Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 29 June 2021
Sec. Perception Science

Dancing Improves Emotional Regulation in Women With Methamphetamine Use Disorder But Use of a Cycle Ergometer Does Not

\r\nQian Tao&#x;Qian Tao1†Chenping Zhang&#x;Chenping Zhang2†Xiawen Li*Xiawen Li3*
  • 1Affiliated Sport School, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 3Department of Physical Education, Shanghai University of Medicine and Health Sciences, Shanghai, China

Background: Emotional regulation is crucial to people who receive a diagnosis of methamphetamine (MA) use disorder. Although evidence that exercise improves emotional regulation is robust, little is known about whether exercise will improve emotional processing in women with MA use disorder.

Methods: In the present study, 36 women with MA use disorder aged 20 to 34 years and residing in the Drug Rehabilitation Bureau of Mogan Mountain in Zhejiang province were assigned to 1 of 2 exercise intervention groups-dancing or stationary cycling. Both types of exercise were performed at 65–75% of the maximum heart rate for 30 min. Immediately before and after the exercise bout, the participants were asked to score their feelings using a nine-point Likert scale as they viewed emotionally negative, positive, or neutral images in blocks of 20 images each, for a total of 60 images. Concurrent with viewing the images and self-rating their emotions, the women also underwent functional near-infrared spectroscopy to assess changes in brain activity.

Results: There were no significant differences in the demographic or MA use characteristics assessed for the women between the two exercise groups. We found main effect of image valence (F2,33 = 69.61, p < 0.01), significant interaction effect of time and image valence was found (F2,33 = 4.27, p < 0.05) and trend increase in the self-rated emotional scale score for viewing negative images in both groups after 30-min exercise intervention, and the dancing group presented more significant trends than cycling group. In addition, activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of dancers, but not of cyclists, while viewing negative images was significantly lower after vs. before dancing (F2,33 = 5.43, p < 0.05). This result suggested that 30 min of dancing decreased neural activity in women with MA use disorder while they viewed negative images specifically in a brain region known to guide the selection of appropriate behaviors, and to shift attention.

Taken together, the findings of this study suggest that for women with MA abuse disorder, 30 min of dancing, rather than of stationary cycling, may ameliorate negative emotional reactions by decreasing attention to negative stimuli.

Introduction

Compared to amphetamine, methamphetamine (MA) has more adverse effects on both the central nervous system and the sympathetic nervous system (Davidson et al., 2001). MA targets the dopamine transporter and can cause emotional disorders. The association between emotional disorders and MA use appears to be bidirectional. Numerous study results have suggested that people who have emotional issues have higher risk for the use of drugs, including MA. For instance, one study found that the lifetime risk of drug abuse or dependence was 6.19 times more likely for individuals with vs. without emotional issues (Grant, 1995). In addition, other studies have found that MA users have a higher proportion of emotional disorders than non-users. Semple et al. (2005) reported that approximately 40% of their participants who were MA users also had moderate to severe depression, and greater intensity of MA use was correlated with higher levels of depressive symptoms in their study, even after controlling for demographic factors. Moreover, MA users are more likely to report a need for psychiatric help (Kalechstein et al., 2000). A negative emotional state may greatly hinder drug users’ intention to withdraw from drug use and may induce drug craving (Childress et al., 1994) and drug-seeking behaviors (Uslaner et al., 1999; Baker et al., 2004), which may lead to drug-taking relapse. Thus, improving the emotional state of MA users will help prevent drug relapse.

Exercise has been shown to effectively improve an individual’s emotional state. It has been suggested that exercise may promote emotional regulation by improving emotional self-efficacy and strengthening top-down executive control (Bernstein and Mcnally, 2017). Another study has shown that exercise improves the subjective evaluation of emotional stress and helps individuals recover from negative emotions (Bartholomew et al., 2005). Neuroscience research has shown that exercise activates brain areas involved in emotional regulation, for example, the ventral and dorsal prefrontal cortex (Etkin et al., 2015; Edwards et al., 2017). Among all the exercise, cycling and dancing were suggested having reduced the attention toward drugs and increased MA-users’ vital capacity (Lv et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2019). However, whether cycling and dancing can improve emotional regulation in MA users and their different effect is still unclear.

