Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 21 April 2017
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Serum Oxytocin Levels and an Oxytocin Receptor Gene Polymorphism (rs2254298) Indicate Social Deficits in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • 1Department of Maternal, Child and Adolescent Health, School of Public Health, Tianjin Medical University, Tianjin, China
  • 2Department of Applied Science, The College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA, USA

The neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) and its receptor (OXTR) have been predicted to be involved in the regulation of social functioning in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Objective of the study was to investigate serum OT levels and the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism in Chinese Han children and adolescents with ASD as well as to identify their social deficits relevant to the oxytocinergic system. We tested serum OT levels using ELISA in 55 ASD subjects and 110 typically developing (TD) controls as well as genotyped the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism using PCR-RFLP in 100 ASD subjects and 232 TD controls. Autistic symptoms were assessed by the Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC) and the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). There were no significant associations between OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and ASD, serum OT levels and age, as well as serum OT levels and intelligent quotient (IQ) in both ASD and TD groups. However, ASD subjects exhibited elevated serum OT levels compared to TD controls and positive correlations between serum OT levels and “adaptation to change score” in the CARS and CARS total scores. Moreover, in the ASD group, significant relationships were revealed between the single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs2254298 and serum OT levels, the category “stereotypes and object use” in the ABC and the category “adaptation to change” in the CARS. These findings indicated that individuals with ASD may exhibit a dysregulation in OT on the basis of changes in OXTR gene expression as well as environmentally induced alterations of the oxytocinergic system to determine their social deficits.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are a set of complex neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by deficits in social interaction and communication as well as restricted, repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A recent epidemiological survey estimated the prevalence of ASD to be one in 68 children, with a male: female ratio of 4.5: 1 (CDC, 2014). The etiology of ASD likely includes both genetic heritability and environmental risk factors (Hallmayer et al., 2011; Ronald and Hoekstra, 2011), and twin studies have demonstrated an estimated heritability of 70–90% (Freitag, 2011; Robinson et al., 2012). ASD result in negative effects on physical health and quality of life among the ASD population as well as heavy burdens to their families and society.

A wealth of literature indicates intense interest in the function of oxytocin (OT) in affiliation and social behavior in both animals and humans (Carter, 1998; Donaldson and Young, 2008; Insel, 2010). Some studies have also suggested a potential association between a primary dysfunction of the central OT system and core symptoms of social deficits in ASD (Insel et al., 1999; Lukas and Neumann, 2013). Because previous work has implied the important role of oxytocin receptor (OXTR) in the modulation of OT activities and the OT-OXTR signaling may be involved in ASD pathophysiology (Yamasue, 2013), the OXTR gene is notable as a potential candidate gene for ASD. These findings have attracted researchers to investigate whether genetic variation in human OXTR gene is associated with social deficits in ASD.

OT is a nonapeptide with a highly conserved structure and is mainly synthesized in magnocellular neurons in the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei of the mammalian hypothalamus. OT affects peripheral tissues and central brain regions, including the central OXTR network. Centrally projecting neuropeptidergic neurons target regions that regulate stress coping, moods, and social behaviors (Taurines et al., 2014). In addition, OT has been reported to be involved in attachment, pair bonding, sexual reproduction, maternal behavior, stress response, social memory, emotional cognition, and social interaction (Insel and Fernald, 2004; Kosfeld et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2009). Metabolic abnormalities of OT (Green et al., 2001) have exhibited a significant association with impairment in social interaction and communication in ASD individuals.

These outcomes indicate that OT may play a role in the etiology of ASD, especially in the social impairment domain. Several exogenous OT treatments have managed to normalize impaired social functioning among individuals with ASD. Adult ASD patients exhibited reduced repetitive behaviors (Hollander et al., 2003) and enhanced affective speech comprehension (Hollander et al., 2007) after intravenous OT infusion. Improved emotional recognition from eye regions in ASD boys was reported after intranasal OT administration (Guastella et al., 2010). Moreover, increased cooperation in simulated ball games, feelings of trust with teammates and time spent gazing at the eye regions of unfamiliar faces (Andari et al., 2010), as well as improvements in social cognition and quality of life (Anagnostou et al., 2012) have been reported in adult ASD patients after OT nasal sprays. These findings provide evidence that OT may have the potential to improve human social cognition and behavior and to partially restore the damaged social cognitive neurological functions (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011; Striepens et al., 2011). Nevertheless, recent double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled studies in ASD individuals did not disclose significant clinical efficacies of intranasal OT on social cognition or behavior. For example, compared to the placebo group, Dadds et al. (2014) demonstrated that the intranasal OT treatment for male youths with ASD did not bring significant improvements on social interaction skills, emotion recognition, or general behavioral adjustment. Guastella et al. (2015) also did not find the efficacy of OT nasal spray for youth with ASD on social responsiveness and social cognition. Moreover, Althaus et al. (2015) detected no significant treatment effects of OT nasal spray for male adults on empathic arousal responses to affective social pictures. Therefore, it is essential to discover the real relationship between the oxytocinergic system and ASD.

Experiments have been designed to measure plasma OT levels in individuals with ASD to compute pertinent correlations. Relative to typically developing (TD) controls, several groups have reported lower plasma OT levels in individuals with ASD (Modahl et al., 1998; Green et al., 2001; Al-Ayadhi, 2005; Andari et al., 2010). However, there are also studies demonstrating elevated plasma OT levels in ASD participants (Jansen et al., 2006; Jacobson et al., 2014). Furthermore, a third group of studies reported no differences in plasma OT levels between ASD cases and unaffected controls (Miller et al., 2013; Parker et al., 2014; Taurines et al., 2014). There still remains questions that whether low or high plasma OT levels act as a biomarker of ASD and/or social deficits.

