Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 14 March 2019
Sec. Neuroenergetics and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Nutrients, Neurotransmitters and Brain Energetics View all 30 articles

Environmental Impoverishment, Aging, and Reduction in Mastication Affect Mouse Innate Repertoire to Explore Novel Environments and to Assess Risk

  • 1Laboratório de Investigações em Neurodegeneração e Infecção, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Hospital Universitário João de Barros Barreto, Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brazil
  • 2Curso de Medicina, Centro Universitário do Estado do Pará, Belém, Brazil

Studies indicate that inhibition of adequate masticatory function, due to soft diet, occlusal disharmony, or molar losses affects the cognitive behavior of rodents. However, no study has tested the effects on new environments exploration and risk assessment coupled with a combination of masticatory function rehabilitation and environmental enrichment. In the present report, we tested the hypothesis that age, environment, and masticatory changes may interact and alter exploratory patterns of locomotor activity and mice preferences in an open field (OF) arena. As OF arenas are widely used to measure anxiety-like behavior in rats and mice. We examined in an open arena, the exploratory and locomotor activities of mature (6-month-old; 6M), late mature (12-month-old; 12M), and aged (18-month-old; 18M) mice, subjected to distinct masticatory regimens and environments. Three different regimens of masticatory activity were used: continuous normal mastication with hard pellets (HD); normal mastication followed by reduced mastication with equal periods of pellets followed by soft powder – HD/SD; or rehabilitated masticatory activity with equal periods of HD, followed by powder, followed by pellets – HD/SD/HD). Under each diet regimen, half of the individuals were raised in standard cages [impoverished environment (IE)] and the other half in enriched cages [enriched environment (EE)]. Animals behavior on the open field (OF) task were recorded by webcam and analyzed with Any Maze software (Stöelting). The locomotor and exploratory activities in OF task declined with age, and this was particularly evident in 18M HD EE mice. Although all groups kept their preference by the peripheral zone, the outcomes were significantly influenced by interactions between environment, age, and diet. Independent of diet regime, 6M young mice maintained in an EE where voluntary exercise apparatus is available, revealed significant less body weight than all other groups. Although body weight differences were minimized as age progressed, 18M EE group revealed intragroup significant influence of diet regimens. We suggest that long life environmental enrichment reduces the tendency to avoid open/lit spaces (OF) and this is particularly influenced by masticatory activity. These measurements may be useful in discussions of anxiety-related tasks.

Introduction

Mastication seems to contribute to maintain body weight within normal limits, and a soft diet has been associated with obesity in murine model (Desmarchelier et al., 2013). The latter outcome appears to be associated with significant changes in hypothalamic synaptic input organization and gliosis (Horvath et al., 2010). This type of diet-induced obesity progressively alters cognition and mouse performance in the elevated plus maze (EPM) task (André et al., 2014). In addition, it has been reported that inhibition of adequate masticatory function, due soft-diet feeding, occlusal disharmony or molar losses, affected mice cognitive behavior (Onozuka et al., 2000; Yamamoto and Hirayama, 2001; Tsutsui et al., 2007; Kubo et al., 2010; Ono et al., 2010; Frota De Almeida et al., 2012; Ekuni et al., 2013; Mendes et al., 2013; Sakatani et al., 2013; Nose-Ishibashi et al., 2014; Utsugi et al., 2014; Pang et al., 2015; Takeda et al., 2016; Aguirre Siancas, 2017). Furthermore, loss of molars early in life (Kawahata et al., 2014) or soft diet consumption (Fukushima-Nakayama et al., 2017) caused loss impaired hippocampal-dependent recognition memory and induced a lateralized preference of object location in recognition tasks (Kawahata et al., 2014). In a previous report, we mimicked an active and a sedentary-life styles in murine model and tested the effects of age, environment and diet regimes on spatial memory, using Morris water-maze task (Mendes et al., 2013). We found that an enriched environment (EE) and masticatory activity rehabilitation recover spatial memory decline in aged mice.

However, only a few reports have investigated the effects of a soft diet (Nose-Ishibashi et al., 2014) and aging on sedentary-like and active murine models and no studies have tested the combined effects of masticatory rehabilitation and environmental enrichment on innate repertoire to explore novel environments and to assess risk. In the present report, we tested the hypothesis that age, environment, and masticatory changes may interact and alter mice exploratory patterns of locomotor activity and preferences in an OF arena. Open arenas are widely used for measuring anxiety-like behavior in mice and rats. In these tasks, when animals explore an unfamiliar area, they remain close to the walls; this preference is taken as an indication of fear-induced anxiety (Lalonde and Strazielle, 2008; Ennaceur, 2014). Hiding behavior may contribute to avoiding attack and predation, and it may be included in the repertoire of animal survival instincts. The species-specific hiding response in mice appears to lie at the root of their natural preference for unlit and protected spaces. Thus, the OF tests are based on the natural tendencies in mice to avoid open/lit areas and to spontaneously explore unfamiliar areas (Komada et al., 2008). Thus, animals appear to innately avoid open and/or lit spaces in the central area of the OF (Ennaceur, 2014). Mouse preference for the safety of the peripheral zone of the OF may reflect this adaptive response; for a recent review (see Ennaceur, 2014).