Among the brain areas involved in emotional processing, the prefrontal cortical region has been shown by numerous studies to participate in different emotional processes. For example, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is thought to integrate affective valuation of specific stimuli and inputs from other regions associated with emotional and social processing, such as the amygdala and medial temporal lobe systems (Ochsner et al., 2002; Ongur et al., 2003; Cunningham et al., 2011) to then evaluate stimuli in a context and in a goal-dependent manner (Oya et al., 2005; Roy et al., 2012). Ochsner et al. (2004) reported that the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) are associated with responses to negative affective stimuli and that they modulate the amygdala. The frontopolar area plays an important role in complicated cognitive processes, one of which is social cognition (Burgess et al., 2007).

Based on the observations of these previous studies, the aim of the present work was to explore whether acute exercise can improve emotional processing in MA users and to investigate the mechanisms underlying any exercise-induced alteration in emotional regulation. To that end, we assessed the self-reported emotional state as well as neural activity in prefrontal lobe structures, including the OFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, dlPFC, and frontopolar area, by using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) of women with MA use disorder as they viewed positive, neutral, and negative emotional stimuli (images). The self-reported emotional state and neural activity assessments were conducted before and after participants performed 1 of 2 types of exercise for 30 min: dancing or using a cycle ergometer (a stationary bicycle).

Materials and Methods

Participants

In total, 39 women who had MA use disorder were recruited from the Drug Rehabilitation Bureau of Mogan Mountain in Zhejiang province (No. 20190811). All participants had received a diagnosis of MA use disorder as described using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fifth edition) criteria and were not dependent on other substances (such as marijuana, heroin, and cocaine). Participants also met the following criteria: (1) aged 18–35 years; (2) abstinent and receiving treatment for at least 3 months; (3) no history of mental illness, brain trauma, alcohol dependence, or other diseases that affect the structure or function of the brain, and no recent taking of any psychiatric drugs; (4) educational level of elementary school or above; and (5) less than two substance abuse withdrawal experiences. The study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Shanghai University of Sport Ethics Committee (No. 2015007). All participants provided written informed consent prior to the study.

Procedure

The participants were assigned to either the dancing group or the stationary cycling group such that their demographic and MA usage characteristics were balanced. The procedure consisted of participants viewing images on a computer screen and providing their emotional responses to those images on a self-rated emotional scale while undergoing fNIRS, both immediately before and after the 30-min exercise intervention (dancing or stationary cycling). The entire procedure took approximately 40 min for each participant to complete (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of the experimental paradigm.

Stimuli and Self-Rated Emotional Assessment

We selected 75 images from the Chinese Affective Picture System (Bai et al., 2005). Of the 75 images, 25 were considered emotionally positive, 25 emotionally neutral, and 25 emotionally negative. Prior to the start of the formal experiment, participants viewed 15 images to practice using the self-rating scale. In the formal experiment, the remaining 60 images (consisting of 20 positive, 20 neutral and 20 negative images) were viewed. The same 60 images were viewed before and after the exercise intervention. Each image was assessed for the two main dimensions of emotion, that is, its valence and arousal, by participants using a nine-point Likert self-assessment scale: a score of one indicated that the participant felt very unpleasant; five, the participant felt neutral; and nine, the participant felt very pleasant. Image valence refers to the emotional valence of images, including positive emotions, negative emotions and neutral emotions; and image arousal means how degree the emotion is aroused by the image.

Image presentation was conducted using a block design that included three blocks: one block contained 20 positive images; one block contained 20 neutral images; and one block contained 20 negative images (Figure 2). Each image was presented for 1,500 ms, with a 500–700 ms interval between image presentations in which participants viewed a plus sign displayed on the screen. Blocks were presented in random order among participants. Participants were asked to rate their feelings after viewing each image. They were given a brief rest before the next block of images was presented.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Image presentation and self-rating procedure. Blocks were presented in random order among the participants.

Acute Exercise Intervention

Two different types of acute exercise intervention were used. Participants in the dancing group were instructed to dance under the supervise of professional teacher for 30 min, and those in the stationary cycling (SH-5000U; Shu hua, Quanzhou, China) group were asked to cycle for the same amount of time. Both exercise interventions included a 5-min warm-up before the exercise and 5 min of stretching after the exercise. Individuals in both groups were asked to control their heart rate between 65–75% of their maximum heart rate based on their age. The maximum heart rate was calculated using the Tanaka formula: 208 – (0.7 × age) (Tanaka et al., 2001). A Suunto Smart Sensor heart rate monitor was used to record each participant’s heart rate.

