Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 11 January 2021
Sec. Autonomic Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Horizon 2030: Innovative Applications of Heart Rate Variability View all 40 articles

Heart Rate Variability Modulates Interoceptive Accuracy

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • 2Department of Sport Science, University of Rostock, Rostock, Germany

Our emotional experiences depend on our interoceptive ability to perceive and interpret changes in our autonomous nervous system. An inaccurate perception and interpretation of autonomic changes impairs our ability to understand and regulate our emotional reactions. Impairments in emotion understanding and emotion regulation increase our risk for mental disorders, indicating that interoceptive deficits play an important role in the etiology and pathogenesis of mental disorders. We, thus, need measures to identify those of us whose interoceptive deficits impair their emotion understanding and emotion regulation. Here, we used cardiac measures to investigate how our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain region regions affects our ability to perceive and interpret cardiac changes. We administered a heartbeat detection task to a sample of healthy individuals (n = 113) whose prefrontal-(para-) limbic engagement had been determined on basis of a heart rate variability recording. We found a positive association between heartbeat detection and heart rate variability, implying that individuals with higher heart rate variability were more accurate in heartbeat detection than individuals with lower heart rate variability. These findings suggest that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. Our findings also show that cardiac measures may be useful to investigate the association between interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in a time- and cost-efficient manner.

Introduction

Interoception refers to our ability to perceive and interpret changes in our autonomous nervous system (Craig, 2002). Cardiac and respiratory activity are examples of autonomic processes that frequently change during emotional events (Kreibig, 2010). The perception and interpretation of these autonomic changes forms the basis of our emotional experiences and helps us to understand and to regulate our emotional reactions (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). For instance, those of us who are sensitive to cardiac changes have less difficulties in understanding and responding to emotional events than those of us who are insensitive to cardiac changes (Fustos et al., 2013; Shah et al., 2017; Lischke et al., 2020a, b). Given that emotion understanding and emotion regulation is central for our mental health (Gross and Jazaieri, 2014), it is not surprising that alterations in the perception and interpretation of autonomic changes increase our risk for mental disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010). For instance, those of us whose cardiac sensitivity is in the abnormal range have more mental health problems than those of us whose cardiac sensitivity is in the normal range (Pollatos et al., 2007b, 2009; Herbert et al., 2011; Shah et al., 2016). The way we perceive and interpret autonomic changes, thus, appears to be of utmost importance for our mental health.

To understand how we perceive and interpret autonomic changes, we need measures that differentiate between different aspects of interoception (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). Fortunately, these measures have already been developed by researchers. Depending on the research question, these measures assess interoceptive accuracy (i.e., objective accounts of interoception), interoceptive sensibility (i.e., subjective accounts of interoception) or interoceptive awareness (i.e., correspondence between objective and subjective accounts of interoception). The most popular measure is the heartbeat detection task (Schandry, 1981), an interceptive accuracy task that requires the tracking of heartbeats within different time intervals. By employing the heartbeat detection task, researchers were able to identify a network of brain regions that is relevant for the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Schulz, 2016). This network comprises several brain regions but prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions appear to be the most important ones. Prefrontal and (para-)limbic show the most pronounced activity changes during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005; Kuehn et al., 2016), implying a close association between prefrontal-(para-)limbic activity and interoceptive accuracy. It may, thus, be possible that our ability to perceive and interpret cardiac changes depends on our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions for this matter.

