Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 02 November 2021
Sec. Movement Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in Invasive and non-Invasive Brain Stimulation in Parkinson’s Disease: From Basic Science to New Technologies View all 15 articles

Frequency-Specific Effects of Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation on Response-Time Performance in Parkinson's Disease

  • 1Pacific Parkinson's Research Centre, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 2Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB), Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
  • 4Department of Software Convergence, Kyung Hee University, Yongin, South Korea
  • 5Faculty of Medicine (Neurology), University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Background: Galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) is being increasingly explored as a non-invasive brain stimulation technique to treat symptoms in Parkinson's disease (PD). To date, behavioral GVS effects in PD have been explored with only two stimulus types, direct current and random noise (RN). The interaction between GVS effects and anti-parkinsonian medication is unknown. In the present study, we designed multisine (ms) stimuli and investigated the effects of ms and RN GVS on motor response time. In comparison to the RN stimulus, the ms stimuli contained sinusoidal components only at a set of desired frequencies and the phases were optimized to improve participants' comfort. We hypothesized GVS motor effects were a function of stimulation frequency, and specifically, that band-limited ms-GVS would result in better motor performance than conventionally used broadband RN-GVS.

Materials and Methods: Eighteen PD patients (PDMOFF/PDMON: off-/on-levodopa medication) and 20 healthy controls (HC) performed a simple reaction time task while receiving sub-threshold GVS. Each participant underwent nine stimulation conditions: off-stimulation, RN (4–200 Hz), ms-θ (4–8 Hz), ms-α (8–13 Hz), ms-β (13–30 Hz), ms-γ (30–50 Hz), ms-h1 (50–100 Hz), ms-h2 (100–150 Hz), and ms-h3 (150–200 Hz).

Results: The ms-γ resulted in shorter response time (RPT) in both PDMOFF and HC groups compared with the RN. In addition, the RPT of the PDMOFF group decreased during the ms-β while the RPT of the HC group decreased during the ms-α, ms-h1, ms-h2, and ms-h3. There was considerable inter-subject variability in the optimum stimulus type, although the frequency range tended to fall within 8–100 Hz. Levodopa medication significantly reduced the baseline RPT of the PD patients. In contrast to the off-medication state, GVS did not significantly change RPT of the PD patients in the on-medication state.

Conclusions: Using band-limited ms-GVS, we demonstrated that the GVS frequency for the best RPT varied considerably across participants and was >30 Hz for half of the PDMOFF patients. Moreover, dopaminergic medication was found to influence GVS effects in PD patients. Our results indicate the common “one-size-fits-all” RN approach is suboptimal for PD, and therefore personalized stimuli aiming to address this variability is warranted to improve GVS effects.

Introduction

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a progressive disorder marked by the degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra projecting to the basal ganglia (BG). As these neurons degenerate, individuals with PD frequently experience the cardinal motor symptoms of slowness of movement, tremor, rigidity, and postural instability. The estimated prevalence and incidence are expected to grow as a result of aging populations (1).

Dopamine-based pharmacologic treatments such as levodopa remain the gold standard for symptomatic treatment of PD (2) and are robust and effective in improving motor function, particularly in the early stages of the disease. However, some symptoms such as gait and balance dysfunction may be poorly responsive to dopaminergic medication (3), and many people who have been treated with levodopa for prolonged periods may experience complications such as dyskinesias and motor fluctuations (2). Deep brain stimulation (DBS) is an effective treatment for advanced PD (4) but utilized in as few as 2% of the PD population (5) for reasons including the invasiveness of surgical intervention and associated potential complications (6), medical comorbidities that prevent surgery, lack of advanced medical care, relatively mild symptoms, and good response to medication. The exact mechanisms underlying DBS effects are not yet fully understood, but likely involve suppression of pathological neural oscillations [e.g., exaggerated beta oscillations (7, 8)] in the BG circuit (9).

Inspired by the success of DBS in alleviating PD symptoms, non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) is being increasingly explored. As with DBS, NIBS techniques can apply electric currents to the brain to modulate neural activity (10, 11) and affect downstream behaviors (12). NIBS can be safely and economically tested within a wide range of the PD population, from early to advanced disease stages. Although NIBS lacks the ability to directly target focal areas for maximum effectiveness of the stimulation compared with DBS, it does not rely on implantable hardware. Hardware that must be implanted has severe constraints on design as it must be small in size, strongly conserve battery power, and have strict temperature regulation. In contrast, NIBS is not affected by these limitations to the same degree and can utilize external (and potentially portable) stimulators. Thus, NIBS techniques can employ more complicated stimulus waveforms such as random noise (RN) and multisine signals that can be delivered to achieve different effects, as we show here, as compared to electrical pulses used in DBS.

Galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) is one type of NIBS technique that applies weak electric currents to the mastoid processes behind the ears to modulate the firing rates of the vestibular afferents. In human studies, GVS has been utilized primarily as a means to activate the vestibular system in order to study balance and head movement responses (13). A pioneering study to investigate GVS effects on PD patients was conducted in 2005 (14) by applying 24-h continuous noisy GVS to six idiopathic PD patients and one patient with akinesia. The stimulation improved short-range heart rate variability, speed of transitions between rest and activity in the trunk, and reaction time in a Go/NoGo task.

Since then, GVS is being increasingly investigated for the treatment of PD symptoms, motivated by anatomical and functional evidence supporting close connections between the vestibular nuclei, thalamus, and BG (1519). Prior GVS studies in PD have reported improvement in autonomic system regulation, postural balance and gait, and motor task performance (Supplementary Table 1). Notably, six out of the nine (66.7%) GVS studies have used RN stimuli while the other studies (33.3%) used direct current (DC) stimuli. The predominance of DC stimuli is likely because it has been long-used in balance research to induce body sways using GVS (20). Similarly, a RN stimulus has been adopted as it was used in the original GVS study in PD (14) and has been supported by the stochastic resonance theory stating that the addition of an appropriate level of random noise can paradoxically enhance the response of the nervous system to a weak signal (2123). Notably, the GVS frequencies used in these studies have been limited to <30 Hz as this reflects the frequency range of most physical movements, and therefore likely reflects the physiological range of endogenous vestibular activation (24). However, we do not know if RN is the most effective stimulus and if different stimulus frequencies significantly influence the motor effects.