fNIRS Data Acquisition

A multichannel, continuous wave fNIRS instrument (NIRScout, NIRx Medical Technologies LLC, Minneapolis, MN, United States) was used to monitor brain hemodynamic activity, specifically, the changes in brain tissue concentrations of oxyhemoglobin (HbO2) associated with neuronal activity and tissue perfusion, as participants viewed the selected images and self-rated their emotional responses to those images. Dual wavelength (760 nm and 850 nm) continuous-wave near-infrared diffuse tomographic measurements were obtained with an acquisition sampling rate of 7.81 Hz. We placed 8 sources and seven optical probe detectors on the scalp to assess PFC and the OFC activity using a 20-channel configuration (for details, see Wang et al., 2019). The probes were positioned according to the International 10–20 electroencephalogram system, with some adjustment to ensure that each emitter was 3 cm from its corresponding detector. The fNIRS channel locations were positioned to capture the relevant brain activity based on previous studies (Tsuzuki et al., 2007; Okamoto et al., 2009).

Hemodynamic Imaging Analyses

After acquiring the fNIRS data, we analyzed only the change in the HbO2 concentration (Δ[HbO2]) signal, rather than the deoxygenated hemoglobin signal, because the former gives a better signal-to-noise ratio than the deoxygenated hemoglobin signal (Niu et al., 2013; Schaeffer et al., 2014). The Homer2 software (MGH-Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging; Boston, MA, United States) (Huppert et al., 2009) script for MATLAB (MathWorks; Natick, MA, United States) was used to analyze [HbO2]. The fNIRS data were first converted into optical density data and then into ΔHbO2 using the modified Beer-Lambert law (Cope et al., 1988). Motion artifacts were then removed using NIRlab software. A bandpass filter between 0.01 and 0.1 HZ was applied to remove any baseline drift and physiological noise (e.g., heartbeat) after removing discontinuous shifts from the time-series data. The mean [HbO2] was calculated for the dancing group and separately for the stationary cycling group for further analysis.

Statistical Analysis

We used SPSS, version 22.0 (SPSS Inc.; Chicago, Il, United States) to assess the behavioral data. Independent-samples t-tests were used to compare the differences in characteristics between the dancing and cycling groups (e.g., age, educational level, and self-rated emotional feelings). We applied two-way repeated-measures analyses of variance to determine the main effects and interactions among the effects for the two exercise interventions, self-reported emotional dimension scores, and brain activation as assessed through hemodynamic changes (i.e., HbO2). All data are reported as means ± standard deviations (SDs). A two-sided p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Demographic and MA Use Characteristics of Participants and Exercise Intensity

All 36 female participants were included in the final analyses. Their ages ranged from 20 to 34 years (26.85 ± 4.20 years). We assigned 17 women to the dancing group and the other 19 women to the cycling group. We balanced the two groups for age, educational level, MA use duration, MA use dosage, and MA use frequency. None of those variables were found to be significantly different between the two groups (Table 1). All of the participants had no prior experience in either of the exercise, and they were assigned to each group based on their demographic and MA use characteristics, not on their preference.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and methamphetamine use characteristics of all participants, by group (N = 36).

The heart rate of each group was controlled between 65–75% of Maximum heart rate, it was 69.95 and 69.69%, respectively, in dancing group and cycling group during the exercise. There was no difference of the heart rate at baseline of both groups (72.95 ± 8.07 bpm) for dancing group and 75.11 ± 6.46 bpm for cycling group, p = 0.38), or during the exercise (132.38 ± 3.83 bpm for dancing group and 131.83 ± 4.04 bpm for cycling group, p = 0.67).

Emotional Response

Before the exercise intervention, the participants used a nine-point Likert scale to self-rate their emotional responses to 60 images, 20 of which were considered positive, 20 negative, and 20 neutral. There was a significant difference in the emotional dimension of valence among the three types of images: positive vs. negative, positive vs. neutral, and negative vs. neutral (all p < 0.001). Although no difference in the emotional dimension of arousal was found between images considered positive and negative (p = 0.28), both positive and negative images were rated by participants as being significantly higher in arousal than neutral images (p < 0.001 for both comparisons) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Valence and arousal scores of images considered positive, negative, or neutral before exercise intervention in 36 women with methamphetamine use disorder.