To test this possibility, we administered measures of interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement to a sample of young adults. Interoceptive accuracy was measured with the heartbeat detection task and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement was measured with a heart rate recording. The heart rate recording was used for the determination of parasympathically induced heart rate changes, a measure of vagally mediated heart rate variability (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Parasympathetically induced heart rate changes are closely associated with activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions (Thayer et al., 2012; Ruiz Vargas et al., 2016), indicating that vagally mediated heart rate variability reflects prefrontal-(para-)limibic engagement (Thayer and Lane, 2009; Smith et al., 2017). The heart rate recording, thus, allowed us to investigate the association between interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in an unobtrusive manner. In light of previous findings showing that activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions are positively associated with the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005; Kuehn et al., 2016), we expected to find a similar association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. Preliminary findings suggest that vagally mediated heart rate variability may be positively associated with heartbeat detection (Owens et al., 2018). We, thus, expected to find a positive rather than negative association between vagally mediated heartrate variability and heartbeat detection.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We based our participant recruitment on an a priori power analysis with GPower 3.1.9.2 (Faul et al., 2009). The power analysis suggested that 82 participants would provide sufficient data to detect meaningful associations between vagally mediated heartrate variability and heartbeat detection in our analyses [correlation analysis (two-tailed): α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.80, r = 0.30; regression analyses (total number of predictors: 8, number of tested predictors: 1): α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.80, f2 = 0.15]. Following this suggestion, we recruited 113 participants for the study (see Table 1). In order to be included in the study, the participants had to be native speakers and to be aged between 18 and 35 years. Participants who were currently in psychotherapeutic treatment were excluded from the study. Inclusion and exclusion of participants was determined on basis of an in-house questionnaire assessing sociodemographic (age and sex), anthropometric (height and weight) and medical (physical activity in terms of aerobic fitness, smoking status, medication status, treatment status) information (Lischke et al., 2018a). All participants that were included in the study provided written-informed consent to the study protocol. The study protocol, which had been approved by the local ethics committee, was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

Procedure

We used a heart rate recording to determine participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. Each recording session was scheduled during the daytime (at least 2 h after wakening time and 5 h before sleeping time) to control for circadian and diurnal variations in participants’ heart rate (Yamasaki et al., 1996; Bonnemeier et al., 2003). Before we started with the recording session, we asked the participants to use the bathroom. This allowed us to rule out that bladder filling and gastric digestion had an effect on participants’ heart rate (Fagius and Karhuvaara, 1989; Rossi et al., 1998). The participants completed the recording session in a comfortable chair that was located in a dimly lit room. The heart rate recording was performed with a mobile heart rate monitor (RS 800, Polar Electro Oy; Kempele, Finland) that has been shown to record heartbeats as accurate as mobile electrocardiogram systems (Weippert et al., 2010).

Heartbeat Detection

Participants’ heartbeat detection was determined during the first part of the recording session. Following an established procedure (Lischke et al., 2020a, b), we asked the participants to count their heartbeats during 25, 35, and 45 s lasting time intervals. They were not informed about the length of the time intervals and they were not allowed to use any measure that may have facilitated the heartbeat detection. We used the number of counted and recorded heartbeats to compute two different indices of participants’ heartbeat detection, a traditional heartbeat detection index1 (Schandry, 1981) and an alternative heartbeat detection index2 (Hart et al., 2013) that has been shown to be to be less sensitive against outliers than the traditional heartbeat detection index (Garfinkel et al., 2015). We used both heartbeat detection indices in our analyses to test the robustness of our findings.

Heart Rate Variability

Participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability was determined during the second part of the recording session. Following an established procedure (Lischke et al., 2018b, 2019), we asked the participants to sit still and to stay awake during a 300 s lasting time interval. The heartbeats that were recorded during this time interval were analyzed with Kubios HRV 2.2 (Tarvainen et al., 2014). The analysis followed the guidelines of the Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (1996): The recordings were detrended (smoothn priors: λ = 500), visually inspected and artifact corrected (adaptive filtering: cubic spline interpolation) before they were subjected to a time-domain and spectral analysis. The time-domain analysis was used for the determination of a heart rate index (meanHR) and for the determination of two vagally mediated heartrate variability indices (the root mean square of successive differences between consecutive heartbeats, RMSSD; the number of successive heartbeat interval pairs that differ more than 50 ms divided by the total number of all heartbeat intervals, pNN50). The values of these indices were in the range of values that have been reported in comparable samples of participants (Dantas et al., 2018). The spectral analysis was used to determine a respiration index (Thayer et al., 2002), the peak frequency of the high frequency band (pHF). We used the respiration index to adjust the vagally mediated heartrate variability indices for respiration-induced alterations (Weippert et al., 2015). Using both vagally mediated heartrate variability indices in our analyses allowed us to test the robustness of our findings.