The OXTR gene is located on chromosome 3p25, spans 17 kb and includes four exons and three introns (Inoue et al., 1994). It encodes a member of the class I family of G protein-coupled receptors, which are characterized by seven transmembrane domains. Oxytocin receptors are concentrated in brain regions involved in social behavior, including the olfactory bulbs, pyriform cortex, amygdala, and lateral septum (Ferguson et al., 2000). Genome-wide linkage studies (McCauley et al., 2005; Lauritsen et al., 2006) have suggested that the OXTR gene is a plausible candidate gene for ASD. In addition, polymorphisms in the OXTR gene may affect human social cognition and attachment (Chen et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2012) and abnormalities in the OXTR gene have been proposed to cause social disorders in individuals with ASD (Choleris et al., 2007; Lerer et al., 2008). The association between OXTR gene polymorphisms and the etiology of ASD has drawn massive attention. At present, a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the third intron, rs2254298, has been identified as a particularly promising candidate to explain differences in oxytocinergic functioning (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011), especially in social functioning (Costa et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011; Thompson et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2012). Comparative genetics imply that a mutation replacing guanine (G) by adenine (A) of OXTR SNP rs2254298 has occurred at some point during human evolution. In European populations, the vast majority of individuals are homozygous for the G allele, but in Asian populations, the frequency of GG genotype is around just 40–50% compared with the frequencies of AA and AG genotypes (Chelala et al., 2009). Reasons for the selection of SNP rs2254298 in our study were on the basis of its special function and the inconsistent conclusions of aforetime studies involving ethnically distinct study populations: a Chinese Han population (Wu et al., 2005), a Caucasian population in the United States (Jacob et al., 2007), Swiss (Nyffeler et al., 2014), and northern European populations (Di Napoli et al., 2014), a Japanese population (Liu et al., 2010), an Israeli population (Lerer et al., 2008) and a German population (Wermter et al., 2010). For instance, studies on Chinese (Wu et al., 2005) and Japanese (Liu et al., 2010) populations discovered that the “A” allele of rs2254298 was associated with ASD. However, Jacob et al. (2007) found the “G” allele of rs2254298 as a risk allele in Caucasian ASD population. These results need further studies to clarify the role of OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism in ASD.

Although recent evidence supporting the essential role of OT and the OXTR gene in social functioning in ASD individuals has been reported, no consensus has been made on the associations among OT levels, the OXTR SNP rs2254298 and ASD. In order to clarify whether significant differences of serum OT levels and OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism would exhibit between ASD and TD groups, as well as seek out the interaction of serum OT levels and OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and their impacts on social deficits of ASD subjects in Chinese Han children and adolescents, the present case-control study tested serum OT levels using ELISA, detected the presence of the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism using PCR-RFLP and measured the ASD social phenotypes using the ABC and CARS scales in ASD subjects and TD controls of both genders. We analyzed the differences of serum OT levels between the ASD and TD groups, the correlations between serum OT levels and age, intelligent quotient (IQ), the ABC scores and the CARS scores. Moreover, the association between the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and ASD, as well as the relationships between OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes and serum OT levels, the ABC scores and the CARS scores were also analyzed. Finally, we hypothesized that Chinese Han children and adolescents with ASD may exhibit a dysregulation in OT on the basis of changes in OXTR gene expression as well as environmentally induced alterations of the oxytocinergic system to determine their social deficits.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study was approved by the Tianjin Medical University Institutional Review Board and written informed consent was obtained from school principals, parents and/or caregivers after a clear elaboration of detailed information. We recruited Chinese Han children and adolescents with ASD from the local special education schools and Chinese Han TD controls from the local mainstream schools in Tianjin. Serum OT levels were measured by ELISA in 55 ASD subjects (43 males and 12 females) and 110 age- and gender-matched TD controls (86 males and 24 females) between 2 and 17 years of age. The OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism was genotyped by PCR-RFLP in 100 ASD subjects (80 males and 20 females) and 232 age- and gender-matched TD controls (186 males and 46 females) between 2 and 18 years of age. Participants for ELISA (ASD group, n = 55; TD group, n = 110) were a subset of participants for PCR-RFLP (ASD group, n = 100; TD group, n = 232). ASD subjects were diagnosed by qualified and experienced psychologists, psychiatrists or neurologists according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1996) in the major hospitals of Tianjin and Beijing, which was conformed with the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) (Schopler et al., 1980) by clinical pediatric psychiatrists in Tianjin. A CARS total score of ≥30 is the cutoff for distinguishing children “at risk” for autism from pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) (Schopler et al., 1988; Chlebowski et al., 2010), and a CARS total score of ≥25.5 is indicative of ASD (Chlebowski et al., 2010). Participant characteristics are reported in Table 1. The Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC) was also administered for the assessment of their autistic symptoms (Rellini et al., 2004). Moreover, intelligent quotient (IQ) was determined using the Chinese Binet Scale (Binet) (Wu, 1982) by clinical pediatric psychologists in Tianjin. ASD subjects were included with a full-scale IQ of ≥30 and TD controls were included with an IQ in the average range. Moreover, 6 ASD subjects for ELISA and 9 ASD subjects for PCR-RFLP had an IQ of >70. In the present study, none of the ASD subjects fulfilled a DSM-IV diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 1996) of any mental disorders (schizophrenia, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, depressive disorder, and bipolar disorder), as well as suffered from epileptic seizures and fragile X syndrome on the basis of medical history reported by parents. TD controls were required to be free of psychiatric, neurological or physical disorders in the present or past on the basis of medical history reported by parents and with a score of <7 on the Clancy Autism Behavior Scale (CABS) (Clancy et al., 1969). To ensure the robustness of the experiments, none of the participants in any group were taking special drugs, such as psychotropic or immune-altering medications.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

Assessment of Autistic Symptoms

Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC)

The Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC) is a behavior checklist that consists of 57 items in 5 categories: “sensory”, “relating”, “stereotypes and object use”, “language”, and “social and self-help”. Each item corresponds to a single score referring to a single symptomatological area (Rellini et al., 2004). The scale utilizes an observer's rating of a series of typical autistic behaviors in a certain subject and provides advice for educational intervention.

Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)

The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) is a 15-item behavior rating scale that includes 14 domains plus one category of general impressions that are rated on a four-point scale (1 = appropriate for age; 2 = mildly abnormal; 3 = moderately abnormal; 4 = severely abnormal) according to interaction and observation. The 15 items are as follows: “relating to people”, “imitative behavior”, “emotional response”, “body use”, “object use”, “adaptation to change”, “visual response”, “listening response”, “perceptive response”, “fear or anxiety”, “verbal communication”, “non-verbal communication”, “activity level”, “level and consistency of intellective relations”, and “general impressions” (Rellini et al., 2004). The CARS is widely used by psychiatrists to identify children with autism and as a further measurement of the severity of this disease. Total scores can range from a low of 15 to a high of 60; scores of less than 30 indicate that the individual is in the non-autistic range, scores between 30 and 36.5 indicate mild/moderate autism, and scores from 37 to 60 indicate severe autism (Schopler et al., 1988).

Sample Collection

Participants were previously informed to fast in the morning before sampling to avoid the influence of food and/or drink.

Five milliliters of venous blood were collected from each participant's antecubital region by trained and qualified nurses between 8:00 and 9:30 a.m. in pro-coagulation tubes with yellow caps (Becton, Dickinson and Company). The tubes were gently inverted five times immediately and left undisturbed at room temperature for 30 min. Then, they were kept on ice until centrifuging at 1,731 × g for 10 min at 4°C. Serum was isolated and divided into 1-ml aliquots and immediately stored at −80°C until assay.

DNA samples were obtained by an experienced technician by taking a sterile swab and rubbing it against the inside of participants' cheeks and placing it into a labeled sterile 2-ml Falcon tube.

DNA Extraction

DNA was extracted from the swabs according to the manufacturer's protocol for the Swab Gen DNA Kit (CW0530, CWBIO, Beijing, China). The DNA solution for the current experiment was then stored at 4°C, and the redundant stock was stored at −20°C. The concentration of each DNA sample was determined using a NanoDrop ND-2000 spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific).

Oxytocin Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay (ELISA)

Serum sample extraction procedures were previously carried out to decrease the potential cross-reacting molecules. A 1:1 mixture of serum sample and 0.1% trifluoroacetic acid in water (TFA-H2O) was centrifuged at 17,000 × g for 15 min at 4°C. Supernatants were applied to the Sep-Pak C18 columns (Waters Corporation Milford, Massachusetts USA) and eluted off with a solution (acetonitrile/0.1% TFA-H2O ratio 60:40). Samples was evaporated at 4°C and stored at −20°C until reconstitution. Serum OT levels were measured using commercially available ELISA Kits (O30152-09, Beijing Biotopped Science & Technology CO., Ltd., China), all steps in the manufacturer's instructions were followed, and results were read under 450 nm wavelength on a Power Wave-I microplate spectrophotometer (Sunrise-Basic BioTek, Tecan). All assays were performed in duplicate. Standard curves and concentrations were computed by Excel. The assay range was from 2 pg/ml to 600 pg/ml. The intra-assay coefficient of variation was 6.9%, and the inter-assay coefficient of variation was 11.8%.

Genotyping

The OXTR SNP rs2254298 was amplified by using polymerase chain reaction-restriction fragment length polymorphism (PCR-RFLP) analysis. The primer sequences were as follows: 5′-TGA AAG CAG AGG TTG TGT GGA CAG G-3′ and 5′-AAC GCC CAC CCC AGT TTC TTC-3′ (Wu et al., 2005). PCR was performed on a C1000 Thermal Cycler (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA) in a 20-μl volume consisting of 0.1 μl Taq (Takara, Code. #DR001A), 2 μl 10 × PCR buffer (with Mg2+), 1.6 μl dNTP Mixture (2.5 mM), 1 μl each of the forward and reverse primers (10 μM/L), 3 μl genomic DNA, and 11.3 μl ddH2O. The PCR process included an initial denaturation step at 95°C for 5 min; 35 cycles of denaturation at 95°C for 30 s, annealing at 64.5°C for 30 s and extension at 72°C for 1 min; followed by a final extension for 7 min at 72°C. PCR products that were 307 bp long were visualized on a 2% agarose gel after electrophoresis. After the PCR products were digested at 65°C for 20 min with 0.5 μl of the restriction enzyme BsrI (New England Biolabs, Cat. #R0527), the samples were run at 80 V for 90 min on an 8% polyacrylamide gel (acrylamide/bis-acrylamide ratio 19:1) and were visualized to distinguish the G allele (bands of 164, 101, 34, and 8 bp) from the A allele (bands of 164, 135, and 8 bp). To ensure the accuracy of the RFLP profiles, gene sequencing was performed by Genewiz Inc, and the sequencing results agreed with the PCR-RFLP results.

Statistical Analysis

The results were analyzed using SPSS17.0 software (IBM Corporation New York, USA) and SAS version 9.3.2 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC). Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was performed to determine the normal distribution of the data. Levene's test was utilized to determine the homogeneity of variance. Differences in serum OT levels between two groups were examined with a Mann-Whitney U test and spearman rank-order correlations (including spearman partial correlation) were used to evaluate strength of associations. The Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium for the genotype frequency distributions of rs2254298 was assessed using a χ2 goodness-of-fit test. A χ2-test was also used in analyzing the genotype and allele frequencies, with a significance level of α = 0.05. A one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with sex, age, and IQ as covariates was applied for comparisons of ABC scores among three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes. Kruskal-Wallis H tests were utilized for comparisons of serum OT levels and CARS total scores among three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes. A ordinal polytomous logistic regression analysis was performed to discover the differences of the scores of each item in the CARS among three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes (while adjusting for sex, age, and IQ). Odds ratios (ORs) were expressed with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI). Post-hoc Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was performed. A p < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Serum Oxytocin (OT) Levels