Here, we assessed the motor performance of PD participants in a simple reaction time task using several band-limited multisine GVS stimuli. Specifically, we compared whether the multisine stimuli can result in better task performance compared to the more traditional RN stimulus. We next sought to answer the following questions: (1) is there a single band-limited stimulus that brings about the most robust and largest effects across individuals?; (2) does the most effective band-limited stimulus vary across individuals?; and (3) how much improvement in motor performance can be evoked by varying stimulation frequency within an individual? Increasing evidence demonstrates that the same transcranial electrical stimulation can induce substantial variability in individual responses (2528) due to various factors including methodological differences in the study protocols and participants' physiological traits (e.g., age and sex) and brain states (e.g., emotional and mental states) (28, 29). Here, we posit that a data-driven approach—whereby individual responses to different stimuli are assessed—may be a strategy to partly ameliorate these innate differences. Finally, for the first time, we aimed to address the question of whether GVS effects interact with levodopa medication by recruiting the same PD participants both off-/on-dopaminergic medication.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total number of 20 PD patients and 22 age-matched healthy controls (HC) took part in this study. The study protocol was approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia. All participants gave written, informed consent prior to participation. No participant had any reported vestibular or auditory disorder, and all were right-handed. The PD patients were classified as having mild-stage PD (Hoehn and Yahr stage 1–2) without atypical Parkinsonism or other neurological disorders. Two PD participants and one HC participant did not complete the entire study protocol (see 2.2 Study protocol) due to extraneous reasons such as occasional coughing and arriving late for the experiment. As the motor task data collected from these subjects were ultimately incomplete, we excluded them from the data analysis. One HC participant was also excluded from the data analysis because the subject did not hold a pressure-sensor bulb as instructed and data were not usable when we subsequently inspected the data. Notably, no subjects failed to complete the entire study protocol due to the intolerability of the GVS.

In total, 18 PD and 20 HC participants were included in the data analysis (Table 1). The Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) Parts II and III were assessed for the PD patients in the off-medication state prior to the experiment.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants.

Study Protocol

In this present paper, we analyzed simple reaction time (SRT) task data collected as a part of a concurrent GVS-EEG study designed to investigate the effects of different GVS frequencies on: (1) cortical activity at rest; and (2) cortical activity and motor performance during the SRT task. In this section, we report the overall experimental procedure of the concurrent GVS-EEG study. The details of the SRT task are described in the next section.

The experiment consisted of 9 blocks with different GVS conditions that were 2 min apart to minimize any confounding post-stimulation effects. Each block included a 60-s rest condition, followed by the SRT task (Figure 1A). Prior to the experiment, each participant's cutaneous threshold to GVS was measured (see section GVS). Then, the participants were fitted with an EEG cap. They were instructed to comfortably sit in front of a computer screen and focus their gaze on a continuously displayed fixed target for 60 s until they saw a written instruction to press a key on the keyboard to start the motor task. Further instruction on how to perform the SRT task was given, followed by a practice run consisting of 10 trials, and then the experiment began.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic diagram of study protocol and simple reaction time task. (A) There were 9 stimulation conditions (blocks) and the order of GVS blocks was randomized for each participant. In each block, participants were instructed to stare at a fixation cross on a computer screen for 60 s (Rest) and perform a simple reaction time task afterward. There was a 120-s break between the blocks to avoid potential carry-over effects (i.e., GVS was not delivered during break). (B) Left: participants were instructed to respond to a visual cue (“Go”) as fast as possible by squeezing a pressure sensor bulb. There were 10 trials in each stimulation condition. For each trial, peak grip pressure (Pmax), time of visual cue (t1), time of movement onset (t2), and time of peak pressure (t3) were identified in the water pressure recording.

The PD participants performed the experiment in two sessions on the same day, in the off-medication (PDMOFF) and on-medication (PDMON) states. They stopped taking their normal levodopa medication and any dopamine agonists at least 12 and 18 h prior to the experiment, respectively. After the first session, they took their regular dose of medication and rested for an hour before beginning the second session. The HC participants performed the experiment once. At the end of the experiment, all the participants were verbally asked whether they felt any particular sensation or experienced pain, vertigo, nausea, or heat sensation at the stimulating electrodes in order to confirm the absence of placebo and adverse effects (13, 31).

Simple Reaction Time Task

Participants were instructed to respond to a visual cue as fast as possible by squeezing a pressure-sensor bulb (Figure 1B). Each trial started with a hold phase in which a fixation cross was presented at the center for a randomized duration that ranged from 1,000 ms to 2,000 ms [N(1500, 500)]. Then, a visual cue (“Go”) appeared for 500 ms followed by a 1,000-ms white blank screen. The motor task in each stimulation block with the same stimulus consisted of 10 trials. A pressure-sensor bulb was used because it provides more descriptive behavior measures than a simple button-press and a prior study reported that PD patients demonstrate abnormal motor control while exerting pressure during a task of repeatedly squeezing a rubber bulb (32). The number of trials was selected such that the PD participants could still complete the entire study protocol without excessive tiredness (particularly during off-medication) while significant differences in task performance between conditions could still be detected.

The pressure was recorded at a sampling frequency of 250 Hz. For each trial, three temporal landmarks (t1, t2, and t3) were defined in the pressure recording (Figure 1B). Response time (RPT) was defined as t3t1, which was divided into two subcomponents: (1) reaction time (RT) (t2t1; the time between the stimulus onset and movement onset); and (2) movement time (MT) (t3t2; the time required to execute the motor response), in order to further investigate whether GVS affects both RT and MT or only one of them exclusively. Mean RPT, RT, and MT across 10 trials in each block were computed for further statistical analyses.