Statistical Analyses

We performed all analyses with the bootstrapping module of SPSS 27 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Preliminary analyses revealed that the datasets of four participants were incomplete or invalid due to a recording error. We, thus, used the datasets of the remaining 109 participants for the main analyses. The main analyses comprised regression and correlation analyses. Multiple regressions were run to analyze the association between participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices constituted the predictor variables and the heartbeat detection indices constituted the criterion variables. Participant characteristics that may distort the association between vagally mediated heartrate variability and heartbeat detection were used as additional predictor variables (age, sex, body mass index, physical activity, respiratory activity, smoking status, and medication status). The regression analyses were complemented by correlation analyses. Partial correlations were run to quantify the association between participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices constituted the dependent variable, the heartbeat detection indices the independent variables and the aforementioned participant characteristics the control variables. We set the significance level for all analyses at α ≤ 0.05 and determined significance values (p), effect size measures (r, R2, ΔR2, and B) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

Associations Between the Vagally Mediated Heart Rate Variability Indices (RMSSD, pNN50) and the Traditional Heartbeat Detection Index (HBDSC)

In the first set of regression models, the traditional heartbeat detection index constituted the criterion variable. Entering the participant characteristics as predictor variables in a first step into the regression models accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the traditional heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: R2 = 0.15, F(8,100) = 2.13, p = 0.040; pNN50: R2 = 0.15, F(8,100) = 2.13, p = 0.040, see Table 2]. However, sex was the only predictor variable that turned out be significant in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.14, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23], t(100) = 2.37, p = 0.015; pNN50: B = 0.13, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.04, 0.23], t(100) = 2.37, p = 0.018; see Table 2]. Entering the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices as predictor variables in a second step into the regression models also accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the traditional heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: ΔR2 = 0.04, ΔF(1,99) = 4.46, p = 0.037; pNN50: ΔR2 = 0.05, F(1,99) = 6.13, p = 0.015, see Table 2]. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices were, besides sex [RMSSD: B = 0.13, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23], t(99) = 2.33, p = 0.013; pNN50: B = 0.12, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.02, 0.22], t(99) = 2.27, p = 0.020; see Table 2], the only significant predictors in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.15, SE B = 0.06, 95% CI [0.02, 0.28], t(99) = 2.11, p = 0.024; pNN50: B = 0.09, SE B = 0.04, 95% CI [0.02, 0.16], t(99) = 2.48, p = 0.009; see Table 2]. Taken together, the regression models suggested that there was a small to medium sized association between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices and the traditional heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: r(99) = 0.21, 95% CI [0.05, 0.37], p = 0.037; pNN50: r(99) = 24, 95% CI [0.05, 0.41], p = 0.015; see Figure 1].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the traditional heartbeat detection index (HBDSC).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scatterplots with lines of best fit and 95% confidence intervals demonstrating raw associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the traditional heartbeat detection index (HBDSC).

Associations Between the Vagally Mediated Heart Rate Variability Indices (RMSSD, pNN50) and the Alternative Heartbeat Detection Index (HBDGA)

In the second set of regression models, the alternative heartbeat detection index constituted the criterion variable. Entering the participant characteristics as predictor variables in a first step into the regression models accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the alternative heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: R2 = 0.14, F(8,100) = 2.12, p = 0.041; pNN50: R2 = 0.14, F(8,100) = 2.12, p = 0.041; see Table 3]. However, sex was the only predictor variable that turned out be significant in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.18, SE B = 0.08, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23], t(100) = 2.36, p = 0.017; pNN50: B = 0.18, SE B = 0.07, 95% CI [0.04, 0.33], t(100) = 2.36, p = 0.009; see Table 3]. Entering the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices as predictor variables in a second step into the regression models also accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the alternative heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: ΔR2 = 0.04, ΔF(1,99) = 4.79, p = 0.031; pNN50: ΔR2 = 0.05, F(1,99) = 6.70, p = 0.011; see Table 3]. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices were, besides sex [RMSSD: B = 0.18, SE B = 0.08, 95% CI [0.03, 0.34], t(99) = 2.32, p = 0.022; pNN50: B = 0.17, SE B = 0.07, 95% CI [0.04, 0.32], t(99) = 2.25, p = 0.015; see Table 3], the only significant predictors in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.23, SE B = 0.09, 95% CI [0.04, 0.40], t(99) = 2.19, p = 0.020; pNN50: B = 0.13, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.22], t(100) = 2.59, p = 0.007; see Table 3]. Taken together, the regression models suggested that there was a small to medium sized association between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices and the alternative heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: r(99) = 0.21, 95% CI [0.05, 0.37], p = 0.031; pNN50: r(99) = 0.25, 95% CI [0.06, 0.42], p = 0.011; see Figure 2].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the alternative heartbeat detection index (HBDGA).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatterplots with lines of best fit and 95% confidence intervals demonstrating raw associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the alternative heartbeat detection index (HBDGA).