A Mann-Whitney U test was used to assess the differences between ASD subjects and TD controls. Serum OT levels was significantly higher in the ASD group compared with the TD group (Z = −2.178, p = 0.029). However, gender had no significant effect on serum OT levels in any comparison, between the two groups (males Z = −1.694, p = 0.090; females Z = −1.309, p = 0.191) and between males and females within the ASD group (Z = −1.100, p = 0.271) and within the TD group (Z = −1.230, p = 0.219) (Figure 1). Correlations between serum OT levels and age, as well as IQ are presented in Table 2 with unadjusted and adjusted spearman rank-order correlations. The positive correlations between serum OT levels and age of all ASD subjects, as well as ASD males vanished after Spearman partial correlation. No associations were found between serum OT levels and IQ with both analyses in ASD and TD groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Vertical bar charts of serum oxytocin levels across diagnosis (ASD compared with TD) and genders (males compared with females) with median (IQR). Error bars show interquartile range (IQR). Group: n = 55 ASD and n = 110 TD. Male: n = 43 ASD and n = 86 TD. Female: n = 12 ASD and n = 24 TD. The ASD group exhibited significantly higher serum oxytocin levels than the TD group.*p < 0.05. ASD, autism spectrum disorders; IQR, interquartile range; TD, typically developing.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between serum OT levels and age, as well as IQ in ASD and TD groups.

Oxytocin Receptor (OXTR) Gene rs2254298 Polymorphism

The distribution of the OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes was consistent with the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium for both groups (χ2 = 0.007, df = 2, p = 0.996 in ASD subjects; χ2 = 0.292, df = 2, p = 0.864 in TD controls). No statistical differences were found from the genotypic and allelic frequencies of the OXTR SNP rs2254298 in ASD subjects and TD controls (p > 0.05) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Participant genotypic and allelic frequencies of the OXTR SNP rs2254298 by group.

Serum OT Levels and OXTR SNP rs2254298 Genotypes

Both blood and DNA samples were available for 55 ASD subjects and 110 TD controls. A Kruskal-Wallis H test detected differences among the three genotypes in the ASD group (χ2 = 6.012, df = 2, p = 0.049) (Figure 2), but a Mann-Whitney U test with Bonferroni correction identified no evidence for significant associations between AA and AG genotypes (p = 0.080), AA and GG genotypes (p = 0.095), as well as AG and GG genotypes (p = 1.000).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Vertical bar charts of serum oxytocin levels for the three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes in both ASD and TD groups are presented as median (IQR). Error bars show interquartile range (IQR). ASD group: n = 9 AA, n = 21 AG, and n = 25 GG. TD group: n = 9 AA, n = 52 AG, and n = 49 GG. The ASD subjects exhibited a significant association between serum oxytocin levels and three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes, and serum oxytocin levels were low in ASD subjects with an AA genotype. *p < 0.05. ASD, autism spectrum disorders; IQR, interquartile range; OXTR, oxytocin receptor; SNP, single-nucleotide polymorphism; TD, typically developing.

Correlations between Serum OT Levels and ASD Social Phenotypes

ABC scores and CARS scores were only determined for ASD subjects. Spearman correlation and Spearman partial correlation (adjusted for sex, age, and IQ) were both applied for all scores of the ABC and CARS. We found a significant positive correlation between serum OT levels and CARS total scores (Spearman correlation: r = 0.488, p = 0.000; Spearman partial correlation: r = 0.465, p = 0.001), suggestive of more social deficits with high serum OT levels. Moreover, serum OT levels also correlated positively with the “adaptation to change score” in the CARS (Spearman correlation: r = 0.352, p = 0.008; Spearman partial correlation: r = 0.296, p = 0.033), indicating that ASD subjects with higher serum OT levels were accompanied by worse abilities in adapting to novel environment. There were no significant corrections between serum OT levels and scores of other items in the CARS and ABC.

OXTR SNP rs2254298 Genotypes and ASD Social Phenotypes

All scores for the 5 ABC categories conformed to a normal distribution and homogeneity of variance (p > 0.05) in two groups, including “ASD subjects for both OT and OXTR” (n = 55) and “ASD subjects for OXTR” (n = 100). In both groups, a one-way covariance (ANCOVA) with sex, age, and IQ as covariates indicated a significant relationship between genotypes and the “stereotypes and object use score” of the ABC [F(2, 52) = 5.178, p = 0.006, n = 55; F(2, 97) = 11.022, p = 0.000, n = 100]. Further the Bonferroni post-hoc test revealed significant differences between the AA and AG genotypes in both ASD groups with different sample sizes (“ASD subjects for both OT and OXTR” group: p = 0.005, n = 30; “ASD subjects for OXTR” group: p = 0.000, n = 56) as well as between the AA and GG genotypes in “ASD subjects for OXTR” group (p = 0.001, n = 58) (Figure 3). The score of subjects with an AA genotype was higher than that of subjects with AG or GG genotypes, indicating that the participants with an AA genotype exhibited worse behavioral deficits in stereotyped behavior and impaired ability of using objects.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Vertical bar charts of ABC subscale score among the three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes are presented as mean ± SEM (adjusted for sex, age and IQ) in two groups. Error bars show standard error of the mean (SEM). Group1 (ASD subjects for both OXT and OXTR): n = 9 AA, n = 21 AG, and n = 25 GG. Group 2 (ASD subjects for OXTR): n = 14 AA, n = 42 AG, and n = 44 GG. ASD subjects with an AA genotype exhibited a higher score than those with an AG genotype or a GG genotype. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ABC, Autism Behavior Checklist; ASD, autism spectrum disorders; OXT, oxytocin; OXTR, oxytocin receptor; SEM, standard error of the mean; SNP, single-nucleotide polymorphism.