GVS

GVS was delivered in bilateral, bipolar fashion through pre-gelled Ag/AgCl electrodes (BIOPAC Systems Inc., USA) placed over the mastoid process behind each ear using a constant current stimulator DS5 (Digitimer, UK). We utilized systematic procedures previously used to determine individual cutaneous sensory threshold level (31, 33, 34). A noisy stimulus was delivered to the mastoid processes for 20 s at an imperceptible level, starting from a basal current level of 0.1 mA. The current intensity was then increased in 0.02 mA intervals until participants perceived a mild, local tingling sensation in the area of the stimulating electrodes for the duration of the stimulus. The current level was then decreased each time by one level until sensation was no longer reported and then increased by one step to confirm the threshold. In the experiment, GVS was applied at 90% of the determined threshold value to avoid the effects of placebo, general arousal, and/or voluntary selective attention.

In each stimulation block (Figure 1A), either random noise (RN; 4–200 Hz) or a band-limited multisine stimulus was delivered. A multisine stimulus was adopted as it has the advantages of reducing experiment time by testing multiple sinusoids at once and preserves the power spectrum over a frequency range of interest without any spectral leakage compared with random noise (35). A multisine signal x(t) can be expressed as:

x(t)=Ak=1Nksin(ωkt+ϕk)

where A is the amplitude, Nk is the number of sinusoidal components, ωk and ϕk are the frequency and phase of each sinusoidal component k, respectively.

Seven multisine stimuli were designed in total. ms-θ, ms-α, ms-β, and ms-γ were designed to cover conventional EEG frequency bands, and ms-h1, ms-h2, and ms-h3 to cover high frequency ranges (Table 2). For each stimulus, the sinusoidal frequencies (ωk) were uniformly distributed every 0.4 Hz (e.g., the ms-β consisted of sinusoids at 13.0, 13.4, …, 29.8 Hz). The phases (ϕk) of the sinusoids were chosen to minimize the crest factor using a clipping algorithm (36) (Figure 2) to increase signal-to-noise ratios and improve participants' comfort (37, 38).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Frequency ranges of the GVS stimuli investigated in the study.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Clipping algorithm to minimize crest factor (CF) of a multisine signal.

The active-GVS blocks were randomly ordered for each participant, and the off-stimulation block was conducted before any active-GVS block to enable a comparison of motor performance between the PD and HC groups without any carry-over stimulation effects. In this study, behavioral effects of GVS were investigated using only the active-GVS blocks that were completely randomized.

Statistical Procedures

GVS thresholds between groups were compared using the two-sample t-test, and correlations between GVS thresholds and age or clinical scores were tested using the Pearson correlation coefficient.

For each group, the RPTs during active-GVS blocks were compared using a one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) with STIM (RN, ms-θ, ms-α, ms-β, ms-γ, ms-h1, ms-h2, and ms-h3) as a within-subject factor. To investigate the interaction effect of GVS and medication for PD participants, we additionally conducted an overall two-way RM-ANOVA with STIM and MED (on and off) as within-subject factors. Mauchly's test was used to assess the ANOVA assumption of sphericity, and the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used if necessary to correct for non-sphericity. When a significant effect was found, post-hoc pairwise comparisons with the Bonferroni correction were conducted. If there was a stimulus that evoked a significantly different RPT compared with RN-GVS, the RT and MT were compared using a paired t-test.

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (version 27). Significance was assigned to P < 0.05.

Results

None of the participants reported any adverse effects nor awareness of any differences between stimulation conditions.

GVS Threshold

There was no significant difference between the PD and HC groups in GVS threshold level [PD: 0.50 ± 0.24 mA; HC: 0.46 ± 0.18 mA; t(36) = 0.56, P = 0.58]. The thresholds of all participants were not significantly correlated with age (r = 0.06, P = 0.73), but there was a significant sex difference [males = 0.51 ± 0.21 mA; females = 0.35 ± 0.13 mA; t(36) = 2.89, P = 0.006]. For PD participants, no significant correlations were found between their thresholds and clinical scores (disease duration: r = −0.11, P = 0.69; UPDRS III: r = 0.22, P = 0.39; bradykinesia: r = 0.35, P = 0.16; tremor: r = −0.19, P = 0.44).

Effects of GVS Frequencies on RPT

A one-way RM-ANOVA revealed a main effect of STIM for the PDMOFF [F(7, 119) = 4.38, P < 0.001] and HC [F(7, 133) = 5.97, P < 0.001] groups, but not for the PDMON group [F(7, 119) = 0.356, P = 0.93] (Figure 3A). For the PDMOFF group, post-hoc tests found that RPT was significantly shorter during ms-β (P = 0.008) and ms-γ (P = 0.026) compared to RPT during RN, and the % change of RPT was −5.5 ± 4.8 and −5.4 ± 5.4%, respectively (Figure 3B). No significant change in RPT was observed for ms-θ (P = 0.51), ms-α (P = 0.11), ms-h1 (P = 0.093), ms-h2 (P = 0.056), and ms-h3 (P = 1.0). For the HC group, post-hoc tests revealed that, compared to RN, the RPT significantly decreased during ms-α (P = 0.01), ms-γ (P = 0.001), ms-h1 (P = 0.012), ms-h2 (P = 0.008), and ms-h3 (P = 0.013), but not during ms-θ (P = 1.0), and ms-β (P = 0.24) (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Response time (RPT) measured in different GVS conditions. Markers and vertical lines represent the group mean and standard error of the mean (SEM), respectively. P values from the post-hoc tests are indicated (*P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01 compared to RN). (B) % change in the RPT compared to RN-GVS. Each dot represents a participant.