Discussion

To explore the possibility that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions for this matter (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005, 2007a; Kuehn et al., 2016), we administered cardiac measures of interoceptive accuracy and prefontal-(para-)limbic engagement to a sample of young adults. Interoceptive accuracy was measured with a heartbeat detection task and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement with a heart rate recording. We used the heartbeat detection task to determine two different heartbeat detection indices and the heart rate recording to determine two different vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). The vagally mediated heart rate indices were both positively associated with the heartbeat detection indices, regardless whether the traditional or alternative heartbeat detection index were considered in the regression and correlation analyses. The regression and correlation analyses were well-powered and well-controlled, ruling out that the association between the vagally mediated heart rate variability index and the heartbeat detection indices was spurious. We, thus, found the expected association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. The association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection supports the idea that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions for the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes.

To understand the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection, we have to delineate the processes that determine the performance on the heartbeat detection task. Heartbeat detection relies on executive control processes (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017), in particular on those that are related to attention. For an accurate perception and interpretation of cardiac changes, attention has to be shifted from the outside to the inside of the body, to be shielded against sensations from the outside of the body and to be focused on sensations inside the body. Attention shifting, attention shielding and attention focusing are executive control processes that are driven by activity changes in prefrontal brain and (para-)limbic regions (Petersen and Posner, 2012). Activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions are associated with vagally mediated heart rate variability (Thayer et al., 2012; Ruiz Vargas et al., 2016), indicating that vagally mediated heart rate variability may also be associated with these executive control processes. Vagally mediated heart rate variability is, in fact, associated with executive control processes (Zahn et al., 2016; Holzman and Bridgett, 2017), including attention shifting, attention shielding and attention focusing (Hansen et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2016; Siennicka et al., 2019; Sorensen et al., 2019). The association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection may, thus, be mediated by executive control processes that are driven by activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions.

There are several prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions that may mediate the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection through executive control processes. These brain regions are organized in networks that are implicated in the up- and downregulation of cardiac changes (Thayer and Lane, 2009), the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Schulz, 2016) and the execution of externally and internally oriented attention changes (Petersen and Posner, 2012). Some brain regions are part of more than one network. These brain regions provide functional and structural connections between the networks (Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). Most connections are provided by the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula (Medford and Critchley, 2010). These connections make the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula to central network hubs that coordinate the interplay between the networks (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Sridharan et al., 2008). The anterior cingulate cortex and the insula monitor and regulate the activity of all brain regions in the networks (Barrett and Simmons, 2015), which explains why activity changes in the anterior cingulate cortex and insula are closely associated with vagally mediated heart rate variability (Chang et al., 2013; Allen et al., 2015; Jennings et al., 2016), heartbeat detection (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005; Kuehn et al., 2016) and attention (Seeley et al., 2007; Eckert et al., 2009; Shulman et al., 2009). Activity changes in the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula may, thus, trigger executive control processes that mediate the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection.

To illustrate the importance of the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula for mediating the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection through executive control processes, we only have to take a look at some of the most common mental disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010). Mood and anxiety disorders are characterized by alterations in vagally mediated heart rate variability (Licht et al., 2008, 2009), heartbeat detection (Pollatos et al., 2009; Dunn et al., 2010) and executive control processes (Mogg et al., 1992, 1995; Lim and Kim, 2005). The alterations in vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection are related to alterations in anterior cingulate cortex and insula activity during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Caseras et al., 2013; Avery et al., 2014; Wiebking et al., 2015; Cui et al., 2020; DeVille et al., 2020), presumably via alterations in executive control processes (Mitterschiffthaler et al., 2008; Etkin et al., 2010). The alterations in anterior cingulate cortex and insula activity account for severe alterations in emotion, cognition and behavior (Caseras et al., 2013; Avery et al., 2014; Wiebking et al., 2015; Cui et al., 2020; DeVille et al., 2020), indicating that interoceptive deficits play an important role in the etiology and pathogenesis of mood and anxiety disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010).