A Kruskal-Wallis H test followed by Bonferroni-corrected Mann-Whitney U test was applied to make the comparisons of CARS total scores among three genotypes of OXTR SNP rs2254298, but no differences were found (p > 0.05). Moreover, a ordinal polytomous logistic regression analysis with post-hoc Bonferroni correction was used to analyze differences of scores of each item in the CARS among OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes (while adjusting for sex, age, and IQ). Significant associations of “adaptation to change score” in the CARS among three genotypes were found in ASD subjects. ORs (95% CI) for “4 = severely abnormal” of “adaptation to change” among three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes in “ASD subjects for both OT and OXTR” (n = 55) and “ASD subjects for OXTR” (n = 100) groups were shown in Table 4. ASD subjects with an AA genotype acted as a protective genotype for suffering from the most severe abnormality of “adaptation to change,” indicating that ASD subjects with AG and GG genotypes tended to perform worse social cognition in novel environment.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Odds ratios (ORs) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of having severe abnormality of 15 items in the CARS among three OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes in two ASD groups.

Discussion

Animal and human studies, including studies of individuals with ASD, have provided evidence for the essential role of OT and the OXTR gene in social functioning. In the present study, we tested serum OT levels of both genders and elevated serum OT levels were observed in the ASD group compared to the TD group. Serum OT levels correlated positively with the “adaptation to change score” and “total scores” of the CARS. No associations were found between serum OT levels and age, as well as IQ in both ASD and TD groups. In addition, we made a comparison of the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism in both groups and observed no statistical significance. However, significant associations were detected between OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes and serum OT levels, the “stereotypes and object use score” of the ABC and the “adaptation to change score” of the CARS in the ASD subjects.

Our finding of higher serum OT levels in ASD participants was similar to several previous studies on plasma OT levels in individuals with ASD. For example, Jansen et al. (2006) observed increased plasma OT levels in a group of male and female ASD adults, and explained that developmental, intellectual, or other factors as well as a physiological reaction on negative moods and stress may relate to the higher OT plasma levels in ASD participants. Thus far, evidence has suggested elevated peripheral OT levels in humans and animals in connection with depression, anxiety and social stress (Grippo et al., 2007; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2011; Miller et al., 2013; Weisman et al., 2013) as well as a relationship between OT levels and altered energetic stress coping approaches and anxiolytic influences (Insel, 2010; Neumann and Landgraf, 2012; Lukas and Neumann, 2013). Exposure to unfamiliar environments is well-known to be stressful for individuals with ASD, which can trigger a stress response to release central OT to counter negative emotions (Gimpl and Fahrenholz, 2001). We could not find the correlations between serum OT levels and age in the ASD group (including males and females) after adjusting for IQ, which predicted that the intellectual factors might be restrictive factors for serum OT levels and developmental stage. However, we detected higher serum OT levels were associated with worse ability in adapting to change and greater social deficits (as evidenced by high CARS total scores) in ASD subjects, predicting impairments in social functioning may lead them to release more OT to withstand different novel environments. Furthermore, Jacobson et al. (2014) reported that prepubertal male children with ASD exhibit elevated plasma OT levels and increased G protein subunit expression relative to TD controls and higher oxytocin levels associated with greater social impairment. We conjectured that a dysregulation in OT signaling pathways likely including a potential compensatory mechanism for the changes of OXTR gene expression may also lead to elevated OT levels in ASD subjects. That is why we designed experiments on both serum OT levels and an OXTR gene polymorphism to find latent disturbances in ASD individuals.

However, Miller et al. (2013) reported that girls exhibit higher plasma OT levels and elevated anxiety scores than boys within an autistic group instead of the difference between the cases and controls. Similarly, Taurines et al. (2014) also did not observe differences in plasma OT levels of children and adolescents between high-functioning male ASD subjects and healthy male controls, nor did Parker et al. (2014) among ASD participants, unaffected siblings and unrelated neurotypical control children of both genders. Moreover, four studies have exhibited lower plasma OT levels. Modahl et al. (1998) reported lower plasma OT levels in prepubertal boys with ASD relative to TD boys, but ASD participants with the highest OT levels were the most socially impaired. Moreover, they found a positive association between OT levels and age for normal children, which was opposite from our finding. Green et al. (2001) observed a decrease in OT and an increase in unprocessed OT peptides in male autistic children, relative to controls. Andari et al. (2010) elucidated lower plasma OT levels in young adults with high-functioning autism (HFA) or Asperger Syndrome, and Al-Ayadhi (2005) reported lower plasma levels of OT in children and adolescents with ASD.

Overall, a substantial body of research has reported inconsistent outcomes in peripheral OT levels, which could be the result of differences among participants and investigative methods, including the multiple demographic characteristics of the ASD population (age, gender, intelligence, ethnicity, comorbidity, medication history, sample size, diagnostic criteria, etc.), the blood sample collection time and the measurement and analysis of OT levels. Decreased OT levels in ASD might be a manifestation of the original genetic insult of ASD, and elevated OT levels in ASD might reflect a physiological response to ameliorate negative symptoms such as anxiety (Taurines et al., 2014) and be consistent with some degree of a dysregulation in OT peptide processing pathways (Green et al., 2001). Because plasma OT concentrations are highly heritable (Parker et al., 2014), family effects should be taken into consideration in subsequent studies.

With the aim of establishing the role of the OXTR gene in ASD, we chose to study a particularly promising candidate: OXTR SNP rs2254298. Many studies have reported that OXTR SNP rs2254298 affects various psychological dimensions and neurological diseases, such as affect, temperament, genetic variation, attachment and social cognition. However, there is extant literature failing to associate the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism with ASD consistently.