We further investigated the nature of the significant RPT changes (Table 3). For the PDMOFF group, ms-β significantly decreased RT (P = 0.0095) but not MT (P = 0.17) whereas ms-γ decreased MT (P = 0.013) but not RT (P = 0.076). For the HC group, RT was decreased by ms-h1 (P = 0.013) and ms-h2 (P = 0.048). Overall, the results did not indicate that variation of GVS frequency evokes an exclusive change in RT or MT.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Post-hoc comparisons of RT and MT measured during GVS.

The two-way RM-ANOVA revealed a main effect of STIM [F(7, 119) = 2.78, P = 0.01] and MED [F(1, 17) = 5.59, P = 0.03]. Although PD participants tended to benefit from multisine stimuli more during off-medication state (Figure 3B), the STIM × MED interaction effect did not reach the statistical significance [F(7, 119) = 2.03, P = 0.056].

Effects of Time Order on RPT

To assess whether there was any spurious time-order effect on RPT, we re-arranged RPTs in chronological order for every participant and performed a one-way RM-ANOVA with TIME as a within-subject factor.

No effect of TIME was found for all three groups [PDMOFF: F(7, 119) = 1.31, P = 0.253; PDMON: F(3.8, 64.8) = 1.38, P = 0.251; HC: F(7, 133) = 1.14, P = 0.341] (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group mean and SEM of the RPTs that are temporally ordered as opposed to stimulus types. The x-axis represents the temporal order of the stimulation blocks. The first off-stimulation block (Figure 1A) is not shown.

Sensitivity of RPT on GVS Frequency

We investigated whether the degree of RPT variation induced by different stimuli differed between the PDMOFF, PDMON, and HC groups. To quantify this, for each participant, we computed the standard deviation (RPTSD) and range (RPTrange; max–min) of RPTs across the eight stimulation blocks.

Group comparisons suggested that both RPTSD and RPTrange were comparable between the PDMOFF and HC groups (Table 4). PDMON participants showed relatively smaller RPTSD and RPTrange compared to the other two groups, but these differences did not reach statistical significance.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparisons of RPT variability (RPTSD and RPTrange) across the eight GVS conditions.

Intersubject Variability in Most and Least Effective Stimuli

Figure 5A shows the distributions of the most effective stimulus (GVSmost) that resulted in the shortest RPT for each participant. Interestingly, the distributions of the PDMOFF and HC groups appeared similar in that 77.8 and 90.0% of the participants, respectively, showed their best task performance during ms-α, ms-β, ms-γ, or ms-h1. By comparison, only 38.9% of the PDMON participants performed the best in these frequency ranges.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Distributions of stimulus types during which RPT was the shortest (GVSmost; top) and the longest (GVSleast; bottom) for the participants in each group.

The contrast between the PDMON and the other two groups was also observed when we investigated the least effective stimulus (GVSleast) that resulted in the longest RPT (Figure 5B). RN and ms-θ were found to be the least effective stimuli for 55.5 and 90.0% of the PDMOFF and HC participants, respectively. On the other hand, only 16.7% of the PDMON participants showed their worst performance during RN and ms-θ.

Significance of the RPT Decrease by GVSmost

To assess whether the RPT evoked by GVSmost was significantly faster compared with the other stimuli, we computed its P value based on the empirical distribution of RPT estimated by a bootstrapping approach (Figure 6A). Note that as the RPT is computed as the mean over 10 randomly selected trials, it can still be shorter than the mean RPT during GVSmost. Figure 6B shows that 83.3, 66.7, and 85% of the PDMOFF, PDMON, and HC participants, respectively, exhibited significantly shorter RPT during GVSmost (P < 0.05) compared with the expected RPT during any GVS stimulus.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. (A) Schematic diagram showing a process to generate an empirical distribution of RPT for each participant. Each bootstrap sample randomly selects RPTs of 10 trials and the mean of the bootstrap samples is computed to generate the distribution. RPTbest represents the mean RPT during GVSmost. (B) P value of RPTbest computed based on (A) is shown for every participant.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study investigating SRT task performance of PD and HC participants while applying GVS across a wide range of frequencies. Overall, our results suggest that RPT can be improved by GVS in PD patients. However, as bradykinesia is a key feature in PD, we cannot disentangle whether or not the RPT improvements were a result of faster decision-making and/or faster movement. This will require examining the simultaneously acquired EEG and will be the topic of another report. We demonstrated that the motor improvement is significantly dependent on the GVS parameters used. Surprisingly, we found that RN-GVS, despite its popularity, did not actually evoke the best task performance in the PDMOFF and HC groups, with ms-γ (30–50 Hz) proving superior in reducing RPT (Table 5). The performance of the off-medicated PD participants during ms-β and ms-γ were comparable to the baseline performance when they were in the on-medication state. We found that the GVS frequency that resulted in the shortest RPT varied considerably across participants, suggesting that a one-size-fits-all stimulus will not be as effective as a personalized stimulus. For most of the PDMOFF and HC participants, the best GVS frequency varied in the range of 8–100 Hz. The worst task performance was found during RN or ms-θ for more than half of the participants in these two groups. These results provide evidence that further work is required to tailor GVS parameters for maximum efficacy.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Summary of RPTs (unit: millisecond) measured during off-stimulation (baseline), RN-GVS, and multisine GVS for the PD and HC participants.

Whether or not RPT is actually delayed in PD has been controversial (3941), partially due to methodological heterogeneity and different clinical characteristics of the participants (42). In this study, the difference in baseline RPT between the PDMOFF and HC groups did not reach statistical significance (P = 0.068; Supplementary Table 2). Instead, the most interesting finding was that responses to different GVS stimuli showed a similar trend between the two groups (Figure 3). This finding may suggest that there are some mechanisms underlying the GVS effects that are common between these groups.

