Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 13 July 2022
Sec. Pediatric Neurology
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Pediatric Neurology: 2021 View all 20 articles

Development, Reliability, and Validity of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale: A Tool for Early Identification of Preschoolers at Risk of Learning Disorder in Mainland China

  • 1School of Public Health, Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, China
  • 2Nanjing Maternity and Child Health Care Hospital, Women's Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, China
  • 3School of Pediatrics, Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, China
  • 4School of Early-Childhood Education, NanJing XiaoZhuang University, Nanjing, China
  • 5Research Center of Learning Science, Southeast University, Nanjing, China

Background: Early identification of children at risk of learning disorders (LD) may mitigate the adverse effects of delayed intervention by guiding children to receive preventive services at an earlier age. However, there is no assessment tool for the early identification of children at risk of LD in Mainland China. Therefore, this study aimed to create a Chinese version of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale and investigate its validity and reliability.

Methods: Firstly, a pilot scale was designed based on literature review and expert review. Secondly, a pre-survey of the pilot scale was conducted. In phase 3, a formal survey was carried out to test the reliability and validity of the scale by involving 2,677 preschool children from 7 kindergartens. Data were collected using a checklist for demographic characteristics, the preschool learning skills scale, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P), and Conners' Rating Scales.

Results: The final scale included 38 items under seven factors. The reliability and validity tests confirmed that the Cronbach's alpha, split-half reliability, and test–retest reliability coefficients of the scale were 0.946, 0.888, and 0.941, respectively. The Spearman correlations of factor-total score ranged from 0.685 to 0.876. The results of criterion-related validity showed a direct and significant association between the preschool learning skills scale with the BRIEF-P (r = 0.641, P < 0.001) and the cognitive problems factor of Conners' Rating Scales (r = 0.564, P < 0.001). The model had a good fit (χ2/df = 3.489, RMSEA = 0.047, RMR = 0.024, CFI = 0.912, TLI = 0.900, and IFI = 0.912). Multigroup confirmatory factor analysis supported the structural and measurement invariance on the preschool learning skills scale across gender and grade.

Conclusions: The developed preschool learning skills scale has good reliability and validity, indicating that the scale can be used to identify preschool children at risk of LD and can be recommended for use in clinical research and practice.

Introduction

Specific learning disorder (LD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder (1). According to DSM-5, SLD is a general term that refers to a group of disorders, which may involve persistent difficulties in reading (dyslexia), written expression (dysgraphia), and/or mathematics (dyscalculia), albeit not accounted for by low intelligence (IQ), sensory acuity (e.g., visual problems), poor learning opportunities, or developmental delay (e.g., intellectual disability) (2). DSM-5 describes LD as a neurodevelopmental disorder with a biological origin, including the interaction of genetic, epigenetic, and environmental factors (3, 4). LD is a lifelong disease that may have adverse consequences for children and adults at the educational, social, financial, and professional levels (5).

Studies have shown that the prevalence of LD in the general population ranges from 3 to 12% depending on factors such as different assessment tools, boundaries of measurement, cultural background, gender, age, etc. (68). The average age at which children are diagnosed with LD is 9 years (grades 3–4 of primary school). This is when the academic demands rise and exceed the individual's limited capacities, and children begin to appear to face academic difficulties in school (9). However, it is generally believed that LD occurs prior to kindergarten and continues into adulthood (10, 11). Delayed intervention may have adverse and lasting consequences on the acquisition of academic skills. On the contrary, early identification of children at risk of LD may mitigate the adverse effects of delayed intervention by guiding children to receive preventive services at an earlier age (9, 12).

Researchers have developed many behavior checklists for the screening of LD, which are primarily suitable for school-age children, mainly focusing on academic skills such as reading, writing, and arithmetic (13, 14). In addition, there are behavioral checklists for specific disorders, such as Dyslexia Screening Instrument, designed to identify children who exhibit behaviors related to spelling, reading, writing, or language-processing difficulties (15, 16). These screening tools are relatively brief and cost-effective measures to justify a more detailed assessment or diagnostic test (17).

