Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 09 September 2022
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Effects of Hormonal Contraceptives on the Brain View all 15 articles

The attention-emotion interaction in healthy female participants on oral contraceptives during 1-week escitalopram intake

\r\nNathalie Beinhlzl,*&#x;Nathalie Beinhölzl1,2*†Ein N. Molloy,,&#x;Eóin N. Molloy1,3,4†Rachel G. Zsido,,&#x;Rachel G. Zsido1,2,5†Thalia Richter,Thalia Richter6,7Fabian A. Piecha,Fabian A. Piecha1,2Gergana Zheleva,Gergana Zheleva1,2Ulrike Scharrer,Ulrike Scharrer1,2Ralf Regenthal&#x;Ralf Regenthal8†Arno Villringer,,,Arno Villringer2,5,9,10Hadas Okon-Singer,&#x;Hadas Okon-Singer6,7†Julia Sacher,,,*&#x;Julia Sacher1,5,9,11*†
  • 1Emotion and Neuroimaging Lab, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 2Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 3University Clinic for Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Otto Von Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 4German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 5Max Planck School of Cognition, Leipzig, Germany
  • 6Department of Psychology, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
  • 7The Integrated Brain and Behavior Research Center (IBBRC), University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
  • 8Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Rudolf Boehm Institute of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
  • 9Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University Hospital Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
  • 10Berlin School of Mind and Brain, MindBrainBody Institute, Charité—Berlin University of Medicine and Humboldt University Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 11Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Helios Park Hospital Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany

Previous findings in healthy humans suggest that selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) modulate emotional processing via earlier changes in attention. However, many previous studies have provided inconsistent findings. One possible reason for such inconsistencies is that these studies did not control for the influence of either sex or sex hormone fluctuations. To address this inconsistency, we administered 20 mg escitalopram or placebo for seven consecutive days in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled design to sixty healthy female participants with a minimum of 3 months oral contraceptive (OC) intake. Participants performed a modified version of an emotional flanker task before drug administration, after a single dose, after 1 week of SSRI intake, and after a 1-month wash-out period. Supported by Bayesian analyses, our results do not suggest a modulatory effect of escitalopram on behavioral measures of early attentional-emotional interaction in female individuals with regular OC use. While the specific conditions of our task may be a contributing factor, it is also possible that a practice effect in a healthy sample may mask the effects of escitalopram on the attentional-emotional interplay. Consequently, 1 week of escitalopram administration may not modulate attention toward negative emotional distractors outside the focus of attention in healthy female participants taking OCs. While further research in naturally cycling females and patient samples is needed, our results represent a valuable contribution toward the preclinical investigation of antidepressant treatment.

Introduction

Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are the first line pharmacological treatment for major depressive and anxiety disorders (Cleare et al., 2015; Hieronymus et al., 2018). While the key molecular mechanisms of SSRI action are relatively well understood, with occupancy of the serotonin transporter (Artigas et al., 2002), resulting in an upregulation of serotonin neurotransmission, the specific mechanism by which SSRIs improve mood still remains unclear. Moreover, though upregulation of serotonin neurotransmission occurs within a relatively short timeframe, clinically relevant changes in mood often take up to several weeks to manifest (Frazer and Benmansour, 2002). Recent findings in healthy participants (Harmer et al., 2004, 2011) and depressed patients (Harmer et al., 2009b; Roiser et al., 2012) have shed some light on this apparent temporal inconsistency by suggesting that SSRIs may modulate implicit emotional and social processing via earlier changes in selective attention (Harmer et al., 2009a). Critically, these attentional changes are hypothesized to precede improvements in mood via modulated processing of emotional stimuli and other downstream neuroadaptive effects (Harmer et al., 2017). In particular, evidence suggests that antidepressant action may be attributed to early changes in biased orienting of attention to aversive stimuli, a process that rebalance negative affective bias by increasing the relative recognition of positive over negative stimuli. On this account, the proposed delay is largely mediated by translation of changes in emotional bias to improved mood. While this model is promising, findings are inconsistent and remains to be fully evaluated.