Considering the importance of interoceptive deficits for the etiology and pathogenesis of mood and anxiety disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010), we need measures that allow us to identify those of us whose interoceptive deficits put them at risk for these disorders. As we have shown, cardiac measures may be useful for this purpose. We combined a cardiac measure of interoceptive accuracy, the heartbeat detection index, with a cardiac measure of prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement, the vagally mediated heart rate variability index. Combining these measures allowed us to demonstrate that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our prefrontal and (para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. It should be noted, however, that we only employed cardiac but not neural measures in our investigation. We, thus, can only assume that vagally mediated heart rate variability reflected prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. Future investigations that supplement cardiac measures with neural measures may help to test this assumption with more rigor (e.g., measuring vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection during functional or structural imaging). Future investigations should also employ a more rigorous control of participant characteristics that affect cardiac measures than we did in our investigation (e.g., excluding participants with medication use or caffeine use). We hope that our investigation opens an avenue for these types of investigations because we believe that cardiac measures are a promising tool for researchers in the field of interoception. Cardiac measures can be obtained from unobtrusive heart rate recordings that do not require dedicated staff or equipment. These measures may, thus, be interesting for researchers who need to investigate the association between interoception and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in a time- and cost-efficient manner.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to AL, alexander.lischke@uni-greifswald.

Ethics Statement

This study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Rostock. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in the study.

Author Contributions

AL, RP, and MW designed the study. AM-M and MW collected the data. AL and MW analyzed the data. AL and RP wrote the manuscript. AM-M, MW, and RP contributed to writing, reviewing, and editing of the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

Funding for this study was supported by an Open Access Publishing grant that was provided by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the University of Rostock. AL was supported by a grant provided by the DFG (LI 2517/2-1). The funding source had no further role in the design of the study, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data; in the writing of the manuscript; and in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ 1HBDSC=13(1-(|nheartbeatsreal-nheartbeatscounted|)nheartbeatsreal)
  2. ^ 2HBDGF=13(1-(|nheartbeatsreal-nheartbeatscounted|)(nheartbeatsreal+nheartbeatscounted)/2)

References

Allen, B., Jennings, J. R., Gianaros, P. J., Thayer, J. F., and Manuck, S. B. (2015). Resting high-frequency heart rate variability is related to resting brain perfusion. Psychophysiology 52, 277–287. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avery, J. A., Drevets, W. C., Moseman, S. E., Bodurka, J., Barcalow, J. C., and Simmons, W. K. (2014). Major depressive disorder is associated with abnormal interoceptive activity and functional connectivity in the insula. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 419–429. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnemeier, H., Richardt, G., Potratz, J., Wiegand, U. K., Brandes, A., Kluge, N., et al. (2003). Circadian profile of cardiac autonomic nervous modulation in healthy subjects: differing effects of aging and gender on heart rate variability. J. Cardiovasc. Electrophysiol. 14, 791–799. doi: 10.1046/j.1540-8167.2003.03078.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336–349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caseras, X., Murphy, K., Mataix-Cols, D., Lopez-Sola, M., Soriano-Mas, C., Ortriz, H., et al. (2013). Anatomical and functional overlap within the insula and anterior cingulate cortex during interoception and phobic symptom provocation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1220–1229. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C., Metzger, C. D., Glover, G. H., Duyn, J. H., Heinze, H. J., and Walter, M. (2013). Association between heart rate variability and fluctuations in resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage 68, 93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, H., Zhang, B., Li, W., Li, H., Pang, J., Hu, Q., et al. (2020). Insula shows abnormal task-evoked and resting-state activity in first-episode drug-naive generalized anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety 37, 632–644. doi: 10.1002/da.23009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dantas, E. M., Kemp, A. H., Andreao, R. V., da Silva, V. J. D., Brunoni, A. R., Hoshi, R. A., et al. (2018). Reference values for short-term resting-state heart rate variability in healthy adults: Results from the Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health-ELSA-Brasil study. Psychophysiology 55:e13052. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVille, D. C., Kuplicki, R., Stewart, J. L., Tulsa, I., Aupperle, R. L., Bodurka, J., et al. (2020). Diminished responses to bodily threat and blunted interoception in suicide attempters. Elife 9:51593.

Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 104, 11073–11078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Stefanovitch, I., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Hawkins, A., and Dalgleish, T. (2010). Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1133–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckert, M. A., Menon, V., Walczak, A., Ahlstrom, J., Denslow, S., Horwitz, A., et al. (2009). At the heart of the ventral attention system: the right anterior insula. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2530–2541. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., Prater, K. E., Hoeft, F., Menon, V., and Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Failure of anterior cingulate activation and connectivity with the amygdala during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 545–554. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagius, J., and Karhuvaara, S. (1989). Sympathetic activity and blood pressure increases with bladder distension in humans. Hypertension 14, 511–517. doi: 10.1161/01.hyp.14.5.511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using GPower 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/brm.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fustos, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., and Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 911–917. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and Jazaieri, H. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: An affective science perspective. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 387–401. doi: 10.1177/2167702614536164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., and Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 48, 263–274. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00073-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, N., McGowan, J., Minati, L., and Critchley, H. D. (2013). Emotional regulation and bodily sensation: interoceptive awareness is intact in borderline personality disorder. J. Pers. Disord. 27, 506–518. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2012_26_049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, B. M., Herbert, C., and Pollatos, O. (2011). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J. Pers. 79, 1149–1175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00717.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holzman, J. B., and Bridgett, D. J. (2017). Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74(Pt A), 233–255. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, J. R., Sheu, L. K., Kuan, D. C., Manuck, S. B., and Gianaros, P. J. (2016). Resting state connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex covaries with individual differences in high-frequency heart rate variability. Psychophysiology 53, 444–454. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 84, 394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuehn, E., Mueller, K., Lohmann, G., and Schuetz-Bosbach, S. (2016). Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1555–1571. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-0989-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Licht, C. M., de Geus, E. J., van Dyck, R., and Penninx, B. W. (2009). Association between anxiety disorders and heart rate variability in The Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Psychosom. Med. 71, 508–518. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e3181a292a6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Licht, C. M., de Geus, E. J., Zitman, F. G., Hoogendijk, W. J., van Dyck, R., and Penninx, B. W. (2008). Association between major depressive disorder and heart rate variability in the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65, 1358–1367. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, S. L., and Kim, J. H. (2005). Cognitive processing of emotional information in depression, panic, and somatoform disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 50–61. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.114.1.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Jacksteit, R., Mau-Moeller, A., Pahnke, R., Hamm, A. O., and Weippert, M. (2018a). Heart rate variability is associated with psychosocial stress in distinct social domains. J. Psychosom. Res. 106, 56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Mau-Moeller, A., Jacksteit, R., Pahnke, R., Hamm, A. O., and Weippert, M. (2018b). Heart rate variability is associated with social value orientation in males but not females. Sci. Rep. 8:7336.

Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Pahnke, R., Mau-Moeller, A., Jacksteit, R., and Weippert, M. (2020a). Sex-specific relationships between interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:67. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Weippert, M., Mau-Moeller, A., Jacksteit, R., and Pahnke, R. (2020b). Interoceptive accuracy is associated with emotional contagion in a valence- and sex-dependent manner. Soc. Neurosci. 15, 227–233. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2019.1690573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Weippert, M., Mau-Moeller, A., Paschke, S., Jacksteit, R., Hamm, A. O., et al. (2019). Sex-specific associations between inter-individual differences in heart rate variability and inter-individual differences in emotion regulation. Front. Neurosci. 12:1040. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.01040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medford, N., and Critchley, H. D. (2010). Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 535–549. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., Williams, S. C., Walsh, N. D., Cleare, A. J., Donaldson, C., Scott, J., et al. (2008). Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression. Psychol. Med. 38, 247–256. doi: 10.1017/s0033291707001523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., and Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 34, 17–36. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01434.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., Mathews, A., and Eysenck, M. (1992). Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states. Cogn. Emot. 6, 149–159. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, A. P., Friston, K. J., Low, D. A., Mathias, C. J., and Critchley, H. D. (2018). Investigating the relationship between cardiac interoception and autonomic cardiac control using a predictive coding framework. Auton. Neurosci. 210, 65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2010). Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 451–463. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 54–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Schandry, R., Auer, D. P., and Kaufmann, C. (2007a). Brain structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive awareness. Brain Res. 1141, 178–187. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E., and Schandry, R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depress Anxiety 26, 167–173. doi: 10.1002/da.20504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E., Schroeder, H., and Schandry, R. (2007b). Interoceptive awareness mediates the relationship between anxiety and the intensity of unpleasant feelings. J. Anxiety Disord. 21, 931–943. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, P., Andriesse, G. I., Oey, P. L., Wieneke, G. H., Roelofs, J. M., and Akkermans, L. M. (1998). Stomach distension increases efferent muscle sympathetic nerve activity and blood pressure in healthy humans. J. Neurol. Sci. 161, 148–155. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(98)00276-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz Vargas, E., Soros, P., Shoemaker, J. K., and Hachinski, V. (2016). Human cerebral circuitry related to cardiac control: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Ann. Neurol. 79, 709–716. doi: 10.1002/ana.24642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18, 483–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, S. M. (2016). Neural correlates of heart-focused interoception: a functional magnetic resonance imaging meta-analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160018. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., and Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, P., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2017). From heart to mind: Linking interoception, emotion, and theory of mind. Cortex 93, 220–223. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, P., Hall, R., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia, not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex 81, 215–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., Franke, D., Pope, D. L., Snyder, A. Z., McAvoy, M. P., et al. (2009). Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. J. Neurosci. 29, 4392–4407. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5609-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siennicka, A., Quintana, D. S., Fedurek, P., Wijata, A., Paleczny, B., Ponikowska, B., et al. (2019). Resting heart rate variability, attention and attention maintenance in young adults. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 143, 126–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Thayer, J. F., Khalsa, S. S., and Lane, R. D. (2017). The hierarchical basis of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 75, 274–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorensen, L., Wass, S., Osnes, B., Schanche, E., Adolfsdottir, S., Svendsen, J. L., et al. (2019). A psychophysiological investigation of the interplay between orienting and executive control during stimulus conflict: A heart rate variability study. Physiol. Behav. 211:112657. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 105, 12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarvainen, M. P., Niskanen, J. P., Lipponen, J. A., Ranta-Aho, P. O., and Karjalainen, P. A. (2014). Kubios HRV–heart rate variability analysis software. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 113, 210–220.