However, preschool age is a crucial period for early identification and intervention of LD. In kindergarten, children with mathematics disorders already have deficits in comparing non-symbolic and symbolic Arabic numbers (18). Several early or pioneer literacy skills measured in preschool-age have demonstrated strong relationships with future decoding and reading comprehension achievement, such as phonological awareness, rapid naming, and oral language (19, 20). Meanwhile, effective, early reading instructions can improve reading outcomes of children with LD (21, 22). Therefore, more and more researchers pay attention to the early recognition of LD in preschool children (23). Some checklists have been developed for early screening of LD in Hong Kong and Taiwan, such as the Hong Kong Learning Behavior checklist for Preschool Children (Parent Version) (24). However, there is no specific assessment tool in mainland China for the early identification of Preschoolers at risk of LD.

Thus, this study aimed to construct a brief, easy-to-use scale to specify the characteristics of LD for parents from mainland China to identify preschoolers at risk of LD at an early stage and to test the validity and reliability of the scale.

Methods

The Preschool Learning Skills Scale (Supplementary File 1) aimed to develop a brief screening measure for the early identification of preschoolers at a risk for LD. The study was ethically approved by the Institutional Review Committee of Nanjing Medical University.

Development of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

The development of the preschool learning skills scale for parents of preschoolers in mainland China followed many stages (25).

In the first stage, based on an extensive review of relevant literature and published questionnaires, as well as on consultations with parents of children with LD, and specialists and teachers who specialize in LD, the primary cognitive and behavioral manifestations of children with LD in the preschool years were determined to identify key components related to LD risk. Finally, a total of 7 dimensions with 71 items were generated from the item pool, which forms the index system framework of the scale, classified into seven categories: attention, memorization, visual perception, auditory perception, motor coordination, verbal competence, and mathematical concept.

In phase 2, a panel of experts in developmental–behavioral pediatrics, psychology, pedagogy, public health, and clinical evaluation (n = 5, including the first and corresponding authors) reviewed the bank of items to classify those contents into each key theme to identify gaps and overlaps between items and to figure out the suitability of items for parents of preschool children. The scale was adapted and revised with inappropriate items removed. Finally, 55 items were selected to form the first draft of the preschool learning skills scale. All items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from never to always.

In phase 3, to ensure the scale's content validity, the questionnaire was reviewed and refined again by a panel of experts (n = 8) to determine whether the items comprehensively reflected the key behaviors that could develop into LD in preschool-aged children to establish content validity. Then, the questionnaire was put to an experimental test to render certain that the parents of preschoolers (n = 20) understood the items as intended. Furthermore, based on the experimental testing results, the questionnaire was refined by modifying the items which were difficult to understand, had semantic ambiguity, and were prone to ambiguity due to the experimental testing results.

Pre-Survey of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

A convenience sample of participants selected 657 preschool children from two kindergartens in Nanjing in December 2020. Exclusionary criteria were children who were diagnosed with a neurodevelopmental disorder such as autism, intellectual disability, or other disabilities. The parents of eligible participants electronically signed the consent document and completed the 55-item learning skills scale (Draft 1 of the preschool learning skills scale) and a demographic survey.

Through the pre-survey, 55 items were scientifically refined in the following methods: critical ratio method, frequency distribution analysis, variation coefficient (CV) method, correlation analysis, Cronbach's alpha coefficient method, and exploratory factor analysis (25). Considering the above six methods comprehensively, if two or more methods excluded an item, the item would be deleted from the scale. Finally, the preschool learning skills scale with 38 items in 7 factors is compiled.

Reliability and Validity of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

A total of 2,677 preschool children were selected from 7 kindergartens in Nanjing. Exclusion and inclusion criteria were the same as those for the pre-test of the study. The parents electronically signed the consent document and completed the questionnaire survey within 3 days of receipt of the survey (first assessment T1). In addition to the 38-item learning skills scale (Draft 2 of the preschool learning skills scale) and a demographic survey, 600 parents were randomly selected to extra complete the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) and Conners' Rating Scales (Conners 3-P) for assessment of criterion-related validity.

The reliability of the scale was assessed using Cronbach's alpha, split-half correlation, and test–retest. A random sample of the parents filled in the preschool learning skills scale 2 weeks after the first survey (T2) to assess test–retest reliability.