Several lines of evidence have provided support for this attentional-emotional interplay. For example, evidence from neuroimaging studies indicates an SSRI-induced alteration in limbic blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) activation in response to emotional stimuli in healthy participants, showing a decrease of activation toward aversive stimuli (Harmer et al., 2006; Murphy et al., 2009a; Sladky et al., 2015), and an increase in response to positive stimuli (Norbury et al., 2009; Outhred et al., 2014). More recent research has presented evidence that a single 20 mg dose of escitalopram can blunt neural responses to averse stimuli in health (Lewis et al., 2021). Moreover, attentional networks are modulated by SSRI-administration, specifically, by increasing the engagement of the medial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (Fales et al., 2009; Ma, 2015), regions known for facilitating selective attention (Ochsner and Gross, 2005). Additionally, attentional bias training shows changes in emotional processing, measured by reduced amygdala reactivity to aversive information and changes in amygdala-prefrontal cortex connectivity (Cohen et al., 2016). Behavioral findings, however, remain comparatively inconsistent. While some studies have shown altered processing of emotional stimuli following subacute SSRI intake (Harmer et al., 2003, 2004), others have reported contrary outcomes, and did not find an effect of SSRI-administration on processing of affective stimuli (Skandali et al., 2018) nor on inhibitory performance and response re-engagement (Drueke et al., 2010) in healthy humans. Browning et al. (2007b), for example, reported that acute administration of 20 mg citalopram may draw participants attention toward positive words, but also enhances recognition of anxiety-related stimuli in healthy volunteers. Additionally, further findings also show that 7 days of 20 mg citalopram induces a decrease in memory for negative information in health (Browning et al., 2011). Another study, however, support the reversing effect of SSRIs on cognitive biases, showing that 1 week of 20 mg citalopram reduces attentional orienting to threatening stimuli, in contrast to reboxetine, a selective noradrenergic reuptake inhibitor (Murphy et al., 2009b). Rose et al. (2006) reported that 7 days administration of 10 mg escitalopram did not have a significant influence on cognitive flexibility, auditory selective attention, verbal learning and recall. Consequently, findings in healthy subjects have been inconclusive, thus presenting a significant barrier toward translating these results to patient samples.

These mixed findings may be a result of varying study designs or other overlooked contributing factors, such as small sample sizes, statistical power, selection bias, and the various distinct methodological approaches between studies. One largely overlooked variable, however, for example is sex. In a systematic review of 51 placebo-controlled trials in healthy participants (Knorr et al., 2019), many studies exhibited unequal sex distribution (62% male and 38% female) and the reported findings were thus largely contradictory. This disparity in sex among participants may be a significant contributing factor, given that sex, sex hormones, and the menstrual cycle are known to modulate both serotonergic signaling (Barth et al., 2015) and SSRI responsivity (LeGates et al., 2019). Moreover, menstrual cycle phase influences the recognition of emotional stimuli (Derntl et al., 2008; Gasbarri et al., 2008; Guapo et al., 2009), state of arousal (Goldstein et al., 2005), and attention (Pilarczyk et al., 2019). Therefore, it is necessary to control for these potential modulating factors, for example by choosing just female participants with regular use of oral contraceptives (OCs). Given that no study to date has taken this approach, it is not clear whether the hypothesis of an SSRI induced modulation of earlier attentional processing is reproducible in a well-powered, healthy female sample with regular OC intake.

