of Cognitive Intervention

In the secondary analysis, there were no statistically significant differences in the hazard probability in any of the intervention groups compared to the control group, with the exception that individuals in the inductive reasoning group were less likely to experience incident difficulty with travel between ages 71–75 years relative to those in the control group [OR = 0.47 (0.16–1.39), p = 0.039] (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparison of each ACTIVE intervention group to control group.

Discussion

Main Findings

In this study, we empirically tested the relative ordering of incident difficulty of seven groupings of IADLs (preparing meals, housework, managing finances, managing health care, phone use, shopping, and travel outside of the home) in a large sample of high-functioning community-living older adults. Overall, the probability of incident IADL difficulty at any one specific age between 65 and 80 years is low, but increases steadily with age, particularly for housework, managing health care, and phone use. Managing health care and phone use represent cognitively demanding tasks, while housework represents more physically demanding tasks.

Population-based cohort studies (14, 15) have reported that difficulty in shopping and housework became difficult first in older adults. While we also found housework to be the most likely to become difficult first in our cohort, the hazard of incident difficulty in both shopping and traveling outside the home was low. This difference in findings may be attributable to selection effects into the ACTIVE study sample. Most participants had to travel to a research facility for assessments and training, necessitating the ability to travel outside the home. Ability to travel is also an essential task for shopping. Despite this, if travel is indeed relatively preserved in the community-living populations of older adults, this has implications for the feasibility of community-based cognitive training interventions, perhaps those with social components, as opposed to self-guided interventions at home.

The steady increase in the probability of incident difficulty with health care tasks is important, given that older adults with multimorbidities require complex health care management (33). Difficulty managing medications is associated with increased hospital admissions, health care costs, and mortality (34). Older adults with memory loss may rate themselves as compliant with medications even when scoring worse on a medication management test compared to healthy peers (35), indicating the early need for assistance with medications. In our study, self-reported incident difficulty with medication management was low but may have been underreported for this reason. Moreover, our study only assessed self-care, but complex health care policies are similarly challenging to navigate for older adults with cognitive impairment (36). Access to care in combination with assistive devices and environmental adaptations could all affect an individual's self-reported difficulty.

It is important to consider that many IADL tasks are sex stereotyped. For example, women traditionally perform cooking and cleaning-related tasks, while men traditionally handle finances. The original developers of early IADL scales suggested using differential scoring for males and females for this reason (6). Trends in IADL difficulty and performance across birth cohorts have shown that the younger cohorts of older adults are less likely to exhibit stereotyped behaviors (37). It is possible that because the majority of subjects in this study are female, we observed little increase in incident difficulty of managing finances and a steady increase in difficulty with housework, although this does not explain why little increase in difficulty with preparing meals was observed.

Cognitive Intervention Effect

Patterns in IADL difficulty may be associated with trajectories of cognitive decline. It is unclear whether cognitive decline or IADL impairment occurs first. Evidence from the ACTIVE study has suggested that deterioration in self-reported difficulty in IADLs precede decline in tests of memory, reasoning, and speed of processing (38); other studies have shown that cognitive impairment predicts future IADL difficulty (39). Executive functioning, in particular, has emerged as an important contributor in ability to perform IADLs (26, 4045). There was no difference in the first IADL difficulty in any of the cognitive training groups relative to the control group in our study. We found no evidence from our analysis to recommend a specific cognitive intervention type to delay IADL difficulty onset; rather, identification of incident cognitively demanding IADL impairment may help select individuals most likely to benefit from cognitive intervention. We plan to further investigate the relationship between incident IADL difficulty and cognitive decline across cognitive intervention groups in future studies.

Although IADL performance is typically worse in persons with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) compared to healthy older adults, it is unclear which IADLs decline first in MCI (46), potentially because specific IADL difficulties may be related to MCI subtype (47). Generally, difficulty with finances has been associated with future progression to dementia, and difficulty with medication use and household activities has been associated with prospective and temporal order memory (48).

Our findings do not contradict previous studies within the ACTIVE cohort showing that cognitive training improves overall self-reported IADL difficulty (26, 49). Previous analyses included individuals with prevalent IADL difficulty at baseline, rather than limiting to individuals who entered the study free of any difficulty. Our study only included individuals with no reported IADL difficulty at baseline, and so, they could not improve on the IADL difficulty scale.