For the exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, data from four kindergartens (n = 1,540) were used for the exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and data from three kindergartens (n = 1,137) for the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The EFA was performed by IBM SPSS Statistics 26.0. The suitability of the data for factorization was evaluated by the value for the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy and Bartlett's test of sphericity (preferably significant) (26). The EFA was done via iterative Maximum Likelihood with Promax Rotation to extract the factors due to the correlation of the factors (27). The criterion for loading and cross-loading was set at 0.4. The CFA was performed with IBM SPSS Amos 22.0. The goodness of model fit was evaluated using the following fit indices: the chi-square goodness of fit (χ2/df ) values, with values <5.0 deemed acceptable; the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA); the root-mean-square residual (RMR) <0.05; the comparative fit index (CFI); the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI); and the incremental fit index (IFI) >0.9 (28, 29). The construct validity of the preschool learning skills scale was examined with the standardized regression coefficients and construct reliability (CR) for convergent validity and the correlation coefficient, and the square root of AVE for discriminant validity.

Multigroup Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

We performed a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis across gender and grade. Subsequently, using the sample from three kindergartens (n = 1,137), we conducted a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis to examine the measurement invariance of the seven-factor structure across gender: boys vs. girls. Measurement invariance was also tested across age groups: Group 1 (Junior Class of kindergarten), Group 2 (Middle Class of kindergarten), and Group 3 (Senior Class of kindergarten). The measurement invariance was evaluated using the following fit indices: the change in chi-square values (Δχ2) and the fit indices (RMR, RMSEA, CFI, ΔRMSEA, and ΔCFI) (30, 31). Measurement invariance is supported when ΔRMSEA is <0.015 and ΔCFI is <0.02 (32).

Results

Participants

As shown in Table 1, the study included 2,677 preschool children: 1,445 boys and 1,232 girls with a mean age of 5.2 years (SD = 0.9, range 3.5–6.8). There was no significant mean age difference between the genders. Most of the questionnaire fillers were mothers (n = 2,090, 78.07%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of participants (n = 2,677).

The Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

The appropriateness of factor analysis was measured by The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of the sampling adequacy and the Bartlett test of sphericity. The results showed KMO = 0.966 and Bartlett significance P < 0.001, indicating that exploratory factor analysis was appropriate.

Seven factors were extracted from the preschool learning skills scale by the EFA (see Table 2). The factor loadings varied from 0.401 to 0.787. The seven factors explained 55.30% of the variance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Factor loadings from the exploratory factor analysis of the preschool learning skills scale (n = 1,540).

Reliability and Validity of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

The reliability results of PLSS are shown in Table 3 below. Cronbach's alpha coefficient of the preschool learning skills scale was 0.946 and Cronbach's alpha coefficients of the factors ranged from 0.674 to 0.871, all >0.70 except for mathematical concepts, which meant that the scale had good internal consistency reliability. The split-half reliability coefficient was 0.888, and the split-half reliability coefficient of the factors varied between 0.638 and 0.834, indicating that the scale had good internal reliability. The test–retest reliability coefficient was 0.941, and the test–retest reliability coefficient of the factors ranged from 0.774 to 0.939, which reflected excellent test–retest reliability of the scale.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The reliability of the preschool learning skills scale (n = 1,540).

The Spearman correlations of the factor-total score ranged from 0.685 to 0.876. The Spearman correlations of the interfactor varied between 0.417 and 0.716 (Table 4); the correlation coefficients among factors A, B, C, D, and F were beyond 0.6, but <0.8, indicating that the correlation between factors had reached an acceptable level. Moreover, all Spearman correlations of the factor-total score were more significant than the interfactor. The results of the correlation analysis showed a direct and significant association of the preschool learning skills scale with the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (r = 0.641, P < 0.001) and the cognitive problems factor of Conners' Rating Scales (r = 0.564, P < 0.001).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Spearman correlations of interfactor and factor-total score (n = 1,540).

The Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale

Figure 1 shows the factor structure and model fit of the preschool learning skills scale using CFA. The CFA of the seven-factor model of the preschool learning skills scale showed that the χ2/df = 3.489, the RMSEA was 0.047, the RMR was 0.024, the CFI was 0.912, the TLI was 0.900, and the IFI was 0.912. These results indicated that the goodness-of-fit index of the model was valid. The standardized regression coefficients of each item of the preschool learning skills scale ranged from 0.490 to 0.747. These values were more than 0.4 (Table 5) and CR values also were more than 1.965 (P < 0.001), which indicated that the items corresponding to each latent variable were highly representative. The discriminant validity of the preschool learning skills scale was evaluated using correlation coefficients among seven factors (Table 6). Correlations ranged from 0.129 to 0.211 among seven factors, all of which were significantly correlated (P < 0.001). The correlation coefficients were all less than the square root of the corresponding AVE. These results were satisfactory and indicated adequate discriminant validity in the study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The standardized path coefficients of the preschool learning skills scale (n = 1,137).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Standardized regression coefficients of the preschool learning skills scale (n = 1,137).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Discriminant validity of the preschool learning skills scale (n = 1,137).