In contrast, we found that the PDMON group showed relatively different responses to GVS. Normally, dopamine is active both phasically and tonically during motor performance. Levodopa has complex effects in PD, which may result in relative normalization of tonic dopamine firing, yet impairment of phasic firing (43). While phasic dopamine firing is normally associated with rewards in reinforcement learning paradigms, it may also be involved in internal representations of desired actions with actual sensory feedback during motor performance (4446). Thus, many studies have suggested that movement-related phasic changes can be observed in nigrostriatal dopamine neuron firing (4749), and that dorsal striatal phasic dopamine signaling is associated with specific kinematic features of movement (43). Complex effects of dopaminergic medication in PD have also been reported in fMRI studies (5054) showing that levodopa medication does not simply restore brain connectivity aberrant in PD. Rather it induces functional connectivity changes distinctive from those identified to be different between PD patients and healthy controls (51, 54). Taken together, this is an important point to consider for future GVS studies, as prior studies (Supplementary Table 1) included only medicated PD patients, and the information on the dosage and timing of the medication was rarely reported.

Given the functional role of pathological beta oscillations in PD (55), the result of particular interest was that ms-β resulted in the largest decrease in RT among the tested stimuli in the PDMOFF group whereas it did not improve motor performance for the HC group. In this regard, there is some evidence to support the concept that beta-frequency stimulation may have clinical effectiveness in PD patients. In a transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) study conducted on 10 PD and 10 HC participants (56), 20-Hz stimulation at the primary motor cortex (M1) yielded a significant decrease in beta-band cortico-muscular coupling in PD patients but not in HC. A TMS study showed that 20–33 Hz stimulation at M1 elicited significant suppression of the motor evoked potential (MEP) in PD patients and the amount of suppression was correlated with their UPDRS III scores (57). It should be noted, however, that there have been only a handful of studies that utilized beta-frequency NIBS in PD patients, and it is difficult to determine from our results whether the ms-β effects observed in the PD participants were related to the pathological beta-band activity. Thus, further neuroimaging studies are strongly suggested to be carried out to validate our results and elucidate the mechanisms of action.

The frequency-dependent GVS effects we observed may be related to the overlap between the neural processes affecting RPT and neural pathways affected by external vestibular inputs. One of the main vestibular pathways is the direct ascending projection from the vestibular complex to the thalamus, primarily targeting the ventral anterior, ventral lateral, ventral posterior lateral, ventral posterior medial, intralaminar nuclei, and the geniculate bodies (5860). Strong activations in these regions by vestibular stimulation (18, 59, 6163) suggest a critical thalamic contribution to processing vestibular information (60, 63). The ventral parts of the thalamus are also closely connected with M1, premotor cortex, and BG (15, 58, 63, 64), modulating a range of aspects in motor control (15, 63, 65). Thus, we conjecture that GVS effects on RPT can be in part explained by vestibular inputs affecting the motor thalamus. This may also explain the mild GVS effects on the PDMON participants as the BG inputs to the motor thalamus would vary at different dopamine levels.

It is also possible that GVS affected the striatum, a region described as an integrative center for sensory information and involved in motor planning and execution. Although the largest inputs to the striatum are from the cortex, recent studies have elucidated the subcortical pathways critical for interpreting and responding to environmental stimuli (66, 67). Electrophysiological studies in animal models and neuroimaging studies in humans have shown that vestibular stimulation activates the head of the caudate nucleus and putamen (1619, 62, 68, 69), likely through the parafascicular thalamic nucleus (PFN) (64, 70). In addition, it has been recently proposed that the striatal tail may play a role as a multisensory integration center (71), and thus it is possible that there are vestibular inputs to this region as well.

Our observations of different motor effects evoked by varying GVS frequency are consistent with many animal studies (72, 73). Surprisingly, canal and otolith afferents in macaque monkeys responded to GVS as a function of frequency such that the response gain (i.e., spikes/s/mA) increased more than twice when the stimulation frequency varied from 0.1 to 25 Hz (72). This seminal finding opposes the common idea that high-frequency GVS would result in smaller gains because the tissues between the electrode and vestibular afferents may act as a low-pass filter. Similarly, the firing rate of the PFN increases when the frequency of stimulation applied to the semicircular canal nerve is >100 Hz (73). Taken together, these findings could explain in part the efficacy of the frequency range we observed in most of the PDMOFF and HC participants.

There are several limitations in our study. Considering the study design and our primary objective to examine different types of GVS stimuli, we did not try to replicate previous findings demonstrating that GVS results in better motor performance compared to baseline performance seen during off-stimulation. Although we think the practice effect on the task performance is unlikely for a simple, over-learned motor task like ours, the possibility was not completely ruled out when the baseline measurement always preceded active-GVS. Similarly, our study was not designed to measure the after-effects of GVS. Post-stimulation behavior effects of GVS are largely unknown (13). Studies that examined GVS aftereffects stimulated participants for more than 30 min, and the results are conflicting (7477). The issue of whether stimulation effects last after the cease of stimulation is not only limited to GVS but is one of the main controversial topics for transcranial electrical stimulation (78). As online stimulation effects differ depending on stimulation parameters (e.g., frequency, intensity, duration, target sites) and experimental tasks, the presence and duration of after-effects appear to be influenced by the stimulation parameters and tasks (79, 80). Although after-effects are infrequently reported, evidence from tES studies shows the presence of after-effects when stimulation was applied at >0.5 mA for longer than 10 min (78, 8183). Given that we applied GVS for a short duration at a low current intensity with a 2-min inter-block off-stimulation break, we suggest that any effects carried over from previous stimulation were relatively mild compared to the online-stimulation effects. Validation of GVS after-effects and their relationships with stimulation parameters will be areas/topics of interest for future work. Note that, since GVS can utilize portable stimulators, reliance on much more subtle after-effects is not as important as other technologies that are not as easily portable (e.g., TMS). Finally, several studies support the notion that GVS effects are mostly spatially restricted to the vestibular organs. For instance, the auditory effects of GVS are rare (20) despite the proximity between the auditory and vestibular systems. GVS evokes circumscribed cortical activation of vestibular areas, and effects on the somatosensory cortex are only seen at specific frequencies (62). At higher intensities, the stimulation of the vestibular system can be self-reported by feelings of vertigo. A recent computational modeling study of the electric field generated by GVS (84) suggests that the bilateral and bipolar configuration, as used in our study, results in the most spatially-restricted current flow to the vestibular organs. However, some current may diffuse to the medulla, pons, and cerebellum. Although we note that both the electrodes (11 mm) and current intensity used here (0.43 ± 0.19 mA) were less than those used in the computational model (30 mm and 1 mA), we cannot completely discount that some of our results may be via modulation of extra-vestibular structures.