Thus, we focused on these preferred regions to compare mouse behaviors and to detect potential differential effects of diet regimes, age and environmental changes on mouse locomotor and exploratory activities.

To that end we examined outcomes of mice exploratory and locomotor activities in an OF. Tested individuals were under influence of different masticatory regimens, environments and age. Mature (6-month-old; 6M), late mature (12-month-old; 12M), and aged (18-month-old; 18M) mice were maintained either in standard laboratory or enriched cages. Masticatory regimens included: continuous normal mastication with hard pellets (HD); normal mastication followed by reduced mastication, with equal periods of pellets followed by soft powder; and rehabilitated masticatory activity with equal periods of HD, followed by powder, followed by pellets.

Materials and Methods

Female albino Swiss mice were maintained in animal housing, in accordance with the guidelines published by the National Institutes of Health (Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals). The experimental protocol was tested and approved prior to study initiation by the Ethics Committee on Experimental Animal Research (from the Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Pará, Brazil, CEPAE-UFPA: BIO223-14).

Age, Diets, Environments, and Body Weight

Masticatory activity was manipulated with different diets. Each age group was subjected to either a diet of HD, which required substantial chewing, or alternation with powder soft food (SD), which required little chewing. Masticatory rehabilitation was employed by feeding different sequences of HD and SD to mice.

The mice were raised either in sedentary-like or active conditions (Figure 1). The impoverished environment (IE) comprised plastic cages (32 cm × 45 cm × 16.5 cm) limited to chopped rice straw on the floor (no equipment or toys) covered with metal grids. Each IE cage housed eight individuals per age group: young (6M), or middle-aged (12M), or aged (18M) mice. The EE consisted of two-level wire cages (100 cm × 50 cm × 100 cm) equipped with rod bridges, tunnels, ropes, toys, and running wheels. Toys were composed of different forms of plastic, wood, and metal of different colors, and they were changed periodically. Each EE cage housed 12 mice per age group: 6M or 12M, or 18M (the total number of animals per experimental group is described in Table 1). Water and food were delivered to the top and bottom levels, respectively. This arrangement obliged mice to move from one floor to the other, when they wanted to drink and eat. All animals had free access to food and water. All mice were raised in a controlled room temperature (23 ± 1°C) and a 12-h light–dark cycle. To detect potential influences of diet regimes and environments on body weight, all animals were weighed at the beginning of experiment and at the end of each time window (6M, 12M, and 18M).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup and timeline. Time is indicated non-linearly, increasing from left to right, from birth (day 0) to euthanasia. Mice were housed in either an impoverished or enriched environment (IE or EE; images of environments are shown in the middle panel, left) from postnatal days 21 to 180 (top, 6 months old; 6M), 365 (in the middle, 12 months old; 12M), or 540 (from bottom, 18 months old; 18M). All animals were submitted to the open field (OF) tasks (test apparatus is shown in the middle panel, right); test was performed at the points indicated with filled circles on the corresponding timelines. Timelines are color-coded to indicate the following diet regimes: HD (blue): pellet diet; HD/SD (red): alternating pellet and powder diets, switched at 3, 6, or 9 months, respectively, for 6M, 12M, or 18M mice; and HD/SD/HD (green): alternating pellet, powder, and pellet diets, switched every 2, 4, or 6 months, respectively, for 6M, 12M, or 18M mice. Bottom panel: images show the hard pellets (HD) and soft powder (SD).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Number of animals for each group.

Behavioral Tests

All mice were behaviorally assessed only once according to age (6, 12, or 18 months). All groups were tested in an OF before euthanasia. The apparatus and test protocol were slightly modified from previous report (Rodgers and Johnson, 1995).

The OF apparatus consisted of a gray polyvinyl chloride box (30 cm × 30 cm × 40 cm), with a floor divided into central and peripheral regions of equal areas (Figure 4). Each animal was placed at the center of the apparatus for 5 min. All experiments were carried out following the same protocol which included also the tests at the same time of day, conditions of luminosity (3 cd/m2) and handling of the animals.

A video camera connected to a computer was placed one meter above the OF. Each training session was recorded for later analysis with Any-Maze software (Stöelting), and this allowed to record the precise position of each mouse throughout OF test. The following parameters were analyzed: distance traveled, minute-by-minute (m), time spent in the peripheral zone (seconds) and in the corners of the test apparatus. The times were also expressed as the percentage of the total time. After each session, the OF apparatus was cleaned with 70% ethanol.