Measurement Invariance Across Gender and Grade

To examine the measurement invariance of the preschool learning skills scale across both gender and grade, we used a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis approach, which assesses the measurement invariance across two or more groups by using a series of increasingly stringent, nested models.

Table 7 presented that the fit indices for Model 1 (configural invariance) indicated that the seven-factor measurement model of the preschool learning skills scale had an acceptable fit within each gender group (RMR = 0.027; RMSEA = 0.036; CFI = 0.899). Based on the indices of practical fit and the change in RMSEA and CFI criterion recommended, our results concluded that Model 2 (Metric invariance), Model 3 (Scalar invariance), and Model 4 (Strict factorial) fit nearly as well as Model 1 (RMR <0.05; RMSEA <0.05; CFI >0.8; ΔRMSEA <0.015; and ΔCFI <0.02), supporting measurement invariance across gender groups.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Multigroup CFA Fit indices for the preschool learning skills scale across gender (n = 1,137).

Subsequently, measurement invariance across grades was examined. As Table 8 showed, the configural invariance was supported by the acceptable absolute fit indices (RMR = 0.029; RMSEA = 0.031; CFI = 0.887). In addition, also measurement invariance (at the metric, scalar, and strict factorial level) across grades was present because all absolute and relative fit indices were acceptable. All these results indicated a reasonable level of structural invariance and measurement invariance in all three grade groups of the preschool learning skills scale.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Multigroup CFA fit indices for the preschool learning skills scale across grade (n = 1,137).

Discussion

Learning disorders refers to a group of disorders characterized by significant difficulties in listening, reading, speaking, writing, attention, memorization, and coordination. These difficulties range from mild to severe. Researchers have made consistent efforts to identify and intervene early to ensure that children receive assistance prior to having poor learning experiences and prevent other problems that may affect their learning abilities (16, 18, 21). As to early identification, it is believed that parents are the ones who closely observe children in parent–child interactions to identify behavioral indicators of LD, such as literacy problems and specific cognitive deficits. Thus, we developed the preschool learning skills scale as a parental checklist to provide information on the characteristics of children with LD at preschool age and as a screening measure to make a more detailed follow-up assessment of children at risk of LD.

Different manifestations of LD can be seen at various ages and as a result of varying learning demands. Delays in speech and language development, numerical and symbolic concepts, motor coordination, and auditory and visual perception are early indicators of children who may have LD (3335). These indicators may occur concomitantly with attention, memorization, or self-regulation problems. The Hong Kong Learning Behavior checklist for Preschool Children (Parent Version), which was developed by Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team in 2006, identified preschoolers at the risk of learning difficulties in seven aspects: language ability, learning ability, writing performance, attention, memorization, sequencing ability, spatial awareness, and motor coordination (24). However, the scale was specifically validated for the population of Hong Kong, and mainly aimed at the early identification of dyslexia and dysgraphia. The spoken language of Hong Kong includes Cantonese and English, and the scale contains some items to check English ability. Besides, the education system of Hong Kong is different from that of mainland China. The teaching of literacy and handwriting begins in early childhood, much earlier than in the Mainland, so there are many items about the ability to learn Chinese (such as reading or interest in words) and writing performance in that scale. Therefore, the scale is not wholly applicable to the early recognition of LD for Mainland children, but the theoretical framework and some items are still worth our reference.