The first association study examining 4 SNPs in 195 Chinese Han autism trios with a family-based association test (FBAT) revealed a significant genetic association with rs2254298A and rs53576A (Wu et al., 2005). A Japanese study only observed a significant difference in rs2254298A between the ASD and normal groups in a population-based case-control test but not in an FBAT (Liu et al., 2010). A study of 57 American Caucasians attempted to replicate the Asian studies but only detected an association with rs2254298G (Jacob et al., 2007). Four studies did not identify any direct associations between the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and ASD. A comprehensive study of 18 tagged SNPs across the entire OXTR gene region in 152 Israeli autistic subjects was undertaken using HapMap data, the Haploview algorithm, a case-control test, and a FBAT. A five-locus haplotype block (rs237897-rs13316193-rs237889-rs2254298-rs2268494) was significantly associated with ASD, but SNP rs2254298 in the case-control test did not reveal any association with ASD except for the relationship with “daily living skills” and “communication” scores of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Lerer et al., 2008). A study in northern European Caucasians reported the association of Asperger Syndrome with multiple haplotypes that included rs2254298, but no association between rs2254298 alone with Asperger Syndrome was observed (Di Napoli et al., 2014). Another study on HFA in Swiss Caucasians between 5 and 17 years of age confirmed no association between rs2254298 and ASD (Nyffeler et al., 2014); this finding was similar to the finding in a family-based single-marker and haplotype association study with 22 SNPs in 100 German families with a high-functioning autistic proband (Wermter et al., 2010).

Our study did not detect any association between rs2254298 and ASD, which was similar to the conclusion of a meta-analysis: a solid role in autism has not been established for OXTR rs2254298 (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn, 2014). However, after the analyses of the genotypes among serum OT levels and the scores for ABC and CARS, some interesting associations were detected in the present study. A significant association between serum OT levels and OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism was observed in the ASD group, and the serum OT levels of an AA genotype showed the potential to be lower than the AG and GG genotypes, which suggested that a dysfunction of OXTR SNP rs2254298 in ASD subjects may somewhat impact OT levels in OXTR-OT signaling pathways. The score for “stereotypes and object use” in the ABC revealed significant differences among the three genotypes in two groups with different sample sizes. The severity of behavioral deficits in ASD participants with an AA genotype was significantly greater than the AG genotype in “ASD subjects for both OT and OXTR” (n = 55) group, and the severity of behavioral deficits in ASD participants with an AA genotype was significantly greater than the AG and GG genotypes in “ASD subjects for OXTR” (n = 100) group. Although no evidence for a direct association between serum OT levels and “stereotypes and object use score,” we found ASD subjects with AG and GG genotypes tend to have higher OT levels and lesser behavioral impairments (including restricted and repetitive behaviors). These results appeared to be consistent with the reduced repetitive behaviors after intranasal OT infusion (Hollander et al., 2003). Furthermore, the score of “adaptation to change” in the CARS was also found to be statistically significant in two groups with different sample sizes, and ASD subjects with an AA genotype were better able to adapt to change than those with AG and GG genotypes. Along with a significant correction between serum OT levels and “adaptation to change score,” we also found ASD subjects with an AA genotype tend to show lower OT levels and lesser social cognitive impairment compared to those with AG and GG genotypes. This result might stem from the different reactions to different novel environments along with different severity of social cognitive deficits (Tops et al., 2013). Above all, we found the ASD subjects with an AA genotype have the better ability to adapt to change, as a result, they might produce less OT to deal with different novel environments. However, elevated OT levels can reduce repetitive behaviors in ASD patients (Hollander et al., 2003), and this may be the reason for higher score of “stereotypes and object use” in ASD subjects with an AA genotype. On the whole, we speculate that differences in serum OT levels and social deficits among OXTR SNP rs2254298 genotypes likely involve changes at the OXTR expression as well as environmentally induced alterations of the basal oxytocinergic system in ASD.

The first strength of our study lies in the combination of the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism with serum OT levels in both ASD and TD groups as well as the effect of the OXTR SNP rs2254298 on ASD social phenotypes. Our results could to some extent explain the potential role of OT in the impairment of social and behavioral skills in ASD (Bartz and Hollander, 2008) and provide ideas to explain the opposite emotion recognition performance after OT administration between different OXTR haplotypes (Chen et al., 2015). Secondly, both age- and gender-matched males and females participated in the study, which extended the findings of most previous studies that involved only males. Thirdly, a standardized protocol for blood and DNA collection with fasting in the morning was used to preclude potential circadian and contaminating effects on serum OT levels and DNA quality.

There are three limitations of our study. First, we chose serum rather than CSF as the test sample, and serum OT levels can not completely reflect OT actions in the central nervous system because the relationship between peripheral and central levels of OT is unclear. Second, we only got informed consent from 55 ASD subjects and 110 TD controls to measure both serum OT levels and the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism, which needs to expand the sample size in further studies. Finally, the PLINK program was not performed in our study, we had to calculate the allele frequencies by hand and then do case-control association analysis by a χ2-test using SPSS. Our findings will need to be replicated in East Asian samples and very large samples will be required to examine these associations in European samples.

In summary, to the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to test serum OT levels in ASD participants and analyze the association between the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and serum OT levels, the ABC scores and the CARS scores. ASD subjects exhibited elevated serum OT levels and a latent association among OT levels, the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and ASD social deficits existed in ASD group. Although we did not detect any direct association between the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism and ASD, between serum OT levels and age, as well as and between serum OT levels and IQ, we could indentify some clues to indicate social deficits in ASD individuals with OT levels and the OXTR rs2254298 polymorphism. Subsequent prospective studies should focus on larger, well-described populations of different races to elucidate the true effects of the OT and OXTR gene on ASD under the considerations of genetic, environmental and developmental factors, then design a reasonable therapeutic schedule for the oxytocinergic system to improve their social deficits.

Author Contributions

SY contributed to the sample collection, laboratory experiments, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data, and preparation of the first draft of manuscript. XD, XG, and YH contributed to sample collection, laboratory experiments, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data. HS and LG contributed to acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data, and revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. WD and YS contributed to the conception and design of the study, sample collection, analysis and interpretation of data, and revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. XZ contributed to the conception and design of the study, sample collection, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data, and review and revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No.81072313).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the subjects, their schools and families for their support and participation.

References

Al-Ayadhi, L. Y. (2005). Altered oxytocin and vasopressin levels in autistic children in Central Saudi Arabia. Neurosciences 10, 47–50.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Althaus, M., Groen, Y., Wijers, A. A., Noltes, H., Tucha, O., and Hoekstra, P. J. (2015). Oxytocin enhances orienting to social information in a selective group of high-functioning male adults with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 79, 53–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1996). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-V. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Association.