Google Scholar

Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (1996). Heart rate variability standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur. Heart J. 17, 354–381. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.eurheartj.a014868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Ahs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J. III, and Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Sollers, J. J. III, Ruiz-Padial, E., and Vila, J. (2002). Estimating respiratory frequency from autoregressive spectral analysis of heart period. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 21, 41–45. doi: 10.1109/memb.2002.1032638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weippert, M., Behrens, K., Rieger, A., Kumar, M., and Behrens, M. (2015). Effects of breathing patterns and light exercise on linear and nonlinear heart rate variability. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 40, 762–768. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2014-0493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weippert, M., Kumar, M., Kreuzfeld, S., Arndt, D., Rieger, A., and Stoll, R. (2010). Comparison of three mobile devices for measuring R-R intervals and heart rate variability: Polar S810i. Suunto t6 and an ambulatory ECG system. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 779–786. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1415-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiebking, C., de Greck, M., Duncan, N. W., Tempelmann, C., Bajbouj, M., and Northoff, G. (2015). Interoception in insula subregions as a possible state marker for depression-an exploratory fMRI study investigating healthy, depressed and remitted participants. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:82. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. P., Thayer, J. F., and Koenig, J. (2016). Resting cardiac vagal tone predicts intraindividual reaction time variability during an attention task in a sample of young and healthy adults. Psychophysiology 53, 1843–1851. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamasaki, Y., Kodama, M., Matsuhisa, M., Kishimoto, M., Ozaki, H., Tani, A., et al. (1996). Diurnal heart rate variability in healthy subjects: effects of aging and sex difference. Am. J. Physiol. 271(1 Pt 2), H303–H310.

Google Scholar

Zahn, D., Adams, J., Krohn, J., Wenzel, M., Mann, C. G., Gomille, L. K., et al. (2016). Heart rate variability and self-control–A meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 115, 9–26. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: heartbeat detection, heart rate variability, interoception, attention, vagal tone, emotion

Citation: Lischke A, Pahnke R, Mau-Moeller A and Weippert M (2021) Heart Rate Variability Modulates Interoceptive Accuracy. Front. Neurosci. 14:612445. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.612445

Received: 30 September 2020; Accepted: 02 December 2020;
Published: 11 January 2021.

Edited by:

Julian F. Thayer, The Ohio State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Elena Makovac, King’s College London, United Kingdom
Veronica Dusi, University of Pavia, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Lischke, Pahnke, Mau-Moeller and Weippert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexander Lischke, alexander.lischke@uni-greifswald.de; Matthias Weippert, matthias.weippert@uni-rostock.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.