In this study, we first defined the connotation and characteristics of LD by reviewing the related researches. A literature search was performed using the following terms: “learning ability,” “LD,” “dyslexia,” “mathematical disorder,” “kindergarten,” and “preschool children” in PubMed, EMBASE, Web of Science, and MEDLINE, CNKI, and Wanfang databases. In the retrieved literature, the behavioral manifestations and characteristics of children with LD were perused, as well as related guidelines, expert consensus, systematic evaluation, and original research on screening and diagnosis of LD. Then, we summarized and compared the scales or tools for early identification of LD in domestic and foreign studies, such as the Pupil Rating Scale Revised (PRS) questionnaire, Hong Kong Learning Behavior checklist for Preschool Children (Parent Version), East Asia-Pacific Early Child Development Scales, South African Early Learning Outcomes Measure, etc., to clear and definite the methods, structures, and indicators of early identification of LD. Finally, we studied the localization theory of early identification for LD in Mainland China. Through in-depth interviews with experts in child psychology, developmental-behavioral pediatrics, teachers, and parents of children with LD, their attitudes, views, and experiences toward the early identification of LD in the preschool age were summarized. From these, the preschool learning skills scale, which is the first scale for early identification of preschoolers at risk of LD in Mainland China, establishes the item pool for the initial experimental checklist based on the theoretical framework of the following: attention, memorization, visual perception, auditory perception, motor coordination, verbal competence, and mathematical concept.

Our research conducted psychometric evaluation through the critical ratio method, frequency distribution analysis, variation coefficient (CV) method, correlation analysis, Cronbach's alpha coefficient method, and exploratory factor analysis, suggesting the retention of 38 items to be included in the revised checklist. To evaluate the reliability and validity of the 38-item preschool learning skills scale, we conducted a questionnaire survey among 2,677 preschool children from 7 kindergartens. Overall, our results demonstrate that the developed preschool learning skills scale had good reliability and validity, and showed an excellent fit of the seven derived factors via exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. After establishing a robust factor structure within a sample, it is essential to know whether this factor structure is also applicable across samples or groups, as this “invariance” is the only condition that can be allowed to investigate group differences, for instance, between different genders and ages, and so on (36). Given the difference between the scale scores of boys and girls (P < 0.001) and three grades of kindergarten (P < 0.001), it is crucial to establish measurement invariance across gender and grade to elucidate if the preschool learning skills scale's sensitivity can identify preschool children at risk of LD. The results of the multigroup confirmatory factor analysis indicated a reasonable level of structural invariance and measurement invariance across gender and grade of the preschool learning skills scale. Therefore, as a screening scale, the preschool learning skills scale was considered to provide information on the cognitive weaknesses of LD and could be used to screen for children at risk of LD for further assessment or preventive interventions.

Nevertheless, further research is needed. Since this study relied on parent reports, it will be necessary for future research to explore the correspondence between parental reports with other reports (such as teachers) and observations of clinicians or educational psychologists (37, 38). More specifically, it will be necessary to further establish the validity of the preschool learning skills scale in the prediction of LD by using multimodal methods and exploring the relationship between the preschool learning skills scale with experimental and behavioral paradigms, such as rapid automatized naming, phonological awareness, letter knowledge, and short-term verbal memorization (19, 39).

Despite these limitations, the preschool learning skills scale has the potential to be a reliable measure that provides a scientific basis for early identification and intervention of LD in preschool age.

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this is the first study to develop a brief, easy-to-use questionnaire to describe the characteristics of LD in preschool-age for early identification of children at risk of LD. Overall, as an instrument, the developed preschool learning skills scale has good reliability and validity, which indicates that the scale can be used for the early identification of preschool children at risk of LD and can be recommended for use in clinical research and practice. However, study findings are limited to the early identification of preschool children at the risk of LD, and the evaluation of the predictability of the scale is needed. Further research is needed to evaluate the validity of the scale by examining the relationship between the preschool learning skills scale and objective predictors of LD, such as rapid automatized naming, phonological awareness, letter knowledge, and short-term verbal memorization.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Medical Ethics Committee in the Nanjing Maternity and Child Health Care Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

MY: conceptualization, methodology, and writing—original draft preparation. JW: data curation, formal analysis, and writing—original draft preparation. PL: methodology and data curation. YX: validation. YG: data collection. LZ: data collection and validation. NS: language proofreading. YL: funding acquisition. DY: methodology. QH: supervision and writing—reviewing and editing. XC: funding acquisition and writing—reviewing and editing. All authors reviewed the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the High Level Talents of Jiangsu Province (Project No. WSN-165/WSW-125), the Key Medical Science and Technology Development Projects in Nanjing (Project No. ZKX 18044), the Medical Science and Technology Development Projects in Nanjing (Project No. YKK 19115), Specialized Disease Cohort Study of Nanjing Medical University (Project No. NMUC2018014A), the Key Young Medical Talents of Jiangsu Province (Project No. QNRC2016100), and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Project No. 32000759).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2022.918163/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

LD, learning disorder; BRIEF, behavior rating inventory of executive function; EFA, exploratory factor analysis; CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; χ2/df, chi-square goodness of fit values; RMSEA, root-mean-square error of approximation; RMR, root-mean-square residual; CFI, comparative fit index; TLI, tucker-lewis index; IFI, incremental fit index; CR, composite reliability; AVE, average variance extracted.