Statistical Analysis of Behavioral Changes

No blinding procedure was applied for behavioral tests since video records and software do not allow researcher influence. It is also important to mention that all experiments were performed according to the protocol described in previous session. Significant differences between groups with respect to the behavioral tasks were evaluated with three-way ANOVA and Tukey’s post hoc test honestly significant difference. We also investigated the influences of age, diet regime, and environment on behavioral outcomes; differences between groups were considered significant with a 95% confidence level cutoff (p < 0.05). Analyses were performed with EzAnova or BioEstat 5.0 (Ayres et al., 2007) software. To apply three-way factorial ANOVA to the results of OF test, we used the percentage of time spent in peripheral area, as a function of the total time test. One-way ANOVA and the Tukey’s post hoc test honestly significant difference were used to analyze significant differences in distance traveled along each minute of OF test. Differences between groups were considered significant at the 95% confidence level (p < 0.05).

Results

Intergroup Analysis

We investigated the effects of masticatory activity changes, environment, and age on the pattern of exploratory activity in the OF test. Thus, three-way ANOVA analysis applied to time spent in peripheral zone revealed that it was significantly influenced by environment [F(1,90) = 4.55; p < 0.036] and diet [F(2,90) = 8.9; p < 0.0003], but not by age [F(2,90) = 3.07; p < 0.0512]. The combination of environment and diet did not show a significant interaction [F(2,90) = 0.76; p < 0.470].

However, all other variables acting together showed that OF test results were significantly influenced by interactions between the environment and age [F(2,90) = 11.3; p < 0.000042]; age and diet [F(4,90) = 6.00; p < 0.00025]; and environment, age, and diet [F(4,90) = 9.72; p < 0.000001]. These variables (environment and age; age and diet; and environment, age, and diet) interacted either subtractive or additively to influence OF results.

Intragroup Analysis

In the OF task, the HD individuals of 6M mice group raised in IE, spent less time in the periphery as compared to mice of the same age under all other diet regimes maintained at the same environmental condition (Figure 2). Related to 6M mice raised in EE, the influence of diet regimes was limited, because the environmental stimulation seems to increase the preference of the control group (HD EE 6M) by the peripheral zone, minimizing the observed differences between diets among the IE animals. In Figure 3, this preference of the HD EE 6M by the periphery and especially by the corners of the apparatus are illustrated.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The percentages of time spent in the periphery of the OF. () Significantly different from 12 months; (+) significantly different from 18 months; (#) significant difference between impoverished (IE) and enhanced environments (EE); line connectors indicate significant differences between diets. HD (blue) = hard diet; HD/SD (red) = hard diet followed by soft diet; HD/SD/HD (green) = hard diet followed by soft diet followed by hard diet.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Quantification of the time spent in the corners of the OF. Times are shown for mice raised in (left) impoverished or (right) enhanced environments. (+) Significantly different from the 18-month group; lines indicate significant differences between diets; HD (blue) = hard diet; HD/SD (red) = hard diet followed by soft diet; HD/SD/HD (green) = hard diet followed by soft diet followed by hard diet.

Among 12M and 18M groups maintained in IE, no significant effect of diet changes is observed, but it is interesting to highlight that only the HD/SD IE group reduces their preference for the peripheral zone when the animals of 6M and 12M are compared with those of 18M (Figure 2). Another particular difference between diet regimens was observed in the old mice group from enriched environment (18M EE). Mice fed with hard diet (HD – control group), spent less time in periphery zone than mice from all other diets. Interestingly, as age progresses, mice tend to decrease their preference for the peripheral zone, and this behavior is more evident among animals kept in EE and with no change in masticatory activity.

In the case of animals raised in IE, this age effect seems to be present only in the HD/SD group as previously commented. In addition, groups that underwent alteration of masticatory activity also appeared to be more sensitive to environmental influences (IE vs. EE in HD/SD 12M and 18M; IE vs. EE in HD/SD/HD 18M).

Because independent of experimental variables, the experimental groups spent at least half of test time in OF peripheral zone, we investigated potential preferences for selected points of this zone such as its corners. We speculate that corners may be associated with less risk. Thus, Figure 3 illustrates the percentage of test time spent at the corners of the apparatus. Note that reduction of time spent in corners was greater related to aging, and this is evident in HD group, independent of the environment where mice were raised.

More detailed comparisons (mean, standard error, F-values, and p-values) were performed for the different experimental groups (see Supplementary Table 1) to determine the effects of diet regime, environment, and age on OF test outcomes.

Figure 4 demonstrate experimental groups preferences for selected OF regions. In this figure, red color indicates higher preferences and blue one, a smaller preference. Note that mice rarely visited the central region of the OF test, independent of age, environment, or diet regimen. However, as discussed earlier, note that the control group 18M maintained in enriched environment (HD EE 18M) avoid the peripheral zone and corners of the OF apparatus while chewing alteration groups avoid the center of the arena (HD/SD and HD/SD/HD at EE 18M).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Graphic representations of preferred areas in the OF test. (Top row, left) Experimental setup and video capture system for recording mouse behavior in the OF test; (top middle and right) the peripheral and central regions are equal in area. (Lower panels) Time spent in all areas by mice raised in impoverished (left) or enriched (right) environments, at different ages (rows). The square region more central (internal) delineates the central (external) and peripheral areas of the OF. Red, yellow, and blue indicate high, intermediate, and low preferences, respectively. Diet regimes are indicated at the top: HD = hard diet; HD/SD = hard diet followed by soft diet; HD/SD/HD = hard diet followed by soft diet followed by hard diet.