Limitations

One limitation of this study is the use of self-reported, rather than objective or informant-reported, measures of IADL difficulty. Self-reported limitations in older adults do not always match actual performance of the same task (50). However, performance-based measures were collected for several IADLs and may be used in future analyses to confirm results. Another important limitation is that it is not known in this study whether adults were using assistive devices that could impact reported task difficulty. For example, keeping track of doctor appointments was rarely the first incident difficult task, perhaps due to use of calendars.

One challenge in this study was the discrete nature of the data collection; follow-up of participants only occurred at years 1, 2, 3, 5, and 10. Although the majority of participants had only one first incident IADL difficulty, a substantial number of people experienced more than one first incident IADL at a study visit. Likely, these IADL difficulties occurred in some order, but this order was unobserved due to it occurring between visits. Changing the analysis time-scale to age and using discrete time follow-up methods were used to combat this challenge.

Because of the large number of IADL tasks (19) collected in this study, related tasks were combined a priori into seven task groups in order to facilitate model fitting and parsimony of result interpretation. However, tasks within a task group are diverse and involve a variety of different skills. It is possible that some of the variation in skills represented is lost when collapsing the 19 tasks into seven task groups.

Importantly, this study considers first incident IADLs across an entire sample of older adults. It is likely that the first incident IADL varies based on whether an individual is experiencing cognitive deterioration, physical deterioration, or both. Future studies should address this heterogeneity by identifying patterns of first incident IADL difficulty across subgroups of individuals.

Conclusion

The IADLs with the greatest probability of incident difficulty in this study were housework, managing health care, and phone use, and the probability of incident difficulty increased with age. Although patterns of early difficulty were heterogenous in this study and in others, early identification of task difficulty allows for intervention that can reduce health care expenditure and poor outcomes.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/icpsrweb/ICPSR/studies/4248/datadocumentation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DF and AG were involved in the study design and data analysis. DF drafted the initial manuscript. All authors provided critical manuscript reviews and edits.