In conclusion, our findings provide key information necessary for the future development of GVS techniques to induce robust and effective therapeutic effects in PD. Future research is warranted to confirm similar behavioral effects of GVS applied at frequencies beyond the assumed physiological ranges and to establish potential mechanisms.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Clinical Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SL designed and conducted the study, including patient recruitment, data collection, and data analysis. MJM participated in the study design, data analysis, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors have participated in manuscript writing and editing and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

SL was supported by Rina M. Bidin Foundation Fellowship in Research of Brain Treatment. WL was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIT) (No. 2021R1C1C1009436). MJM was supported by John Nichol Chair in Parkinson's Research and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) grant (453374).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Christy Jones for assisting in recruiting participants and are enormously grateful to the research participants for their involvement in this project.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2021.758122/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Ray Dorsey E, Elbaz A, Nichols E, Abd-Allah F, Abdelalim A, Adsuar JC, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of Parkinson's disease, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. (2018) 17:939–53. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30295-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Armstrong MJ, Okun MS. Diagnosis and treatment of Parkinson Disease: a review. JAMA. (2020) 323:548–60. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.22360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sethi K. Levodopa unresponsive symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:S521–33. doi: 10.1002/mds.22049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Krack P, Batir A, Van Blercom N, Chabardes S, Fraix V, Ardouin C, et al. Five-year follow-up of bilateral stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in advanced Parkinson's disease. N Engl J Med. (2003) 349:1925–34. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa035275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kestenbaum M, Ford B, Louis ED. Estimating the proportion of essential tremor and Parkinson's disease patients undergoing deep brain stimulation surgery: five-year data from Columbia University Medical Center (2009–2014). Mov Disord Clin Pract. (2015) 2:384–7. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.12185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cyron D. Mental side effects of deep brain stimulation (DBS) for movement disorders: the futility of denial. Front Integr Neurosci. (2016) 10:17. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2016.00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Androulidakis AG, Doyle LMF, Yarrow K, Litvak V, Gilbertson TP, Brown P. Anticipatory changes in beta synchrony in the human corticospinal system and associated improvements in task performance. Eur J Neurosci. (2007) 25:3758–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05620.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Little S, Pogosyan A, Neal S, Zavala B, Zrinzo L, Hariz M, et al. Adaptive deep brain stimulation in advanced Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. (2013) 74:449–57. doi: 10.1002/ana.23951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lozano AM, Lipsman N, Bergman H, Brown P, Chabardes S, Chang JW, et al. Deep brain stimulation: current challenges and future directions. Nat Rev Neurol. (2019) 15:148–60. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0128-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Liu A, Vöröslakos M, Kronberg G, Henin S, Krause MR, Huang Y, et al. Immediate neurophysiological effects of transcranial electrical stimulation. Nat Commun. (2018) 9:5092. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07233-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

11. Yang D, Shin Y, Hong KS. Systemic review on transcranial electrical stimulation parameters and EEG/fNIRS features for brain diseases. Front Neurosci. (2021) 15:629323. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.629323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Krause B, Cohen Kadosh R. Can transcranial electrical stimulation improve learning difficulties in atypical brain development? A future possibility for cognitive training. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2013) 6:176–94. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Lee S, Liu A, McKeown MJ. Current perspectives on galvanic vestibular stimulation in the treatment of Parkinson's disease. Expert Rev Neurother. (2021) 21:405–18. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2021.1894928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Yamamoto Y, Struzik ZR, Soma R, Ohashi K, Kwak S. Noisy vestibular stimulation improves autonomic and motor responsiveness in central neurodegenerative disorders. Ann Neurol. (2005) 58:175–81. doi: 10.1002/ana.20574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bosch-Bouju C, Hyland BI, Parr-Brownlie LC. Motor thalamus integration of cortical, cerebellar and basal ganglia information: implications for normal and parkinsonian conditions. Front Comput Neurosci. (2013) 7:163. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Matsunami K, Cohen B. Afferent modulation of unit activity in globus pallidus and caudate nucleus: changes induced by vestibular nucleus and pyramidal tract stimulation. Brain Res. (1975) 91:140–6. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90473-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bottini G, Sterzi R, Paulesu E, Vallar G, Cappa SF, Erminio F, et al. Frackowiak RS. Identification of the central vestibular projections in man: a positron emission tomography activation study. Exp brain Res. (1994) 99:164–9. doi: 10.1007/BF00241421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bense S, Stephan T, Yousry TA, Brandt T, Dieterich M. Multisensory cortical signal increases and decreases during vestibular galvanic stimulation (fMRI). J Neurophysiol. (2001) 85:886–99. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kim N, Barter JW, Sukharnikova T, Yin HH. Striatal firing rate reflects head movement velocity. Eur J Neurosci. (2014) 40:3481–90. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Dlugaiczyk J, Gensberger KD, Straka H. Galvanic vestibular stimulation: from basic concepts to clinical applications. J Neurophysiol. (2019) 121:2237–55. doi: 10.1152/jn.00035.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. McDonnell MD, Ward LM. The benefits of noise in neural systems: bridging theory and experiment. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:415–26. doi: 10.1038/nrn3061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Mendez-Balbuena I, Manjarrez E, Schulte-Monting J, Huethe F, Tapia JA, Hepp-Reymond M-C, et al. Improved sensorimotor performance via stochastic resonance. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:12612–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0680-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Moss F, Ward LM, Sannita WG. Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin Neurophysiol. (2004) 115:267–81. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Forbes PA, Siegmund GP, Schouten AC, Blouin JS. Task, muscle and frequency dependent vestibular control of posture. Front Integr Neurosci. (2015) 8:94. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Chew T, Ho KA, Loo CK. Inter- and intra-individual variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) at varying current intensities. Brain Stimul. (2015) 8:1130–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Krause B, Kadosh RC. Not all brains are created equal: the relevance of individual differences in responsiveness to transcranial electrical stimulation. Front Syst Neurosci. (2014) 8:25. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fertonani A, Miniussi C. Transcranial electrical stimulation: what we know and do not know about mechanisms. Neurosci. (2017) 23:109–23. doi: 10.1177/1073858416631966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Guerra A, López-Alonso V, Cheeran B, Suppa A. Variability in non-invasive brain stimulation studies: reasons and results. Neurosci Lett. (2020) 719:133330. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.12.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Terranova C, Rizzo V, Cacciola A, Chillemi G, Calamuneri A, Milardi D, et al. Is there a future for non-invasive brain stimulation as a therapeutic tool? Front Neurol. (2019) 10:1146. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.01146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2649–53. doi: 10.1002/mds.23429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Utz KS, Korluss K, Schmidt L, Rosenthal A, Oppenlnder K, Keller I, et al. Minor adverse effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation in persons with stroke and healthy individuals. Brain Inj. (2011) 25:1058–69. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2011.607789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Mazzoni P, Shabbott B, Cortés JC. Motor control abnormalities in Parkinson's disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. (2012) 2:a009282. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a009282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wilkinson D, Nicholls S, Pattenden C, Kilduff P, Milberg W. Galvanic vestibular stimulation speeds visual memory recall. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 189:243–8. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1463-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lee S, Kim DJ, Svenkeson D, Parras G, Oishi MMK, McKeown MJ. Multifaceted effects of noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation on manual tracking behavior in Parkinson's disease. Front Syst Neurosci. (2015) 9:5. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