Anagnostou, E., Soorya, L., Chaplin, W., Bartz, J., Halpern, D., Wasserman, S., et al. (2012). Intranasal oxytocin versus placebo in the treatment of adults with autism spectrum disorders: a randomized controlled trial. Mol. Autism 3:16. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-3-16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andari, E., Duhamel, J. R., Zalla, T., Herbrecht, E., Leboyer, M., and Sirigu, A. (2010). Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 4389–4394. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910249107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2014). A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatr. Genet. 24, 45–51. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283643684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., and Hollander, E. (2008). Oxytocin and experimental therapeutics in autism spectrum disorders. Prog. Brain Res. 170, 451–462. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00435-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CDC (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years – autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 63, 1–21.

Chelala, C., Khan, A., and Lemoine, N. R. (2009). SNPnexus: a web database for functional annotation of newly discovered and public domain single nucleotide polymorphisms. Bioinformatics 25, 655–661. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btn653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. S., Barth, M. E., Johnson, S. L., Gotlib, I. H., and Johnson, S. C. (2011). Oxytocin Receptor (OXTR) polymorphisms and attachment in human infants. Front. Psychol. 2:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. S., Kumsta, R., Dvorak, F., Domes, G., Yim, O. S., Ebstein, R. P., et al. (2015). Genetic modulation of oxytocin sensitivity: a pharmacogenetic approach. Transl. Psychiatry 5:e664. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chlebowski, C., Green, J. A., Barton, M. L., and Fein, D. (2010). Using the childhood autism rating scale to diagnose autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 787–799. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0926-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choleris, E., Little, S. R., Mong, J. A., Puram, S. V., Langer, R., and Pfaff, D. W. (2007). Microparticle-based delivery of oxytocin receptor antisense DNA in the medial amygdala blocks social recognition in female mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 4670–4675. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700670104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clancy, H., Dugdale, A., and Rendle-Short, J. (1969). The diagnosis of infantile autism. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 11, 432–442. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1969.tb01461.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, B., Pini, S., Gabelloni, P., Abelli, M., Lari, L., Cardini, A., et al. (2009). Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1506–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dadds, M. R., MacDonald, E., Cauchi, A., Williams, K., Levy, F., and Brennan, J. (2014). Nasal oxytocin for social deficits in childhood autism: a randomized controlled trial. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 521–531. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1899-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Napoli, A., Warrier, V., Baron-Cohen, S., and Chakrabarti, B. (2014). Genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene is associated with Asperger Syndrome. Mol. Autism 5:48. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donaldson, Z. R., and Young, L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322, 900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, J. N., Young, L. J., Hearn, E. F., Matzuk, M. M., Insel, T. R., and Winslow, J. T. (2000). Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat. Genet. 25, 284–288. doi: 10.1038/77040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitag, C. M. (2011). Genetic risk in autism: new associations and clinical testing. Expert Opin. Med. Diagn. 5, 347–356. doi: 10.1517/17530059.2011.579101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gimpl, G., and Fahrenholz, F. (2001). The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol. Rev. 81, 629–683.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Green, L., Fein, D., Modahl, C., Feinstein, C., Waterhouse, L., and Morris, M. (2001). Oxytocin and autistic disorder: alterations in peptide forms. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 609–613. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01139-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grippo, A. J., Gerena, D., Huang, J., Kumar, N., Shah, M., Ughreja, R., et al. (2007). Social isolation induces behavioral and neuroendocrine disturbances relevant to depression in female and male prairie voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 966–980. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guastella, A. J., Einfeld, S. L., Gray, K. M., Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Lambert, T. J., et al. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 692–694. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guastella, A. J., Gray, K. M., Rinehart, N. J., Alvares, G. A., Tonge, B. J., Hickie, I. B., et al. (2015). The effects of a course of intranasal oxytocin on social behaviors in youth diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders: a randomized controlled trial. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 56, 444–452. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallmayer, J., Cleveland, S., Torres, A., Phillips, J., Cohen, B., Torigoe, T., et al. (2011). Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 1095–1102. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollander, E., Bartz, J., Chaplin, W., Phillips, A., Sumner, J., Soorya, L., et al. (2007). Oxytocin increases retention of social cognition in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 498–503. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollander, E., Novotny, S., Hanratty, M., Yaffe, R., DeCaria, C. M., Aronowitz, B. R., et al. (2003). Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with autistic and Asperger's disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 193–198. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Birmingham, W., and Light, K. C. (2011). The influence of depressive symptomatology and perceived stress on plasma and salivary oxytocin before, during and after a support enhancement intervention. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 1249–1256. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inoue, T., Kimura, T., Azuma, C., Inazawa, J., Takemura, M., Kikuchi, T., et al. (1994). Structural organization of the human oxytocin receptor gene. J. Biol. Chem. 269, 32451–32456.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R. (2010). The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron 65, 768–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., and Fernald, R. D. (2004). How the brain processes social information: searching for the social brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 697–722. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., O'Brien, D. J., and Leckman, J. F. (1999). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: is there a connection? Biol. Psychiatry 45, 145–157. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00142-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, S., Brune, C. W., Carter, C. S., Leventhal, B. L., Lord, C., and Cook, E. H. Jr. (2007). Association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism. Neurosci. Lett. 417, 6–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, J. D., Ellerbeck, K. A., Kelly, K. A., Fleming, K. K., Jamison, T. R., Coffey, C. W., et al. (2014). Evidence for alterations in stimulatory G proteins and oxytocin levels in children with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 40, 159–169. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, L. M., Gispen-de Wied, C. C., Wiegant, V. M., Westenberg, H. G., Lahuis, B. E., and van Engeland, H. (2006). Autonomic and neuroendocrine responses to a psychosocial stressor in adults with autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 891–899. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0124-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., and Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435, 673–676. doi: 10.1038/nature03701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauritsen, M. B., Als, T. D., Dahl, H. A., Flint, T. J., Wang, A. G., Vang, M., et al. (2006). A genome-wide search for alleles and haplotypes associated with autism and related pervasive developmental disorders on the Faroe Islands. Mol. Psychiatry 11, 37–46. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. J., Macbeth, A. H., Pagani, J. H., and Young, W. S. III. (2009). Oxytocin: the great facilitator of life. Prog. Neurobiol. 88, 127–151. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerer, E., Levi, S., Salomon, S., Darvasi, A., Yirmiya, N., and Ebstein, R. P. (2008). Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship to Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and cognition. Mol. Psychiatry 13, 980–988. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Kawamura, Y., Shimada, T., Otowa, T., Koishi, S., Sugiyama, T., et al. (2010). Association of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the Japanese population. J. Hum. Genet. 55, 137–141. doi: 10.1038/jhg.2009.140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukas, M., and Neumann, I. D. (2013). Oxytocin and vasopressin in rodent behaviors related to social dysfunctions in autism spectrum disorders. Behav. Brain Res. 251, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCauley, J. L., Li, C., Jiang, L., Olson, L. M., Crockett, G., Gainer, K., et al. (2005). Genome-wide and Ordered-Subset linkage analyses provide support for autism loci on 17q and 19p with evidence of phenotypic and interlocus genetic correlates. BMC Med. Genet. 6:1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2350-6-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Domes, G., Kirsch, P., and Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 524–538. doi: 10.1038/nrn3044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M., Bales, K. L., Taylor, S. L., Yoon, J., Hostetler, C. M., Carter, C. S., et al. (2013). Oxytocin and vasopressin in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: sex differences and associations with symptoms. Autism Res. 6, 91–102. doi: 10.1002/aur.1270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modahl, C., Green, L., Fein, D., Morris, M., Waterhouse, L., Feinstein, C., et al. (1998). Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol. Psychiatry 43, 270–277. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00439-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, I. D., and Landgraf, R. (2012). Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 35, 649–659. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyffeler, J., Walitza, S., Bobrowski, E., Gundelfinger, R., and Grünblatt, E. (2014). Association study in siblings and case-controls of serotonin- and oxytocin-related genes with high functioning autism. J. Mol. Psychiatry 2:1. doi: 10.1186/2049-9256-2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, K. J., Garner, J. P., Libove, R. A., Hyde, S. A., Hornbeak, K. B., Carson, D. S., et al. (2014). Plasma oxytocin concentrations and OXTR polymorphisms predict social impairments in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 12258–12263. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402236111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rellini, E., Tortolani, D., Trillo, S., Carbone, S., and Montecchi, F. (2004). Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) and Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC) correspondence and conflicts with DSM-IV criteria in diagnosis of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 703–708. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-5290-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E. B., Koenen, K. C., McCormick, M. C., Munir, K., Hallett, V., Happé, F., et al. (2012). A multivariate twin study of autistic traits in 12-year-olds: testing the fractionable autism triad hypothesis. Behav. Genet. 42, 245–255. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9500-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ronald, A., and Hoekstra, R. A. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 156B, 255–274. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., DeVellis, R. F., and Daly, K. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 10, 91–103. doi: 10.1007/BF02408436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schopler, E., Reichler, R., and Rochen Renner, B. (1988). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Striepens, N., Kendrick, K. M., Maier, W., and Hurlemann, R. (2011). Prosocial effects of oxytocin and clinical evidence for its therapeutic potential. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 32, 426–450. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taurines, R., Schwenck, C., Lyttwin, B., Schecklmann, M., Jans, T., Reefschläger, L., et al. (2014). Oxytocin plasma concentrations in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: correlation with autistic symptomatology. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 6, 231–239. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0145-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. J., Parker, K. J., Hallmayer, J. F., Waugh, C. E., and Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs2254298) interacts with familial risk for psychopathology to predict symptoms of depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 144–147. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., Huffmeijer, R., Linting, M., Grewen, K. M., Light, K. C., Koole, S. L., et al. (2013). The role of oxytocin in familiarization-habituation responses to social novelty. Front. Psychol. 4:761. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisman, O., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Schneiderman, I., Gordon, I., and Feldman, R. (2013). Plasma oxytocin distributions in a large cohort of women and men and their gender-specific associations with anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 694–701. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wermter, A. K., Kamp-Becker, I., Hesse, P., Schulte-Körne, G., Strauch, K., and Remschmidt, H. (2010). Evidence for the involvement of genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in the etiology of autistic disorders on high-functioning level. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 153B, 629–639. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, N., Li, Z., and Su, Y. (2012). The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. J. Affect. Disord. 138, 468–472. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, S., Jia, M., Ruan, Y., Liu, J., Guo, Y., Shuang, M., et al. (2005). Positive association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with autism in the Chinese Han population. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, T. M. (1982). The Guide to Chinese Binet Test, 3rd Edn (in Chinese). Beijing: Beijing University Press.

Yamasue, H. (2013). Function and structure in social brain regions can link oxytocin-receptor genes with autistic social behavior. Brain Dev. 35, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorders, oxytocin, oxytocin receptor gene, rs2254298, social deficits, children and adolescents

Citation: Yang S, Dong X, Guo X, Han Y, Song H, Gao L, Dai W, Su Y and Zhang X (2017) Serum Oxytocin Levels and an Oxytocin Receptor Gene Polymorphism (rs2254298) Indicate Social Deficits in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front. Neurosci. 11:221. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00221

Received: 13 September 2016; Accepted: 04 April 2017;
Published: 21 April 2017.

Edited by:

Ahmet O. Caglayan, Istanbul Bilim University, Turkey

Reviewed by:

Vivek Agarwal, King George's Medical University, India
Suma Jacob, University of Minnesota, USA

Copyright © 2017 Yang, Dong, Guo, Han, Song, Gao, Dai, Su and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xin Zhang, zhangxin@tmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.