Funding

DF's doctoral training and research is supported by grant 5T32AG000247-24 issued by the National Institute on Aging.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the members of the ACTIVE everyday functioning group for their thoughts on this topic.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2020.550577/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Field M, Jette A. The Future of Disability in America. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (2007).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. Lin S-F, Beck AN, Finch BK, Hummer RA, Master RK. Trends in US older adult disability: exploring age, period, and cohort effects. Am J Public Health. (2012) 102:2157–63. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Agich GJ. Dependence and Autonomy in Old Age: An Ethical Framework for Long-Term Care. 2nd ed. rev. Cambridge; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (2003). doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511545801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Andersen CK, Wittrup-Jensen KU, Lolk A, Andersen K, Kragh-Sørensen P. Ability to perform activities of daily living is the main factor affecting quality of life in patients with dementia. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2004) 2:52. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-2-52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Liu K, Wall S, Wissoker D. Disability and medicare costs of elderly persons. Milbank Q. (1997) 75:461–93. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. (1969) 3:179–86. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Katz S, Ford AB, Moskowitz RW, Jackson BA, Jaffe MW. Studies of illness in the aged: the index of ADL: a standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA. (1963) 185:914–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Spector WD, Katz S, Murphy JB, Fulton JP. The hierarchical relationship between activities of daily living and instrumental activities of daily living. J Chronic Dis. (1987) 40:481–9. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(87)90004-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Dunlop DD, Hughes SL, Manheim LM. Disability in activities of daily living: patterns of change and a hierarchy of disability. Am J Public Health. (1997) 87:378–83. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.87.3.378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Jagger C, Arthur AJ, Spiers NA, Clarke M. Patterns of onset of disability in activities of daily living with age. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2001) 49:404–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49083.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bendayan R, Cooper R, Wloch EG, Hofer SM, Piccinin AM, Muniz-Terrera G. Hierarchy and speed of loss in physical functioning: a comparison across older U.S. and English men and women. J Gerontol Ser A. (2017) 72:1117–22. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rudberg MA, Parzen MI, Leonard LA, Cassel CK. Functional limitation pathways and transitions in community-dwelling older persons. Gerontologist. (1996) 36:430–40. doi: 10.1093/geront/36.4.430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gill TM, Robison JT, Tinetti ME. Predictors of recovery in activities of daily living among disabled older persons living in the community. J Gen Intern Med. (1997) 12:757–62. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.1997.07161.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Seidel D, Crilly N, Matthews FE, Jagger C, Clarkson PJ, Brayne C. Patterns of functional loss among older people: a prospective analysis. Hum Factors. (2009) 51:669–80. doi: 10.1177/0018720809353597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Stineman MG, Streim JE, Pan Q, Kurichi JE, Schüssler-Fiorenza Rose SM, Xie D. Activity limitation stages empirically derived for activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental ADL in the U.S. adult community-dwelling medicare population. PM&R. (2014) 6:976–87. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2014.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Mueller-Schotte S, Zuithoff NPA, van der Schouw YT, Schuurmans MJ, Bleijenberg N. Trajectories of limitations in instrumental activities of daily living in frail older adults with vision, hearing, or dual sensory loss. J Gerontol Ser A. (2019) 74:936–42. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fujiwara Y, Yoshida H, Amano H, Fukaya T, Liang J, Uchida H, et al. Predictors of improvement or decline in instrumental activities of daily living among community-dwelling older Japanese. Gerontology. (2008) 54:373–80. doi: 10.1159/000151221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Dodge HH, Du Y, Saxton JA, Ganguli M. Cognitive domains and trajectories of functional independence in nondemented elderly persons. J Gerontol Ser A. (2006) 61:1330–7. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.12.1330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Cecchi F, Marchionni N, Salani B, Kasper J, et al. Constant hierarchic patterns of physical functioning across seven populations in five countries. Gerontologist. (1998) 38:286–94. doi: 10.1093/geront/38.3.286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Njegovan V, Man-Son-Hing M, Mitchell SL, Molnar FJ. The hierarchy of functional loss associated with cognitive decline in older persons. J Gerontol Ser A. (2001) 56:638–43. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.10.M638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Barberger-Gateau P, Rainville C, Letenneur L, Dartigues J-F. A hierarchical model of domains of disablement in the elderly: a longitudinal approach. Disabil Rehabil. (2000) 22:308–17. doi: 10.1080/096382800296665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Edjolo A, Proust-Lima C, Delva F, Dartigues J-F, Pérès K. Natural history of dependency in the elderly: a 24-year population-based study using a longitudinal item response theory model. Am J Epidemiol. (2016) 183:277–85. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwv223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wolinsky FD, Armbrecht ES, Wyrwich KW. Rethinking functional limitation pathways. Gerontologist. (2000) 40:137–46. doi: 10.1093/geront/40.2.137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Fong TG, Gleason LJ, Wong B, Habtemariam D, Jones RN, Schmitt EM, et al. Cognitive and physical demands of activities of daily living in older adults: validation of expert panel ratings. PM&R. (2015) 7:727–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2015.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ng T-P, Niti M, Chiam P-C, Kua E-H. Physical and cognitive domains of the instrumental activities of daily living: validation in a multiethnic population of Asian older adults. J Gerontol Ser A. (2006) 61:726–35. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.7.726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gross AL, Rebok GW, Unverzagt FW, Willis SL, Brandt J. Cognitive predictors of everyday functioning in older adults: results from the ACTIVE cognitive intervention trial. J Gerontol Ser B. (2011) 66:557–66. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Tennstedt SL, Unverzagt FW. The ACTIVE study: study overview and major findings. J Aging Health. (2013) 25:3–20. doi: 10.1177/0898264313518133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. (1992) 30:473–83. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dean DO, Bauer DJ, Shanahan MJ. A discrete-time multiple event process survival mixture (MEPSUM) model. Psychol Methods. (2014) 19:251–64. doi: 10.1037/a0034281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lamarca R, Alonso J, Gomez G, Munoz A. Left-truncated data with age as time scale: an alternative for survival analysis in the elderly population. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (1998) 53:337–43. doi: 10.1093/gerona/53A.5.M337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Singer JD, Willett JB. It's about time: using discrete-time survival analysis to study duration and the timing of events. J Educ Stat. (1993) 18:155–95. doi: 10.3102/10769986018002155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. McNemar Q. Note on the sampling error of the difference between correlated proportions or percentages. Psychometrika. (1947) 12:153–7. doi: 10.1007/BF02295996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Muth C, Blom JW, Smith SM, Johnell K, Gonzalez-Gonzalez AI, Nguyen TS, et al. Evidence supporting the best clinical management of patients with multimorbidity and polypharmacy: a systematic guideline review and expert consensus. J Intern Med. (2019) 285:272–88. doi: 10.1111/joim.12842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Benjamin RM. Medication adherence: helping patients take their medicines as directed. Public Health Rep. (2012) 127:2–3. doi: 10.1177/003335491212700102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Sumida CA, Vo TT, Van Etten EJ, Schmitter-Edgecombe M. Medication management performance and associated cognitive correlates in healthy older adults and older adults with aMCI. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2019) 34:290–300. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acy038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. McWilliams JM, Afendulis CC, McGuire TG, Landon BE. Complex medicare advantage choices may overwhelm seniors—especially those with impaired decision making. Health Aff. (2011) 30:1786–94. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Sheehan C, Domingue BW, Crimmins E. Cohort trends in the gender distribution of household tasks in the United States and the implications for understanding disability. J Aging Health. (2019) 31:1748–69. doi: 10.1177/0898264318793469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tomaszewski Farias S, Giovannetti T, Payne BR, Marsiske M, Rebok GW, Warner Schaie K, et al. Self-perceived difficulties in everyday function precede cognitive decline among older adults in the ACTIVE study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2018) 24:104–12. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Lau KM, Parikh M, Harvey DJ, Huang C-J, Farias ST. Early cognitively based functional limitations predict loss of independence in instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2015) 21:688–98. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cahn-Weiner DA, Farias ST, Julian L, Harvey DJ, Kramer JH, Reed BR, et al. Cognitive and neuroimaging predictors of instrumental activities of daily living. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2007) 13:747–57. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Jefferson A, Paul R, Ozonoff A, Cohen R. Evaluating elements of executive functioning as predictors of instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2006) 21:311–20. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cahn-Weiner DA, Boyle PA, Malloy PF. Tests of executive function predict instrumental activities of daily living in community-dwelling older individuals. Appl Neuropsychol. (2002) 9:187–91. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0903_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Carlson MC, Fried LP, Xue Q-L, Bandeen-Roche K, Zeger SL, Brandt J. Association between executive attention and physical functional performance in community-dwelling older women. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (1999) 54:262–70. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54B.5.S262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Aretouli E, Brandt J. Everyday functioning in mild cognitive impairment and its relationship with executive cognition. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2010) 25:224–33. doi: 10.1002/gps.2325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bell-McGinty S, Podell K, Franzen M, Baird AD, Williams MJ. Standard measures of executive function in predicting instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2002) 17:828–34. doi: 10.1002/gps.646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Jekel K, Damian M, Wattmo C, Hausner L, Bullock R, Connelly PJ, et al. Mild cognitive impairment and deficits in instrumental activities of daily living: a systematic review. Alzheimers Res Ther. (2015) 7:17. doi: 10.1186/s13195-015-0099-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bangen KJ, Jak AJ, Schiehser DM, Delano-Wood L, Tuminello E, Han SD, et al. Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2010) 16:630–9. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Gold DA. An examination of instrumental activities of daily living assessment in older adults and mild cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2012) 34:11–34. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.614598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball K, Elias J, Koepke KM, et al. Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. J Am Med Assoc. (2006) 296:2805–14. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.23.2805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Reuben DB, Valle LA, Hays RD, Siu AL. Measuring physical function in community-dwelling older persons: a comparison of self-administered, interviewer-administered, and performance-based measures. J Am Geriatr Soc. (1995) 43:17–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1995.tb06236.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: IADLs, older adults, activites of daily living, MEPSUM, cognitive training

Citation: Feger DM, Willis SL, Thomas KR, Marsiske M, Rebok GW, Felix C and Gross AL (2020) Incident Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Difficulty in Older Adults: Which Comes First? Findings From the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study. Front. Neurol. 11:550577. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.550577

Received: 09 April 2020; Accepted: 09 September 2020;
Published: 22 October 2020.

Edited by:

Benjamin M. Hampstead, University of Michigan, United States

Reviewed by:

Ota Gál, Charles University, Czechia
Ning Cao, Moss Rehabilitation Hospital, United States

Copyright © 2020 Feger, Willis, Thomas, Marsiske, Rebok, Felix and Gross. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Danielle M. Feger, dfeger1@jhmi.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.