References

1. Mcdonough EM, Flanagan DP, Sy M, Alfonso VC. Specific learning disorder. Handbook of DSM-5 Disord Child Adolesc. (2017) 77–104. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-57196-6_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Lyon G. Learning disabilities. Fut Child. (1996) 6:54–76. doi: 10.2307/1602494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub (2013).

Google Scholar

4. Georgitsi M, Dermitzakis I, Soumelidou E, Bonti E. The polygenic nature and complex genetic architecture of specific learning disorder. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:631. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11050631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bonti E, Giannoglou S, Georgitsi M, Sofologi M, Porfyri GN, Mousioni A, et al. Clinical profiles and socio-demographic characteristics of adults with specific learning disorder in northern greece. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:602. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11050602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kucian K, Aster MV. Developmental dyscalculia. Eur J Pediatr. (2015) 174:1–13. doi: 10.1007/s00431-014-2455-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Peters L, Ansari D. Are specific learning disorders truly specific, and are they disorders? Trends Neurosci Educ. (2019) 17:100115. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2019.100115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Wagner RK, Zirps FA, Edwards AA, Wood SG, Beal B. The prevalence of dyslexia: a new approach to its estimation. J Learn Disabil. (2020) 53:354–65. doi: 10.1177/0022219420920377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Shaywitz SE. Current concepts: dyslexia. N Engl J Ofmed. (1998) 338:307–12. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801293380507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sullivan A, Kohli N, Farnsworth E, Sadeh S, Jones L. Longitudinal models of reading achievement of students with learning disabilities and without disabilities. School Psychol Quart. (2017) 32:336–49. doi: 10.1037/spq0000170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Zhang X, Räsänen P, Koponen T, Aunola K, Lerkkanen M, Nurmi J. Early cognitive precursors of children's mathematics learning disability and persistent low achievement: a 5-year longitudinal study. Child Devel. (2020) 91:7–27. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Stevens EA, Walker MA, Vaughn S. The effects of reading fluency interventions on the reading fluency and reading comprehension performance of elementary students with learning disabilities: a synthesis of the research from 2001 to 2014. J Learn Disabil. (2016) 2016:0022219416638028. doi: 10.1177/0022219416638028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Shiota M, Koeda T, Takeshita K. [Clinical studies of learning disability. Part I: Two-axial diagnosis of learning disability using the Pupil Rating Scale Revised (PRS) and WISC-R]. Brain Devel. (1995) 27:455–460.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

14. Patrick KE, Mccurdy M, Chute DL, Mahone EM, Zabel TA, Jacobson LA. Clinical utility of the colorado learning difficulties questionnaire. Pediatrics. (2013) 132:1257–64. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. ASHAWeb. Dyslexia Screening Instrument. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (1994).

Google Scholar

16. Chan DW, Ho SH, Chung KKH, Tsang SM, Lee SH. The Hong Kong behaviour checklist for primary students: developing a brief dyslexia screening measure. Int J Disab. (2012) 59:173–96. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2012.676437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lonigan, Christopher J. Development, assessment, and promotion of preliteracy skills. Early Educ Devel. (2006) 17:91–114. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1701_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Desoete A, Ceulemans A, De Weerdt F, Pieters S. Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. Brit J Educ Psychol. (2012) 82:64–81. doi: 10.1348/2044-8279.002002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sittner Bridges M, Catts H. The use of a dynamic screening of phonological awareness to predict risk for reading disabilities in kindergarten children. J Learn Disab. (2011) 44:330–8. doi: 10.1177/0022219411407863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mazzocco M, Grimm K. Growth in rapid automatized naming from grades K to 8 in children with math or reading disabilities. J Learn Disab. (2013) 46:517–33. doi: 10.1177/0022219413477475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Clemens N, Lee K, Henri M, Simmons L, Kwok O, Al Otaiba S. Growth on sublexical fluency progress monitoring measures in early kindergarten and relations to word reading acquisition. J School Psychol. (2020) 79:43–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2020.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Al Otaiba S, Folsom J, Schatschneider C, Wanzek J, Greulich L, Meadows J, et al. Predicting first grade reading performance from kindergarten response to tier 1 instruction. Except Child. (2011) 77:453–70. doi: 10.1177/001440291107700405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wong YE. The development of a behaviour checklist for parents to screen preschoolers at risk for specific learning difficulties in reading and writing (SpLD). Hku Theses Online. (2004). doi: 10.5353/th_b2979322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Wong E, Ho C, Chung K, Tsang S, Lee S. The Hong Kong Learning Behavior Checklist for Preschool Children (Parent Version). Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team (2006). Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/10722/255770