The aim of the current study, therefore, is to test whether 7 days of SSRI administration modulates early attentional orienting during emotional processing in healthy female individuals using OCs. In so choosing a healthy sample, we aimed to refine applicability of this hypothesis of SSRI action (Harmer and Cowen, 2013) to an overlooked, yet substantial demographic of SSRI users, with a view toward enhancing applicability to patient samples. To assess the effects of SSRI intake on the interaction between attentional and emotional processing, we administered 20 mg of escitalopram or placebo for 1 week (with an additional assessment after a 1-month wash-out period). We chose 20 mg escitalopram due to its robust blockade of up to 80% of the serotonin transporter (Klein et al., 2006), relatively fast onset of action (Sanchez et al., 2014), higher clinical efficacy (Garnock-Jones and McCormack, 2010), and higher tolerability relative to other common SSRIs (Cipriani et al., 2009). Participants performed a modified version of the emotional flanker task (mEFT) at four time points: before drug administration, after a single dose, after 7 days of SSRI intake, and after a 1-month wash-out period. Unlike previously used paradigms (Harmer et al., 2003; Drueke et al., 2009), the mEFT simultaneously combines attentional and emotional information (Lichtenstein-Vidne et al., 2012). This combination of 1-week of escitalopram intake and our tailored task conditions, therefore, was specifically designed to investigate early effects of SSRI intake on the attentional-emotional interplay in an underrepresented sample, namely, female participants with regular OC use. We hypothesized that 1-week escitalopram intake would reduce focus on negative distractor stimuli during task performance, resulting in enhanced selective attention as indicated by disengaging from negative distractors and reduced RT in case of negative distractors, compared to placebo.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the general public and the local database of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences. Eighty-eight participants were rigorously screened, and exclusions were made based on tobacco use, other medication use, presence or history of neurological or psychological disorders, body-mass index (BMI) outside the range of 18.5–25 kg/m2, alcohol abuse, or drug abuse. We screened for psychiatric and neurological health using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (non-patient version) (SCID-I) (First, 2002), the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Hamilton, 1960), the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (McCrae et al., 2005) and the Mood Spectrum Self−Report Measure (Dell’Osso et al., 2002). Electrocardiogram (ECG) recordings were used to screen for abnormal QT times. All participants were female, taking estrogen- and progesterone-combined OCs to downregulate pulse frequency of gonadotropin releasing hormone, for at least 3 months prior to participation. By suppressing levels of follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) and luteinizing hormone (LH), OC-use thus prevents follicular development and ovulation (Bastianelli et al., 2018) thereby controlling for sex hormone fluctuations during natural menstrual cycle (Mihm et al., 2011) that may modulate SSRI responsivity (LeGates et al., 2019). In addition, we strictly limited assessments to the pill-interval to control for potential hormonal effects during the pill-free interval. Participants were between 18 and 35 years of age. Seventy-one participants were enrolled, of whom 65 completed the assessment week. Six participants voluntarily discontinued participation during the assessment week. Additionally, two (placebo n = 2) participants did not complete a follow-up assessment. Of the 63 participants who completed the study, three were excluded due to QC concerns (Figure 1). All participants were monitored by a physician during the study, provided written informed consent prior to enrollment, and received financial compensation. We obtained ethical approval from the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine at Leipzig University (approval number 390/16-ek) and conducted all study procedures in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki of 2013.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Inclusions and exclusions: The figure depicts the number of participants included in each step of enrollment, assessment, and analysis. Due to self-reported side effects, 6 participants voluntarily discontinued participation during the assessment week (placebo = 2, escitalopram = 4). Two participants did not return for the follow-up assessment. Three participants (placebo = 1, escitalopram = 2) were excluded following implementation of quality control measures.

Study design

We administered 20 mg of escitalopram (n = 29) or placebo (n = 31) to healthy female participants for 7 days. During the administration week, we assessed behavioral responses to an emotional distractor task. Task performance was initially assessed at baseline, prior to escitalopram or placebo intake. Following the baseline measurement, participants were randomly assigned to receive either escitalopram or placebo using a 1:1 allocation method. Both the participant and the experimenter were blind to condition allocation. Behavioral performances were subsequently assessed after a single dose (Day 1), and again following the third assessment, which took place after 7 days (Day 7). Escitalopram and placebo intake occurred at fixed times each day. All participants returned for a follow-up assessment following a 4–6-week follow-up, in which no escitalopram or placebo was administered. ECG recordings were conducted at Days 1, 4, 7, and follow-up, to monitor QT intervals. Adverse reactions to escitalopram were recorded using the antidepressant side-effects checklist (ASEC) (Uher et al., 2009). Changes in mood and anxiety were recorded with different psychological inventories: the POMS (German language version of the profile of mood spectrum) (McNair et al., 1981), a validated (Gibson, 1997) self-report measure assessing various mood status, the DAS (Dysfunctional attitude scale) (Weissman and Beck, 1978), a validated (Weissman, 1979; de Graaf et al., 2009) questionnaire to assess negative attitudes and cognitive vulnerability and the STAI (State trait anxiety index) (Spielberger et al., 1983) validated (Guillén-Riquelme and Buela-Casal, 2014) measuring both state and trait anxiety, transient reactions in specific situations, and specific attributes of personality.