We also analyzed the total distance traveled in each minute of test, independent of spatial location occupied in the OF (Figure 5) and found significant reductions in the locomotor and exploratory activities over the 5 min. These minute-by-minute analysis revealed that traveled distance decrease as time progresses, and that the time-dependent reduction was accentuated by aging and environmental enrichment.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Distance traveled in the OF test. Total distance per minute traveled during the OF test is shown for each diet regime (color-coded), age (top row: 6M, middle row: 12M, and bottom row: 18M), and environment (left: impoverished and right: enriched environment). () Significantly different from the 1st minute; (+) significantly different from the 2nd minute; HD (blue) = hard diet; HD/SD (red) = hard diet followed by soft diet; HD/SD/HD (green) = hard diet followed by soft diet followed by hard diet.

More detailed comparisons (mean, standard error, F-values, and p-values) were performed for the different experimental groups (Supplementary Table 2) to determine the effects of diet regime, environment, and age on OF outcomes.

Body Weight

At the beginning of the experiment no statistically significant difference was found related to weights of the animals in the different groups.

Intergroup Analysis

A three-way ANOVA of body weights recorded on the day before euthanasia of each group revealed that body weight was significant influenced by environment [F(1,90) = 32.4, p < 0.000001], age [F(2,90) = 3.45, p < 0.036], and diet [F(2,90) = 14.4, p < 0.000004]. Significant interactions between environment and diet [F(2,90) = 3.62, p < 0.03], and between environment and age [F(2,90) = 15.1, p < 0.000002] were observed. There was not significant interaction between age and diet [F(4.90) = 2.29, p < 0.066] or between environment, age, and diet [F(4,90) = 0.201, p < 0.937].

Intragroup Analysis

Within the 6M and 12M age group, created in IE, mice rehabilitated to normal masticatory activity (HD/SD/HD) showed significant higher body weights than that mice with reduced masticatory activity (HD/SD group) (Figure 6). This increasing condition was not observed in the groups created in EE at the same time windows. However, HD weighed significantly more than HD/SD in IE and EE at 6M, and more than HD/SD/HD in EE at the same age. The same occurred at 18 months of age.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Quantification of the weight body at the end of each age analyzed. () Significantly different from 12 months; (+) significantly different from 18 months; (#) significant difference between impoverished (IE) and enhanced environments (EE); line connectors indicate significant differences between diets. HD (blue) = hard diet; HD/SD (red) = hard diet followed by soft diet; HD/SD/HD (green) = hard diet followed by soft diet followed by hard diet.

Comparing the environments, the EE 6M was associated with a significant body weight reduction compared to IE mice, independent of diet regime, but no significant differences were detected between IE and EE mice at 12 or 18 months.

Related to different ages, the weight gain of animals in the 12M group, regardless of the diet regime, was significantly higher compared to 6M, as well as the weight gain at 18M, and this was especially noted in animals raised in EE.

More detailed comparisons (mean, standard error, F-values, and p-values) were performed for the different experimental groups to determine the effects of diet regime, environment, and age on weight outcomes and are depicted on Supplementary Table 3.

Discussion

In a previous report, we demonstrated that a 24-h, complex EE had differential effects on performance in episodic-like and water maze memory tests, among young and aged female albino Swiss mice (Diniz et al., 2010). We also showed that imposing reduced masticatory activity, by offering a powder diet to mice previously fed a pellet (HD/SD), caused impaired spatial learning, but a diet that imposed masticatory rehabilitation (HD/SD/HD) reversed the impairments in aged mice (Mendes et al., 2013).

Part of these effects can be explained by studies that evidence that masticatory dysfunction, sustainably activates the Hypothalamic-Hypophysis-Adrenal (H-H-A) axis increasing blood stream glucocorticoid levels. The continuous increase in circulating glucocorticoids in turn interrupts H-H-A axis negative feedback system, further increasing secretion of glucorticoids (Azuma et al., 2017). However, neurogenesis in dentate gyrus, which is critical to hippocampal-mediated modulation in H-H-A axis also appears to be impaired by masticatory dysfunction causing cognitive deficits which are dependent on morphofunctional integrity of the hippocampus (Snyder et al., 2011).

In this context, high levels of glucorticoids could still be modulating anxiety-like or fear behavior. The act of hiding to avoid attacks and predation ends up being an innate strategy to guarantee animal survival and this logic follows OF tests in which apparatus they have a natural tendency to avoid open areas (Komada et al., 2008).

The EPM, although widely used in these cases, seems to suffer undesirable influence from environmental manipulation that may increase or decrease locomotor activity generating false positive or negative test results (Dawson and Tricklebank, 1995). Exposure to a new environment immediately prior to EPM, for example, increases motor activity in maze increasing time spent in open arms (File and Wardill, 1975a,b; Pellow et al., 1985).