Google Scholar

25. Cheng C, Martin-Biggers J, Quick V, Spaccarotella K, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Validity and reliability of HOP-Up: a questionnaire to evaluate physical activity environments in homes with preschool-aged children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Acti. (2016) 13:91. doi: 10.1186/s12966-016-0417-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Iorio C. Measurement in Health Behavior: Methods for Research and Evaluation. New York, NY: Jossey-Bass (2006).

Google Scholar

27. Preacher KJ, Maccallum RC. Repairing tom swift's electric factor analysis machine. Understand Stat. (2003) 2:13–43. doi: 10.1207/S15328031US0201_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Phillips JM, Uljarevi M, Schuck RK, Schapp S, Hardan AY. Development of the stanford social dimensions scale: initial validation in autism spectrum disorder and in neurotypicals. Mol Autism. (2019) 10:48. doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0298-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Tabachnick G, Fidell L. Using Multivariate Statistics, 4th ed. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon (2001).

Google Scholar

30. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing MI. Struct Equat Mod Multid J. (2002) 9:235–55. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Meade AW, Johnson EC, Braddy PW. Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. J Appl Psychol. (2008) 93:568–92. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Vandenberg RJ, Lance CE. A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizat Res Meth. (2000) 5:139–58. doi: 10.1177/109442810031002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Gartland D, Strosnider R. Learning disabilities and young children: identification and intervention. Learn Disab Quart. (2007) 30:63–72. doi: 10.2307/30035516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lyon GR, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. A definition of dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. (2003) 53:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Mazibuko X, Flack P, Kvalsvig J. Towards a South African model of language-based learning disability. South Afri J Commun Dis. (2019) 66:e1–8. doi: 10.4102/sajcd.v66i1.634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Rens V, Lugtig P, Hox J. A checklist for testing measurement invariance. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2012) 9:486–92. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2012.686740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Pediatrics A. American academy of pediatrics: developmental surveillance and screening of infants and young children. Pediatrics. (2001) 108:192–5. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.1.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Souroulla AV, Panayiotou G, Kokkinos CM. The role of the teacher in identifying learning disabilities. J Learn Disab. (2009) 42:483–93. doi: 10.1177/0022219409335217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ozernov-Palchik O, Norton ES, Sideridis G, Beach SD, Wolf M, Gabrieli JD, et al. Longitudinal stability of pre-reading skill profiles of kindergarten children: implications for early screening and theories of reading. Dev Sci. (2016) 20:e12471. doi: 10.1111/desc.12471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: learning disorder, preschoolers, early identification, scale development, validity and reliability

Citation: Yao M, Wang J, Liu P, Guo Y, Xie Y, Zhang L, Su N, Li Y, Yu D, Hong Q and Chi X (2022) Development, Reliability, and Validity of the Preschool Learning Skills Scale: A Tool for Early Identification of Preschoolers at Risk of Learning Disorder in Mainland China. Front. Neurol. 13:918163. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.918163

Received: 12 April 2022; Accepted: 20 June 2022;
Published: 13 July 2022.

Edited by:

Hong Ni, Children's Hospital of Soochow University, China

Reviewed by:

Fei Li, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China
John Gilmour, The University of Queensland, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Yao, Wang, Liu, Guo, Xie, Zhang, Su, Li, Yu, Hong and Chi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xia Chi, chixia2001@njmu.edu.cn; Qin Hong, rambler_hq@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.