Task procedure and primary outcome measure

We assessed emotional and attentional responses using a mEFT (Lichtenstein-Vidne et al., 2012), implemented in EPrime 2.0 professional (Stahl, 2006) running on the Windows XP operating system. Visual task stimuli were obtained from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Bradley et al., 2001). The experiment contained 3 blocks of 32 trials each, resulting in 96 trials. Participants were instructed to indicate whether a target picture appeared above or below a fixation cross, via a keypress, while ignoring flanker images. In each trial, peripheral distracting pictures contained negative or neutral emotional valences while targets were either negative or positive. Participants were asked to respond as quickly and accurately as possible. The outcome measure was reaction time (RT), measured in milliseconds (ms), and calculated as the difference between the time of target onset and participant response. Three independent within-subject variables were manipulated: flanker location congruency with the target picture (congruent, incongruent), flanker valence (negative, neutral), and target valence (negative, positive) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Visual stimulus of the emotional distractor task. Depicted here is an example of a typical trial. Participants view a fixation cross which is then replaced by a second fixation cross in a 9-panel grid. Both the target picture and the distractors subsequently appear, prompting participants to indicate the target picture location, while ignoring distracting flanker pictures. The different conditions of this panel are shown: flankers were neutral or negative (A) and incongruent or congruent to the target pictures (B); targets were either positively or negatively valenced (C). Target location was either above or below the previously shown fixation cross. Ms, milliseconds. Images are from The International Affective Picture System (IAPS).

Data analysis

Demographics and mood assessment

Statistical analyses were conducted with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, v24). Peripheral plasma escitalopram levels were quantified by high-performance chromatography using quality control (QC) sample (Teichert et al., 2020). Moreover, we measured plasma levels of FSH and LH to assess regular OC-induced suppression of these hormones. We used independent sample t-tests to assess potential group differences in age, BMI, and downregulated hormonal profiles. ASEC scores at both single dose and steady state were also assessed with independent sample t-tests. Potential changes in mood and anxiety, as recorded by the DAS, STAI, and POMS were each assessed separately using a 2 × 3 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group and time as factors. Results of mood and anxiety analyses were considered statistically significant at a Bonferroni-corrected α-level of p < 0.016 to account for multiple testing (0.05/3).

Data preprocessing and quality control

We preprocessed all data using EPrime (version 2.0). Each assessment contained 10 practice trials that were excluded during pre-processing. To identify outlier trials, we calculated the mean and standard deviation for each trial. Individual outlier trials were removed with SPSS, using a cutoff of ± 2.5 standard deviations from the mean (2% of all trials for all participants). Additionally, only trials in which a correct response was given were included (98% of all trials of all participants).

Validation of task performance

Prior to analyses comparing SSRI and placebo conditions, we performed a control analysis to confirm that participants understood task instructions. To this end, we assessed performance at baseline in all participants who completed this measurement (n = 71) and passed data QC, without reference to group allocation or to further exclusion. In line with previous studies with this task (Lichtenstein-Vidne et al., 2012), we specified an ANOVA to test for a main effect of congruency, a main effect of flanker valence, and for an interaction between congruency and target valence.