In our laboratory, mice are often subjected to a sequence of tasks, which include OF testing, object recognition, followed by EPM and forced swimming. In these cases, it is common to perform each task on separate occasions. However, in face of such evidence of pre-exposure to another environment influencing the EPM results we assume that careful consideration should be given when designing experiments using EPM.

Therefore, we tested the hypothesis that a combination of masticatory rehabilitation and EE would minimize the impaired exploratory and locomotor behavioral changes induced by a combination of IE and reduced masticatory activity. We demonstrated that all mice, independent of the masticatory condition, environment, and age, exhibited a similar temporal organization of their spatial horizontal exploratory activity in the OF task. Nevertheless, we found that the EE, aging, and normal masticatory activity (HD), interacting with each other, reduced the innate tendency to avoid open/lit spaces in the OF task. We also demonstrated that contrasting diet regimes, designed to reduce or rehabilitate masticatory activity, had differential effects at different time windows.

In the OF test sedentary young adults from IE with reduced masticatory activity or rehabilitation (HD/SD and HD/SD/HD IE 6M) showed a higher preference for the periphery of the OF than control animals from similar environment (HD IE 6M). This result may suggest that the preference for the peripheral zone observed in the groups with masticatory alterations may be associated with potential stress due to masticatory changes, either associated with masticatory reduction or rehabilitation. This result is in line with previous report, showing that the total distance of locomotion was significantly higher for animals fed with soft diet previously fed with hard diet, suggesting that the shift to powdered diet may affect the responsiveness of mice exposed to new environments (Nose-Ishibashi et al., 2014). Indeed, it has been proposed that early in life masticatory manipulation may increase vulnerability for mental disorders (Nose-Ishibashi et al., 2014).

Horizontal locomotor exploration comprises part of the innate repertoire used by animals to explore novel environments and to assess risk in the wild (Augustsson and Meyerson, 2004; Ennaceur, 2014). It has been suggested that this strategy arises from the drives to avoid and to explore a perceived threatening stimulus (Crusio, 2001). In the present study, we promptly recognized this stereotypical behavior, when mice explored the OF (Lister, 1990; Kalueff et al., 2006; Ennaceur, 2014).

With exposure to predator odors, a previous report (Sampedro-Piquero et al., 2016), demonstrated that EE could reduce anxiety in aged Wistar rats, when confronted with cat odor. That finding was indicated by a time reduction in freezing behavior. Similar EE effects were observed in aged Wistar rats on an elevated-zero maze task (Sampedro-Piquero et al., 2013, 2014).

Although a controversial issue due to methodological differences, species and strains used in the tests, it is important to highlight that EE may contribute either to reduce or increase anxiety-like behavior and depression (Greenwood et al., 2003; Burghardt et al., 2004; Van Hoomissen et al., 2004; Duman et al., 2008; Leasure and Jones, 2008).

In the present report, the HD EE 18M group compared with HD EE 6M individuals, exhibited a reduction in anxiety-like behavior, suggesting that open arena adaptive behavior in this group may be associate with a continuous non-aversive EE which includes normal chewing, voluntary exercise, and visuospatial stimulation as part of the necessary repertoire to adapt to an open arena with reduced stress level (Dupire et al., 2013). In agreement, our findings revealed that, as compared with individuals from IE, animals raised in EE, decreased traveled distance more rapidly along the test, and this was particularly significant to 18M mice.

An alternative view is that the environmental enrichment may increase stress level due to the weekly change in the location or substitution of old by new toys (Larsson et al., 2002). This chronic low level of stress may adapt old mice from EE to face novelty (Konkle et al., 2010).

Allied to this, as in the present work the tests were performed only in females, it is important to discuss some possible influences of estrous cycle. We have seen that estrus phases and respective ovarian hormones oscillations in rats may be mediating defensive behaviors (Blume et al., 2017), which are associated with fear and anxiety (Graham and Scott, 2018; Pentkowski et al., 2018), including neuronal changes in amygdala nuclei (such as the lateral and basal). However, despite evidence on these influences, it is considered that estrous cycle may be affected by animal housing conditions too. Andrade et al. (2002) reported that females from overpopulation cages, without males, appear to exhibit a phase designated as anestrous characterized by the absence of estrus cycles. In our case, the estrous cycle phases were not investigated in the animals and since we only worked with females and only females inhabited our laboratory, we expect little influence of the cycle on the behavioral results detected. However, because we did not measure potential influences of hormones on behavior, future studies should take this as a limitation of the present study. Despite of this, it is consistent to say that the older mice was depleted of estrogenic protection. Thus, we reasoned that at least part of the behavioral changes may be related to the estropause.

