Skip to main content

FRONTIERS COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 11 October 2012
Sec. Neurogenomics

Dancing combines the essence for successful aging

  • 1Ageing and Living Conditions Programme, Centre for Population Studies, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
  • 2Umeå Centre for Functional Brain Imaging, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden

A commentary on

Superior sensory, motor, and cognitive performance in elderly individuals with multi-year dancing activities
by Kattenstroth, J.-C., Kolankowska, I., Kalisch, T., and Dinse, H. R. (2010). Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:31. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00031

In the scientific community much room has been given to the relationships between aging, cognition, and the brain. Unfortunately, there is strong evidence that aging is associated with impaired cognitive function (Rönnlund et al., 2005) as well as negative changes in brain structure (Salami et al., 2012) and in brain function (Nyberg et al., 2010). Consequently, with a worldwide population that is rapidly getting older the search for factors that facilitates successful aging, which in the present paper is referred to as maintained cognitive and brain functions, is a major and important challenge (Nyberg et al., 2012). In fact, there are suggestions regarding what can be done in order to maintain, or even improve, our memory functions over the lifespan. One line of research has focused on memory training. It has been shown that elderly engaged in a 5 week working memory training program improved their working memory function (Dahlin et al., 2008). However, it was also shown that the effect was limited to only the trained task for the elderly, and that only young participants showed evidence for transfer, i.e., an improvement in an untrained task, importantly, however, only to a task within the same cognitive domain. A second line of research has focused on the importance of a social network providing emotional and intellectual simulation in order to maintain cognitive and brain functions at older age (Fratiglioni et al., 2000). Moreover, this line of research has even suggested that absence of close social ties may be a risk factor for dementia. A third line of research has focused on how a physically active lifestyle may protect against age related cognitive decline. In several studies it has been shown how elderly that frequently engage in physical activity also have preserved brain structure (Erickson et al., 2009) as well as cognitive functions (Hillman et al., 2008), compared with elderly that do not take part in physical activities, especially cardiovascular activities. Moreover, it has also been shown that sedentary elderly individuals that starts to engage in moderate to low physical cardiovascular exercise actually improve their cognitive performance (Hillman et al., 2008), they increase their brain activity, measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (Colcombe et al., 2004), and they even show a preserved and increased (!) volume of a brain structure that is important for memory, the hippocampus (Erickson et al., 2011). Thus, these separate lines of research suggests that factors such as keeping your memory system active, socializing, and being physically active may actually help to reduce the decline of cognitive functions associated with increased age.

Kattenstroth et al. (2010) hypothesized that dance would combine these different factors and provide superior sensory, motor, and cognitive performance in elderly that had been dancing for quite some time, compared with inactive elderly participants. To test the hypothesize they examined a group of elderly that could be categorized as amateur dancers with a longtime, over 16 years on average, history of dancing. They compared these individuals to a group of non-dancer controls, with no record of neither dancing nor sporting activities. There was no difference between the two groups in terms of education, gender or age. The authors measured a multitude of functions such as cognitive performance, reaction time performance, posture and balance, motor performance, and tactile performance investigating the hypothesis that long-year dancing activity promotes general advantages and preserves a multitude of body and mind functions. The results showed that in each of the different measured categories the dancing group outperformed the controls. The results from this study imply that the general decline in motor and cognitive performances often associated with increased age may be reduced if one engages in dancing during the life span. The effects were also reflected in every day competence such as independence, general health status, and life contentment. Interestingly, examining individual data it was clear that the effects were not a result of single individuals showing a superior behavior. Rather, individual data revealed that there were no individuals with a lifelong history of dancing that showed a poor performance on the different measures, as was observed from the participants without a dancing history.

An important feature of the Kattenstroth et al. (2010) study was that they were able to show the effects on multitude functions including cognition, posture, balance, as well as sensorimotor functions. Many studies have only measured functions within one category, for example cognition, and thus, have only been able to examine and make conclusions regarding isolated variables. In comparison, Kattenstroth and colleagues were able to show how dancing appears to have global effects on the body and on the mind.

Although the study by Kattenstroth et al. (2010) did not compare the results from their group of dancers with groups of elderly that have been engaging in other types of physical activities, it is tempting to suggest that dancing actually combines the essence for successful aging, at least in terms of maintained cognitive and brain functions. This can be hypothesized based on the fact that dance includes the three separate lines of research that has been suggested to promote brain functions including cardiovascular physical activity, cognitive training (as in learning new dance routines with different coordination), as well as social and emotional interactions. Thus, in future research it would be of interest to use dance as the intervention and to examine if similar beneficial effects could be reached in groups of elderly without prior history of dancing. Until then, the study by Kattenstroth and colleagues definitely proposes that one should gladly accept invitations for some turns and twists on a dance floor.

References

Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 3316–3321.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dahlin, E., Stigsdotter-Neely, A., Larsson, A., Bäckman, L., and Nyberg, L. (2008). Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science 320, 1510–1512.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Hu, L., Morris, K. S., et al. (2009). Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 19, 1030–1039.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fratiglioni, L., Wang, H.-X., Ericsson, K., Maytan, M., and Winblad, B. (2000). Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet 355, 1315–1319.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hillman, C. H., Erikson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kattenstroth, J.-C., Kolankowska, I., Kalisch, T., and Dinse, H. R. (2010). Superior sensory, motor, and cognitive performance in elderly individuals with multi-year dancing activities. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:31. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 16, 292–305.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nyberg, L., Salami, A., Andersson, M., Eriksson, J., Kalpouzos, G., Kauppi, K., et al. (2010). Longitudinal evidence for diminished frontal cortex function in aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 22682–22686.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rönnlund, M., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., and Nilsson, L.-G. (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychol. Aging 20, 3–18.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salami, A., Eriksson, J., Nilsson, L.-G., and Nyberg, L. (2012). Age-related white matter microstructural differences partly mediate age-related decline in processing speed but not cognition. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1822, 408–415.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Olsson C-J (2012) Dancing combines the essence for successful aging. Front. Neurosci. 6:155. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00155

Received: 14 May 2012; Accepted: 25 September 2012;
Published online: 11 October 2012.

Edited by:

Hauke R. Heekeren, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany

Copyright: © 2012 Olsson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: cj.olsson@ddb.umu.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.