35. Pintelon R, Schoukens J. System identification: A Frequency Domain Approach. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (2012). doi: 10.1002/9781118287422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Van der Ouderaa E, Schoukens J, Renneboog J. Peak factor minimization using a time-frequency domain swapping algorithm. IEEE Trans Instrum Meas. (1988) 37:145–7. doi: 10.1109/19.2684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Forbes PA, Dakin CJ, Geers AM, Vlaar MP, Happee R, Siegmund GP, et al. Electrical vestibular stimuli to enhance vestibulo-motor output and improve subject comfort. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e84385. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ehtemam F, Forbes PA, Schouten AC, van der Helm FCT, Happee R. Galvanic vestibular stimulation elicits consistent head-neck motion in seated subjects. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. (2012) 59:1978–84 doi: 10.1109/TBME.2012.2195178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Muslimović D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B. Motor procedural learning in Parkinson's disease. Brain. (2007) 130:2887–97. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Yokochi F, Nakamura R, Narabayashi H. Reaction time of patients with Parkinson's disease, with reference to asymmetry of neurological signs. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1985) 48:702–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.48.7.702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kumru H, Summerfield C, Valldeoriola F, Valls-Solé J. Effects of subthalamic nucleus stimulations on characteristics of EMG activity underlying reaction time in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2004) 19:94–100. doi: 10.1002/mds.10638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gauntlett-Gilbert J, Brown VJ. Reaction time deficits and Parkinson's disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1998) 22:865–81. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00014-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Albin RL, Leventhal DK. The missing, the short, and the long: L-Dopa responses and dopamine actions. Ann Neurol. (2017) 82:4–19. doi: 10.1002/ana.24961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Fee MS. Oculomotor learning revisited: a model of reinforcement learning in the basal ganglia incorporating an efference copy of motor actions. Front Neural Circuits. (2012) 6:38. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fee MS. The role of efference copy in striatal learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2014) 25:194–200. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Yin HH. The basal ganglia in action. Neuroscientist. (2017) 23:299–313. doi: 10.1177/1073858416654115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Jin X, Costa RM. Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature. (2010) 466:457–62. doi: 10.1038/nature09263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Dodson PD, Dreyer JK, Jennings KA, Syed ECJ, Wade-Martins R, Cragg SJ, et al. Representation of spontaneous movement by dopaminergic neurons is cell-type selective and disrupted in parkinsonism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2016) 113:E2180–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515941113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Barter JW, Li S, Lu D, Bartholomew RA, Rossi MA, Shoemaker CT, et al. Beyond reward prediction errors: the role of dopamine in movement kinematics. Front Integr Neurosci. (2015) 27:39. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ballarini T, RuŽička F, Bezdicek O, RuŽička E, Roth J, Villringer A, et al. Unraveling connectivity changes due to dopaminergic therapy in chronically treated Parkinson's disease patients. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:14328. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31988-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tahmasian M, Bettray LM, van Eimeren T, Drzezga A, Timmermann L, Eickhoff CR, et al. A systematic review on the applications of resting-state fMRI in Parkinson's disease: does dopamine replacement therapy play a role? Cortex. (2015) 73:80–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kelly C, Zubicaray G, de Martino A Di, Copland DA, Reiss PT, Klein DF, et al. L-dopa modulates functional connectivity in striatal cognitive and motor networks: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:7364–78. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0810-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Agosta F, Caso F, Stankovic I, Inuggi A, Petrovic I, Svetel M, et al. Cortico-striatal-thalamic network functional connectivity in hemiparkinsonism. Neurobiol Aging. (2014) 35:2592–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ng B, Varoquaux G, Poline JP, Thirion B, Greicius MD, Poston KL. Distinct alterations in Parkinson's medication-state and disease-state connectivity. NeuroImage Clin. (2017) 16:575–85. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Little S, Brown P. The functional role of beta oscillations in Parkinson's disease. Park Relat Disord. (2014) 20:S44–8. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(13)70013-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Krause V, Wach C, Südmeyer M, Ferrea S, Schnitzler A, Pollok B. Cortico-muscular coupling and motor performance are modulated by 20 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) in Parkinson's disease. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:928. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hanajima R, Terao Y, Shirota Y, Ohminami S, Tsutsumi R, Shimizu T, et al. Triad-conditioning transcranial magnetic stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Brain Stimul. (2014) 7:74–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Percheron G, François C, Talbi B, Yelnik J, Fénelon G. The primate motor thalamus. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. (1996) 22:93–181. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(96)00003-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Meng H, May PJ, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Vestibular signals in primate thalamus: properties and origins. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:13590–602. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3931-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lopez C, Blanke O. The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans. Brain Res Rev. (2011) 67:119–46. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Bucher SF, Dieterich M, Wiesmann M, Weiss A, Zink R, Yousry TA, et al. Cerebral functional magnetic resonance imaging of vestibular, auditory, and nociceptive areas during galvanic stimulation. Ann Neurol. (1998) 44:120–5. doi: 10.1002/ana.410440118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Stephan T, Deutschländer A, Nolte A, Schneider E, Wiesmann M, Brandt T, et al. Functional MRI of galvanic vestibular stimulation with alternating currents at different frequencies. Neuroimage. (2005) 26:721–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Wijesinghe R, Protti DA, Camp AJ. Vestibular Interactions in the Thalamus. Front Neural Circuits. (2015) 9:79. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Stiles L, Smith PF. The vestibular–basal ganglia connection: balancing motor control. Brain Res. (2015) 1597:180–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Hwang K, Bertolero MA, Liu WB, D'Esposito M. The human thalamus is an integrative hub for functional brain networks. J Neurosci. (2017) 37:5594–607. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0067-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. McHaffie JG, Stanford TR, Stein BE, Coizet V, Redgrave P. Subcortical loops through the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. (2005) 28:401–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Fisher SD, Reynolds JNJ. The intralaminar thalamus-an expressway linking visual stimuli to circuits determining agency and action selection. Front Behav Neurosci. (2014) 8:115. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Stiles L, Reynolds JN, Napper R, Zheng Y, Smith PF. Single neuron activity and c-Fos expression in the rat striatum following electrical stimulation of the peripheral vestibular system. Physiol Rep. (2018) 6:e13791. doi: 10.14814/phy2.13791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Stiles L, Zheng Y, Smith PF. The effects of electrical stimulation of the peripheral vestibular system on neurochemical release in the rat striatum. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0205869. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Lai H, Tsumori T, Shiroyama T, Yokota S, Nakano K, Yasui Y. Morphological evidence for a vestibulo-thalamo-striatal pathway via the parafascicular nucleus in the rat. Brain Res. (2000) 872:208–14. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02457-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Valjent E, Gangarossa G. The tail of the striatum: from anatomy to connectivity and function. Trends Neurosci. (2021) 44:203–14. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kwan A, Forbes PA, Mitchell DE, Blouin J-S, Cullen KE. Neural substrates, dynamics and thresholds of galvanic vestibular stimulation in the behaving primate. Nat Commun. (2019) 10:1904. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09738-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kim N, Choi MA, Koo H, Park BR, Han SW, Cheong C, et al. Activation of the thalamic parafascicular nucleus by electrical stimulation of the peripheral vestibular nerve in rats. Exp Brain Res. (2017) 235:1617–25. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4864-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Fujimoto C, Yamamoto Y, Kamogashira T, Kinoshita M, Egami N, Uemura Y, et al. Noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation induces a sustained improvement in body balance in elderly adults. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:37575. doi: 10.1038/srep37575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Fujimoto C, Egami N, Kawahara T, Uemura Y, Yamamoto Y, Yamasoba T, et al. Noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation sustainably improves posture in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:900. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Keywan A, Badarna H, Jahn K, Wuehr M. No evidence for after-effects of noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation on motion perception. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:2545. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59374-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Nooristani M, Maheu M, Houde M-S, Bacon B-A, Champoux F. Questioning the lasting effect of galvanic vestibular stimulation on postural control. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0224619. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Pozdniakov I, Vorobiova AN, Galli G, Rossi S, Feurra M. Online and offline effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation of the primary motor cortex. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:3854. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83449-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. (2001) 57:1899–901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. (2000) 527:633–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Strüber D, Rach S, Neuling T, Herrmann CS. On the possible role of stimulation duration for after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front Cell Neurosci. (2015) 9:311. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Korai SA, Ranieri F, Di Lazzaro V, Papa M, Cirillo G. Neurobiological after-effects of low intensity transcranial electric stimulation of the human nervous system: from basic mechanisms to metaplasticity. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:587771. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.587771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Jamil A, Batsikadze G, Kuo H, Labruna L, Hasan A, Paulus W, et al. Systematic evaluation of the impact of stimulation intensity on neuroplastic after-effects induced by transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. (2017) 595:1273–88. doi: 10.1113/JP272738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Thomas C, Truong D, Clark TK, Datta A. Understanding current flow in galvanic vestibular stimulation: a computational study. Proc Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc EMBS. (2020) 2020:2442–6. doi: 10.1109/EMBC44109.2020.9176716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson's disease, galvanic vestibular stimulation, stimulation frequency, response time, simple reaction time task

Citation: Lee S, Smith PF, Lee WH and McKeown MJ (2021) Frequency-Specific Effects of Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation on Response-Time Performance in Parkinson's Disease. Front. Neurol. 12:758122. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.758122

Received: 13 August 2021; Accepted: 11 October 2021;
Published: 02 November 2021.

Edited by:

Maria Sheila Guimarães Rocha, Hospital Santa Marcelina, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Abhimanyu Mahajan, Rush University Medical Center, United States
Pedro Caldana Gordon, University of Tübingen, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Lee, Smith, Lee and McKeown. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Soojin Lee, soo-jin.lee@ubc.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.