When analyzed the differences in the body weight of the animals, as compared with controls HD or rehabilitated HD/SD/HD mice, the body weight from HD/SD mice, fed proportionally longer with soft diet revealed significant reduction of their body weight. This finding does not agree with previous data demonstrating long term soft diet-induced obesity (Nojima et al., 2006; Desmarchelier et al., 2013; André et al., 2014). Coherently, we found no correlation between preferred zone of the OF and body weight as previously described in rats with weight losses induced by gastrectomy as compared with rats fed with hypercaloric diets (Himel et al., 2018).

Notwithstanding, it is important to recognize that the environmental enrichment influenced the weight gain. Indeed, independently of the diet regime at 6M all EE groups weighed less than those from IE. In contrast to animals from IE, EE seemed to enhance body weight differences between 6M, 12M, and 18M mice reducing age related metabolic differences.

Therefore, we conclude that changes in masticatory activity, influence the pattern of exploration by zones in the OF test and environmental impoverishment seems to enhance the effects of aging, increasing the preference for the peripheral zone of OF by the animals that not have undergone chewing alteration. Finally, the environmental enrichment also influences mice weight gain, but this is not correlated with spatial pattern of explored zones in the OF.

Author Contributions

FCCSM participated in the development and methodological design, collection and treatment of data, analysis and interpretation of data and writing. LTVBP participated in the collection and processing of data. CWPD participated in the development and methodological design, supervision, analysis and interpretation of data and writing. MCKS participated in the development and methodological design, supervision, analysis and interpretation of data and writing.

Funding

This project was supported by research funds from the Brazilian Government. FCCSM was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior CAPES as part of Doctoral Program. MCKS was supported by grants from the Brazilian Research Council CNPq (Grant No. 475677/2008-0), the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Pará (FAPESPA, Grant No. 136/08), and CWPD was supported by CAPES/Pró-Amazônia (Grant No. 3311/2013). Research funds from the Fundação de Amparo e Desenvolvimento da Pesquisa (FADESP) and the Pró-Reitoria de Pesquisa e Pós-Graduação (PROPESP/UFPA) payed for proofreading, editing, and publication fees.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2019.00107/full#supplementary-material

References

Aguirre Siancas, E. E. (2017). Influence of chewing behaviour on memory and spatial learning in albino BALB/c mice. Neurologia 32, 236–240. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2015.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrade, A., Pinto, S. C., Oliveira, R. S., and Orgs, S. (2002). Animais de Laboratório: Criação e Experimentação. Rio de Janeiro: Editora Fiocruz.

Google Scholar

André, C., Dinel, A. L., Ferreira, G., Layé, S., and Castanon, N. (2014). Diet-induced obesity progressively alters cognition, anxiety-like behavior and lipopolysaccharide-induced depressive-like behavior: focus on brain indoleamine 2,3-dioxygenase activation. Brain Behav. Immun. 41, 10–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Augustsson, H., and Meyerson, B. J. (2004). Exploration and risk assessment: a comparative study of male house mice (Mus musculus musculus) and two laboratory strains. Physiol. Behav. 81, 685–698. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayres, M., Ayres, M., Ayres, D. L., Dos Santos, A. S., and Ayres, L. L. (2007). BioEstat 5.0: Aplicações Estatísticas Nas Áreas Das Ciências Biológicas e Médicas. Belém: Sociedade Civil Mamirauá, Brasília CNPq.

Google Scholar

Azuma, K., Zhou, Q., Niwa, M., and Kubo, K. Y. (2017). Association between mastication, the hippocampus, and the HPA Axis: a comprehensive review. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 18:1687. doi: 10.3390/ijms18081687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blume, S. R., Freedberg, M., Vantrease, J. E., Chan, R., Padival, M., Record, M. J., et al. (2017). Sex- and estrus-dependent differences in rat basolateral amygdala. J. Neurosci. 37, 10567–10586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0758-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burghardt, P. R., Fulk, L. J., Hand, G. A., and Wilson, M. A. (2004). The effects of chronic treadmill and wheel running on behavior in rats. Brain Res. 1019, 84–96. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.05.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crusio, W. E. (2001). Genetic dissection of mouse exploratory behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 125, 127–132. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00280-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, G. R., and Tricklebank, M. D. (1995). Use of the elevated plus maze in the search for novel anxiolytic agents. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 16, 33–36. doi: 10.1016/S0165-6147(00)88973-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmarchelier, C., Ludwig, T., Scheundel, R., Rink, N., Bader, B. L., Klingenspor, M., et al. (2013). Diet-induced obesity in ad libitum-fed mice: food texture overrides the effect of macronutrient composition. Br. J. Nutr. 109, 1518–1527. doi: 10.1017/S0007114512003340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diniz, D. G., Foro, C. A., Rego, C. M., Gloria, D. A., De Oliveira, F. R., Paes, J. M., et al. (2010). Environmental impoverishment and aging alter object recognition, spatial learning, and dentate gyrus astrocytes. Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 509–519. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07296.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duman, C. H., Schlesinger, L., Russell, D. S., and Duman, R. S. (2008). Voluntary exercise produces antidepressant and anxiolytic behavioral effects in mice. Brain Res. 1199, 148–158. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupire, A., Kant, P., Mons, N., Marchand, A. R., Coutureau, E., Dalrymple-Alford, J., et al. (2013). A role for anterior thalamic nuclei in affective cognition: interaction with environmental conditions. Hippocampus 23, 392–404. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekuni, D., Endo, Y., Tomofuji, T., Azuma, T., Irie, K., Kasuyama, K., et al. (2013). Effects of apoE deficiency and occlusal disharmony on amyloid-beta production and spatial memory in rats. PLoS One 8:e74966. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennaceur, A. (2014). Tests of unconditioned anxiety - pitfalls and disappointments. Physiol. Behav. 135, 55–71. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

