Skip to main content

FRONTIERS COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 25 September 2012
Sec. Neurogenomics

Interaction between Emotion and Attention Systems

  • Department of Neurology, Shimane University, Izumo, Japan

A commentary on

Diverting attention suppresses human amygdala responses to faces
by Morawetz, C., Baudewig, J., Treue, S., and Dechent, P. (2010). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:226. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00226

Rapid and efficient selection of emotionally salient or goal-relevant environmental stimuli is crucial for flexible and adaptive behaviors. Emotional events rapidly and automatically capture attention by activating subcortical neural structures (Ohman, 2005; Zikopoulos and Barbas, 2012). Behavioral data suggest that emotion processing, selective attention, and the interactions between them extract emotional values from sensory stimuli and lead to the generation of appropriate responses. The amygdala plays a pivotal role in emotional information processing. Furthermore, through feedback connections to sensory cortical processing regions, the amygdala may facilitate attentional, and perceptual processes. Thus, the link and interaction between emotion and attention systems have been the subjects of great interest in the field (Pessoa, 2005; Pourtois et al., 2012).

With regard to the interaction of emotion and attention, there are unsolved and controversial issues. For example, to what extent does emotional stimulus processing depend on attentional resource availability? Previous studies have shown conflicting results, probably due to several factors, including attentional load allocated to competing tasks. Some studies have demonstrated that emotional faces evoke amygdalar responses even when attention is diverted to other stimuli, suggesting that some types of emotional perception occur outside of top-down directed attention (Vuilleumier et al., 2001; Anderson et al., 2003). However, others have suggested that the perception of emotional items requires attention. This was demonstrated with attentional manipulations that were designed to utilize a high level of processing resources, leaving less bandwidth for processing unattended emotional items (Bishop et al., 2007; Lim et al., 2008). Another conflicting issue is whether the spatial location of emotional stimuli (foveal or peripheral) affects emotion processing. Previous studies have demonstrated that the amygdala was activated independent of attentional location when emotional stimuli were presented in the peripheral visual field (Williams et al., 2005); however, this was not the case when stimuli were presented foveally. Ignored emotional faces presented foveally led to increased amygdalar response only under low attentional load conditions (Pessoa, 2005). This discrepancy in the interaction between attentional load and stimulus location may be related to the anatomical differences in visual pathways for foveal and peripheral stimuli (i.e., parvocellular and magnocellular pathways, respectively).

Morawetz et al. (2010) directly addressed these questions in a series of block-designed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments, in which they systematically manipulated both the attentional load and the spatial position of emotional stimuli. They imposed four types of tasks with two attentional conditions; two attend-faces conditions (gender-matching and emotion-matching tasks) and two ignore-faces conditions (easy and difficult digit/letter-matching tasks). The behavioral data suggested that the experimental design successfully controlled the attentional load and perceptual properties of emotional stimuli (i.e., image size) presented foveally or peripherally in the visual field. Their study resulted in two main findings. First, amygdalar activity was strongly suppressed during high attentional loads, which suggests that emotional information processing might be suspended when attention-demanding cognitive tasks are performed simultaneously. Although this finding is not new (Pessoa, 2005), this was the first study in which attentional load was systematically manipulated in a quantitative manner. The result expands the hypothesis that amygdalar emotional information processing is dependent on available attentional resources and could be affected by top-down attentional control from other brain structures. Second, the eccentricity of the emotional stimuli did not affect the amygdalar response, and there was no interaction effect between attentional load and stimulus location. This finding did not support the hypothesis of predominant processing of peripherally presented emotional stimuli, but it is hard to compare the results with those of previous studies because the experimental designs and analysis methods were different. The block-design paradigm and region of interest–based analysis employed in the current experiment may have reduced the sensitivity for detecting habituated and/or very focal amygdalar responses.

In summary, Morawetz et al. (2010) provided compelling evidence of a spatial attention effect on emotion processing in the amygdala by using a well-designed experimental paradigm. A recent fMRI study with a cue-associated covert attention task reported results in line with their findings (Brassen et al., 2010). However, their analyses of fMRI data were restricted to the amygdala and fusiform gyrus. It is obvious that spatial attention involves a widely distributed network, including the prefrontal cortex, thalamus, parietal cortex, and subcortical structures. Studies have suggested that emotionally salient and threatening stimuli automatically capture attention (Carlson et al., 2009). Thus, further analyses of blood oxygen level–dependent signals in other brain areas engaged in attentional processes would provide new insight into the bidirectional interaction between attention and emotion systems. Because the processing speeds of both networks may be too rapid to detect with fMRI, other neurophysiological tools, such as electroencephalography or magneto encephalography, could be suitable assessments (Pessoa, 2010). The functional interaction of emotional and attentional systems remains to be fully explored, but it might play an important role in psychopathological conditions, such as anxiety or phobia.

References

Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. J. Neurosci. 23, 5627–5633.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bishop, S. J., Jenkins, R., and Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Neural processing of fearful faces: effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1595–1603.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brassen, S., Gamer, M., Rose, M., and Buchel, C. (2010). The influence of directed covert attention on emotional face processing. Neuroimage 50, 545–551.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Carlson, J. M., Reinke, K. S., and Habib, R. (2009). A left amygdala mediated network for rapid orienting to masked fearful faces. Neuropsychologia 47, 1386–1389.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lim, S. L., Padmala, S., and Pessoa, L. (2008). Affective learning modulates spatial competition during low-load attentional conditions. Neuropsychologia 46, 1267–1278.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morawetz, C., Baudewig, J., Treue, S., and Dechent, P. (2010). Diverting attention suppresses human amygdala responses to faces. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:226. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00226

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ohman, A. (2005). The role of the amygdala in human fear: automatic detection of threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 953–958.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 188–196.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pessoa, L. (2010). Emotion and attention effects: is it all a matter of timing? Not yet. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:172. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00172

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30, 829–841.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, M. A., McGlone, F., Abbott, D. F., and Mattingley, J. B. (2005). Differential amygdala responses to happy and fearful facial expressions depend on selective attention. Neuroimage 24, 417–425.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zikopoulos, B., and Barbas, H. (2012). Pathways for emotions and attention converge on the thalamic reticular nucleus in primates. J. Neurosci. 32, 5338–5350.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Yamaguchi S and Onoda K (2012) Interaction between emotion and attention systems. Front. Neurosci. 6:139. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00139

Received: 12 June 2012; Accepted: 06 September 2012;
Published online: 25 September 2012.

Edited by:

Hauke R Heekeren, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Idan Segev, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel

Copyright: © 2012 Yamaguchi and Onoda. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: yamagu3n@med.shimane-u.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.