Two-way repeated-measures was employed to explore the main effect of time (before vs. after exercise intervention), image valence (positive, neural, or negative) and their interactions on Self-Rated Emotional Assessment Scale Score. The main effect of image valence was significant (F2,33 = 69.61, p < 0.01), specifically, participants had highest Self-Rated Emotional Assessment Scale Score after watching positive images, then were the neutral images, and had the lowest score after watching negative images. There was no main effect of time (F2,33 = 0.97, p = 0.76) on Self-Rated Emotional Assessment Scale Score.

For the Self-Rated Emotional Assessment Scale Score, a significant interaction effect of time and image valence was found (F2,33 = 4.27, p < 0.05), the participants had significant higher score after the exercise intervention when watching negative images. No interaction effect of image valence and group (F2,33 = 0.02; p = 0.98). Though there was no interaction effect of time and group (F2,33 = 0.19; p = 0.67), we found a trend increase in the self-rated emotional scale score for viewing negative images in both groups after 30-min exercise intervention, and the dancing group presented more significant trends than cycling group. By contrast, there was no such trends of both groups on the self-rated emotional scale scores for viewing positive or neutral images before and after the acute exercise intervention (Figure 3). In addition, no interaction was found among the main effects of image valence, group, or time (before vs. after exercise intervention) (F2,33 = 0.36; p = 0.70).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparison of self-rated emotional scale score for viewing different valence images before and after exercise intervention in the two groups. (A) The self-rated emotional scale score for viewing neural images before and after exercise intervention in the two groups; (B) The self-rated emotional scale score for viewing negative images before and after exercise intervention in the two groups; (C) The self-rated emotional scale score for viewing positive images before and after exercise intervention in the two groups. Though there was no interaction effect of time and group (F2,33 = 0.19; p = 0.67), we found a trend increase in the self-rated emotional scale score for viewing negative images in both groups after 30-min exercise intervention, and the dancing group presented more significant trends than cycling group.

Hemodynamic Response

The Δ[HbO2], which represents neural activation was recorded. We only found the interaction effect of time and group in channel two (F2,33 = 5.43, p < 0.05), specifically, the Δ[HbO2] was significantly decreased in channel two after dancing (vs. before dancing) when dancers viewed images associated with negative emotion (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean changes in oxyhemoglobin concentration (HbO), which represents neural activation, for channel two as participants viewed negative stimuli before and after acute exercise, by group. Channel two was positioned to capture activity in the dlPFC, bars represent participant means, and vertical lines represent standard errors of the mean. *p < 0.05.

No main effect of time (F2,33 = 3.65, p = 0.07), image valence (F2,33 = 0.043 p = 0.96) was found in all channels. There was no interaction between image valence and group (F2,33 = 0.24, p = 0.79), or between image valence and time (F2,33 = 0.28, p = 0.75) in all channels. In addition, no interaction was found among the main effects of image valence, group, or time (before vs. after exercise intervention) (F2,33 = 1.61; p = 0.22) in all channels. This result suggested that 30 min of dancing, rather than cycling significantly decreased the neural response to negative images in women with MA use disorder.

Discussion

It has been suggested that negative emotional experiences have a greater impact than positive ones, both in the short- and long-term (Baumeister et al., 2001). This postulation has been supported by data on clinical disorders, specifically, that people who have problems regulating negative emotions have much higher risk of having mental disorders than those who have problems regulating positive emotions (American Psychiatric Association, 1995). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. The main finding of our study was that acute exercise appeared to regulate emotion in women with MA use disorder by reducing the attention given to negative stimuli, both in terms of a behavioral response and brain activation. Our results indicated that 30 min of dancing had the trend in increasing participants’ self-rating of their feelings and decreased the neural activation associated with channel two when the participants viewed negative images. These results suggested that 30 min of dancing decreased an individual’s negative reaction when viewing negative images, and that this decrease may be attributable to decreased attention to negative stimuli.

Channel two was positioned to capture activity in the dlPFC, which has been suggested to be associated with attention shifting (Wager et al., 2004). Numerous studies have indicated that the dlPFC extracts important information (Wallis et al., 2001)-especially information that is behaviorally relevant (Rainer et al., 1998)-among mass data, indicating that the dlPFC is involved in information processing. Miller and Cohen (2001) hold that the dlPFC plays an important role in judging situational contexts and will guide the selection of appropriate behaviors. Thus, our finding of decreased activation in the dlPFC suggests that the women in our study were paying less attention to negative images. This decrease in attending to negative images would explain why participants had higher scores on a self-rated emotional scale after 30 min of dancing. Thus, dancing improved the feelings of women with MA use disorder by reducing their attention to negatives stimuli.