File, S. E., and Wardill, A. G. (1975a). The reliability of the hole-board apparatus. Psychopharmacologia 44, 47–51. doi: 10.1007/BF00421183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

File, S. E., and Wardill, A. G. (1975b). Validity of head-dipping as a measure of exploration in a modified hole-board. Psychopharmacologia 44, 53–59.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Frota De Almeida, M. N., de Siqueira Mendes Fde, C., Gurgel Felício, A. P., Falsoni, M., Ferreira De Andrade, M. L., Bento-Torres, J., et al. (2012). Spatial memory decline after masticatory deprivation and aging is associated with altered laminar distribution of CA1 astrocytes. BMC Neurosci. 13:23. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukushima-Nakayama, Y., Ono, T., Hayashi, M., Inoue, M., Wake, H., Ono, T., et al. (2017). Reduced mastication impairs memory function. J. Dent. Res. 96, 1058–1066. doi: 10.1177/0022034517708771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, B. M., and Scott, E. (2018). Effects of systemic estradiol on fear extinction in female rats are dependent on interactions between dose, estrous phase, and endogenous estradiol levels. Horm. Behav. 97, 67–74. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwood, B. N., Foley, T. E., Day, H. E., Campisi, J., Hammack, S. H., Campeau, S., et al. (2003). Freewheel running prevents learned helplessness/behavioral depression: role of dorsal raphe serotonergic neurons. J. Neurosci. 23, 2889–2898. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-02889.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Himel, A. R., Cabral, S. A., Shaffery, J. P., and Grayson, B. E. (2018). Anxiety behavior and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis altered in a female rat model of vertical sleeve gastrectomy. PLoS One 13:e0200026. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, T. L., Sarman, B., García-Cáceres, C., Enriori, P. J., Sotonyi, P., Shanabrough, M., et al. (2010). Synaptic input organization of the melanocortin system predicts diet-induced hypothalamic reactive gliosis and obesity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 14875–14880. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1004282107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V., Keisala, T., Minasyan, A., Kuuslahti, M., and Tuohimaa, P. (2006). Temporal stability of novelty exploration in mice exposed to different open field tests. Behav. Process. 72, 104–112. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2005.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawahata, M., Ono, Y., Ohno, A., Kawamoto, S., Kimoto, K., and Onozuka, M. (2014). Loss of molars early in life develops behavioral lateralization and impairs hippocampus-dependent recognition memory. BMC Neurosci. 15:4. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-15-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komada, M., Takao, K., and Miyakawa, T. (2008). Elevated plus maze for mice. J. Vis. Exp. 22:1088. doi: 10.3791/1088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konkle, A. T., Kentner, A. C., Baker, S. L., Stewart, A., and Bielajew, C. (2010). Environmental-enrichment-related variations in behavioral, biochemical, and physiologic responses of Sprague-Dawley and Long Evans rats. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 49, 427–436.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kubo, K. Y., Ichihashi, Y., Kurata, C., Iinuma, M., Mori, D., Katayama, T., et al. (2010). Masticatory function and cognitive function. Okajimas Folia Anat. Jpn. 87, 135–140. doi: 10.2535/ofaj.87.135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalonde, R., and Strazielle, C. (2008). Relations between open-field, elevated plus-maze, and emergence tests as displayed by C57/BL6J and BALB/c mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 171, 48–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsson, F., Winblad, B., and Mohammed, A. H. (2002). Psychological stress and environmental adaptation in enriched vs. impoverished housed rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73, 193–207. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00782-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leasure, J. L., and Jones, M. (2008). Forced and voluntary exercise differentially affect brain and behavior. Neuroscience 156, 456–465. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lister, R. G. (1990). Ethologically-based animal models of anxiety disorders. Pharmacol. Ther. 46, 321–340. doi: 10.1016/0163-7258(90)90021-S