Our results also showed that not all acute exercise improved participants’ emotional states. Despite participants exercising for the same amount of time and maintaining the same heart rate level, dancing but not stationary cycling, impacted their emotions and brain activation. Previous studies have also found that different types of sports lead to different effects (Baches et al., 2018), that is, not all types of exercise improve people’s physical or mental health. For example, one study indicated that dance, tai chi, and running significantly improved olfactory function in people, whereas walking did not have the same effect (Zhang et al., 2020). The results of the difference in the two types of exercise examined in the present study may be because dancing requires more cognition than stationary cycling, and brain regions involved in cognition overlap with those involved in emotional regulation, including the PFC. Given that our study showed that not all types of exercise improve emotional regulation among women who have MA use disorder, the exercise type should be considered in future studies.

There are some limitations that should be taken into account when interpreting the results of the present study. First, there was no control group, we would suggest to have control group in future study, thus we could see the different effects of dancing and cycling on different participants cohorts. However, the aim of our study was to explore the effects of two types of acute exercise on emotional regulation in women with MA use disorder, and women were assigned to each group such that there were no differences in the baseline demographic or MA usage characteristics examined between the 2 groups. Second, we examined the effects of only an acute exercise intervention; future studies should consider longer-term exercise interventions on emotional regulation. Third, the two groups are not randomly assigned, we balanced the two groups for age, educational level, MA use duration, MA dosage, and MA use frequency to avoid the impact of these factors.

Conclusion

Emotional regulation, especially the ability to regulate negative emotions, is critical to individuals who have MA use disorder to help prevent drug relapse. The present study found that 30 min of dance exercise, but not stationary cycling, increased self-rated emotions among women with MA use disorder by reducing their attention to negative stimuli.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by ethics committee of the Shanghai University of Sport (No. 2015007). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

XL designed the experiment and final approved it. XL and QT conducted the experiment. CZ and QT analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Social Science Foundation of China (grant number 17ZDA330) and Shanghai Science and Technology Planning Project (grant number 20080502800).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1995). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Baches, J. P., Cerf, S. J. W., Morgado, R. G., Lima, M. V., Roberto, S. N., and Claudio, S. (2018). Difference in articular degeneration depending on type of sport. Rev. Bras. Ortop. 54, 509–515.

Google Scholar

Bai, L., Ma, H., Huang, Y. X., and Luo, Y. J. (2005). The development of native chinese affective picture system-a pretest in 46 college students. Chinese Mental Health J. 19, 719–722.

Google Scholar

Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., and Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol. Rev. 111, 33–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.111.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartholomew, J. B., Morrison, D., and Ciccolo, J. T. (2005). Effects of acute exercise on mood and well-being in patients with major depressive disorder. Med. Sci. Sports Exercise 37:2032. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000178101.78322.dd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., and Vohs, K. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 323–370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, E. E., and Mcnally, R. J. (2017). Acute aerobic exercise helps overcome emotion regulation deficits. Cogn. Emot. 31, 834–843. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1168284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I., and Gilbert, S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 290–298. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Childress, A. R., Ehrman, R., McLellan, A. T., MacRae, J., Natale, M., and O’Brien, C. P. (1994). Can induced moods trigger drug-related responses in opiate abuse patients? J. Subs. Abuse Treat. 11, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(94)90060-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cope, M., Delpy, D. T., Reynolds, E. O. R., Wray, S., Wyatt, J., and van der Zee, P. (1988). Methods of quantitating cerebral near infrared spectroscopy data. Adv.Exp. Med. Biol. 222, 183–189. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-9510-6_21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, W. A., Johnsen, I. R., and Waggoner, A. S. (2011). Orbitofrontal cortex provides cross-modal valuation of self-generated stimuli. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 6, 286–293. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, C., Gow, A. J., Lee, T. H., and Ellinwood, E. H. (2001). Methamphetamine neurotoxicity: necrotic and apoptotic mechanisms and relevance to human abuse and treatment. Brain Res. 36, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00054-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. K., Rhodes, R. E., and Loprinzi, P. D. (2017). A randomized control intervention investigating the effects of acute exercise on emotional regulation. Am. J. Health Behav. 41, 534–543. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.41.5.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., Büchel, C., and Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, B. F. (1995). Comorbidity between DSM–IV drug use disorders and major depression: results of a national survey of adults. J. Subs. Abuse 7, 481–497. doi: 10.1016/0899-3289(95)90017-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., and Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Appl. Opt. 48:D280.

Google Scholar

Kalechstein, A. D., Newton, T. F., Longshore, D., Anglin, M. D., van Gorp, W. G., and Gawin, F. H. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity of methamphetamine dependence in a forensic sample. J. Neuropsychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 12, 480–484. doi: 10.1176/jnp.12.4.480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lv, Y., Zhao, Q., and Zhou, C. (2019). “Rehabilitation dances reduces the attentional bias for drugs of female methamphetamine disorders,” in Proceedings of the 11th National Sports Science Conference, (Nanjing).

Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niu, H. J., Li, X., Chen, Y. J., Ma, C., Zhang, J. Y., and Zhang, Z. J. (2013). Reduced frontal activation during a working memory task in mild cognitive impairment: a non-invasive near-infrared spectroscopy study. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 19, 125–131. doi: 10.1111/cns.12046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1215–1229. doi: 10.1162/089892902760807212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23, 483–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okamoto, M., Tsuzuki, D., Clowney, L., Dan, H., Singh, A. K., and Dan, I. (2009). Structural atlas-based spatial registration for functional near-infrared spectroscopy enabling inter-study data integration. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1320–1328. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ongur, D., Ferry, A. T., and Price, J. L. (2003). Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 460, 425–449. doi: 10.1002/cne.10609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oya, H., Adolphs, R., Kawasaki, H., Bechara, A., Damasio, A., and Howard, M. A. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of reward prediction error recorded in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 8351–8356. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500899102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainer, G., Asaad, W. F., and Miller, E. K. (1998). Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature 393, 577–579. doi: 10.1038/31235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, M., Shohamy, D., and Wager, T. D. (2012). Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaeffer, J. D., Yennu, A. S., Gandy, K. C., Tian, F., Liu, H., and Park, H. (2014). An fNIRS investigation of associative recognition in the prefrontal cortex with a rapid event-related design. Neurosci. Methods 235, 308–315. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semple, S. J., Zians, J., Grant, I., and Patterson, T. L. (2005). Impulsivity and methamphetamine use. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 29, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, H., Monahan, K. D., and Seals, D. R. (2001). Age-predicted maximal heart rate revisited. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 37, 153–156. doi: 10.1016/s0735-1097(00)01054-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuzuki, D., Jurcak, V., Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Watanabe, E., and Dan, I. (2007). Virtual spatial registration of stand-alone fNIRS data to MNI space. Neuroimage 34, 1506–1518. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uslaner, J., Kalechstein, A., Richter, T., Ling, W., and Newton, T. (1999). Association of depressive symptoms during abstinence with the subjective high produced by cocaine. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 1444–1446.

Google Scholar

Wager, T. D., Jonides, J., and Reading, S. (2004). Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 22, 1679–1693. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallis, J. D., Anderson, K. C., and Miller, E. K. (2001). Single Neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature 411, 953–956. doi: 10.1038/35082081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Chen, Y., Li, X., Wang, J., Zhou, Y., and Zhou, C. (2019). Moderate-Intensity aerobic exercise restores appetite and prefrontal brain activity to images of food among persons dependent on methamphetamine: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:400. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J., Tian, X., and Chen, Z. (2019). “Effects of cycling on the cardiopulmonary function of drug addicts,” in Proceedings of the 11th National Sports Science Conference, (Nanjing).

Google Scholar

Zhang, C., Li, D., and Wang, X. (2020). Role of physical exercise type in olfactory deterioration in ageing. Rhinology 58, 145–151. doi: 10.4193/Rhin19.274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, HbO2 concentration, negative images, emotional regulation, dancing

Citation: Tao Q, Zhang C and Li X (2021) Dancing Improves Emotional Regulation in Women With Methamphetamine Use Disorder But Use of a Cycle Ergometer Does Not. Front. Neurosci. 15:629061. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.629061

Received: 13 November 2020; Accepted: 04 June 2021;
Published: 29 June 2021.

Edited by:

Rufin VanRullen, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Reviewed by:

Jennifer L. Scheid, Daemen College, United States
Kazuhiko Yamamuro, Nara Medical University, Japan

Copyright © 2021 Tao, Zhang and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiawen Li, siwenli@yeah.net

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.