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendes, F. E. C., de Almeida, M. N., Felício, A. P., Fadel, A. C., Silva Dde, J., Borralho, T. G., et al. (2013). Enriched environment and masticatory activity rehabilitation recover spatial memory decline in aged mice. BMC Neurosci. 14:63. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-14-63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nojima, K., Ikegami, H., Fujisawa, T., Ueda, H., Babaya, N., Itoi-Babaya, M., et al. (2006). Food hardness as environmental factor in development of type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Res. Clin. Pract. 74, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.diabres.2006.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nose-Ishibashi, K., Watahiki, J., Yamada, K., Maekawa, M., Watanabe, A., Yamamoto, G., et al. (2014). Soft-diet feeding after weaning affects behavior in mice: potential increase in vulnerability to mental disorders. Neuroscience 263, 257–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ono, Y., Yamamoto, T., Kubo, K. Y., and Onozuka, M. (2010). Occlusion and brain function: mastication as a prevention of cognitive dysfunction. J. Oral Rehabil. 37, 624–640. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2010.02079.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onozuka, M., Yamamoto, T., Kubo, K. Y., and Onozuka, M. (2000). Impairment of spatial memory and changes in astroglial responsiveness following loss of molar teeth in aged SAMP8 mice. Behav. Brain Res. 108, 145–155. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00145-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, Q., Hu, X., Li, X., Zhang, J., and Jiang, Q. (2015). Behavioral impairments and changes of nitric oxide and inducible nitric oxide synthase in the brains of molarless KM mice. Behav. Brain Res. 278, 411–416. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellow, S., Chopin, P., File, S. E., and Briley, M. (1985). Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods 14, 149–167. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pentkowski, N. S., Litvin, Y., Blanchard, D. C., and Blanchard, R. J. (2018). Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats. Physiol. Behav. 194, 41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, R. J., and Johnson, N. J. (1995). Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and ethological measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 52, 297–303. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00138-M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakatani, K., Tsujii, T., Hirayama, T., Katayama, Y., Takeda, T., Amemiya, A., et al. (2013). Effects of occlusal disharmony on working memory performance and prefrontal cortex activity induced by working memory tasks measured by NIRS. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 765, 239–244. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-4989-8_33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampedro-Piquero, P., Arias, J. L., and Begega, A. (2014). Behavioral testing-related changes in the expression of Synapsin I and glucocorticoid receptors in standard and enriched aged Wistar rats. Exp. Gerontol. 58, 292–302. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2014.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampedro-Piquero, P., Castilla-Ortega, E., Zancada-Menendez, C., Santín, L. J., and Begega, A. (2016). Environmental enrichment as a therapeutic avenue for anxiety in aged Wistar rats: effect on cat odor exposition and GABAergic interneurons. Neuroscience 330, 17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampedro-Piquero, P., Zancada-Menendez, C., Begega, A., Rubio, S., and Arias, J. L. (2013). Effects of environmental enrichment on anxiety responses, spatial memory and cytochrome c oxidase activity in adult rats. Brain Res. Bull 98, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, J. S., Soumier, A., Brewer, M., Pickel, J., and Cameron, H. A. (2011). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis buffers stress responses and depressive behaviour. Nature 476, 458–461. doi: 10.1038/nature10287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeda, Y., Oue, H., Okada, S., Kawano, A., Koretake, K., Michikawa, M., et al. (2016). Molar loss and powder diet leads to memory deficit and modifies the mRNA expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the hippocampus of adult mice. BMC Neurosci. 17:81. doi: 10.1186/s12868-016-0319-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsutsui, K., Kaku, M., Motokawa, M., Tohma, Y., Kawata, T., Fujita, T., et al. (2007). Inflluences of reduced masticatory sensory input from soft-diet feeding upon spatial memory/learning ability in mice. Biomed. Res. 28, 1–7. doi: 10.2220/biomedres.28.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Utsugi, C., Miyazono, S., Osada, K., Sasajima, H., Noguchi, T., Matsuda, M., et al. (2014). Hard-diet feeding recovers neurogenesis in the subventricular zone and olfactory functions of mice impaired by soft-diet feeding. PLoS One 9:e97309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Hoomissen, J. D., Holmes, P. V., Zellner, A. S., Poudevigne, A., and Dishman, R. K. (2004). Effects of beta-adrenoreceptor blockade during chronic exercise on contextual fear conditioning and mRNA for galanin and brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 1378–1390. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.6.1378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, T., and Hirayama, A. (2001). Effects of soft-diet feeding on synaptic density in the hippocampus and parietal cortex of senescence-accelerated mice. Brain Res. 902, 255–263. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02410-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reduced mastication, environmental changes, aging, locomotor and exploratory activities, anxiety-like behavior

Citation: Siqueira Mendes FCC, Paixão LTVB, Diniz CWP and Sosthenes MCK (2019) Environmental Impoverishment, Aging, and Reduction in Mastication Affect Mouse Innate Repertoire to Explore Novel Environments and to Assess Risk. Front. Neurosci. 13:107. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00107

Received: 13 July 2018; Accepted: 29 January 2019;
Published: 14 March 2019.

Edited by:

Rubem C. A. Guedes, Federal University of Pernambuco, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Nafisa M. Jadavji, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute (OHRI), Canada
Marcelo Cavenaghi Pereira Da Silva, Federal University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2019 Siqueira Mendes, Paixão, Diniz and Sosthenes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cristovam Wanderley Picanço Diniz, cwpdiniz@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.