Analysis of attentional and valence-dependent task performance

To test our main hypothesis, we employed a five-way repeated measures mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) in SPSS. Here, we specified 5 independent variables; group (escitalopram, placebo), time (baseline, Day 1, Day 7, Follow-up), target valence (positive, negative), flanker valence (negative, neutral), and congruency (congruent, incongruent). We specified group as the between subjects factor, time as the within subjects factor and RT in each of the target, flanker valence, and congruency conditions as the dependent variable.

Correlation between peripheral plasma escitalopram levels and modified version of the emotional flanker task performance

We tested potential correlations between peripheral measures of plasma escitalopram and behavioral performance using a bivariate Pearson’s correlation implemented in SPSS. Here we correlated peripheral plasma levels acquired at day 7 of escitalopram intake with the mean performance in task performance during each of the valence and congruency conditions in the escitalopram group only (n = 29). We employed 6 models to assess a potential association between (i) congruency (congruent and incongruent RT), (ii) flanker valence (Negative and Neutral RT) and (iii) target valence conditions (Negative and Positive RT). Results were considered statistically significant at a Bonferroni corrected threshold of p < 0.008 (0.05/6) to account for multiple testing.

Bayesian analysis

Finally, we employed a Bayesian estimation to assess the likelihood of the null hypothesis in each task condition. To this end, we used JASP (v0.12.2—JASP Team, 2020) to implement a Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA for each condition in all cognitive and valence specific analyses [i.e., one Bayesian approach for each of the congruency (congruent/incongruent), target valence (negative/positive), and flanker valence (negative/neutral) conditions], resulting in 6 Bayesian estimations in total.

Results

Demographics

Analyses of demographic variables yielded no significant group differences on any baseline control measures. Group comparisons of time between final escitalopram or placebo intake and follow-up measurement also indicated no significant differences. Escitalopram levels were consistent with previously reported data (Rao, 2007). Analyses of ASEC scores indicated a significant group difference in mean self-reported side effects at single dose (t = −3.389, p = 0.001) but not at steady state (t = −0.675, p = 0.502). Ovulation inhibition was confirmed as suppressed via gonadotropins measurement (Goldzieher et al., 1970; Table 1). Estrogen- and progesterone-combined hormonal contraceptives taken by the participants are listed in Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic analysis overview: Baseline and demographic variables across both groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Contraceptive usage.

Mood and anxiety monitoring

We did not observe group differences on measures of either (i) state anxiety [time: F(1, 55) = 1.939, p = 0.154, partial eta2 = 0.066; time by group: F(1, 55) = 2.941; p = 0.061; partial eta2 = 0.097; group: F(1, 1) = 0.665, p = 0.418, partial eta2 = 0.012], or (ii) mood [DAS: time: F(1, 59) = 2.374, p = 0.102, partial eta2 = 0.074; time by group: F(1, 59) = 1.024, p = 0.365, partial eta2 = 0.034; group: F(1, 1) = 0.812, p = 0.371; partial eta2 = 0.013]; POMs [time: F(1, 60) = 7.207, p = 0.002, partial eta2 = 0.194; time by group: F(1, 60) = 1.145, p = 0.325, partial eta2 = 0.037; group: F(1, 1) = 0.396, p = 0.531, partial eta2 = 0.006].

Baseline replication analysis

Analysis of baseline (n = 71) performance yielded an outcome comparable to previous studies, with a significant main effect for location congruency [F(1, 70) = 42.306; p < 0.001; partial eta2 = 0.377], represented by faster RTs in congruent vs. incongruent trials. Additionally, results showed a congruency by flanker valence interaction [F(1, 70) = 4.570; p = 0.036; partial eta2 = 0.061], with slower RTs in congruent trials during negative flankers as compared to the neutral condition. In contrast to previous findings, however, we did not observe a significant effect of distractor valence [F(1, 70) = 1.758; p = 0.189; partial eta2 = 0.024] nor target valence [F(1, 70) = 2.942; p = 0.091; partial eta2 = 0.040].

Task performance analysis over time

Five-way analyses of group performance over time

Our results show a significant effect of time [F(1, 3) = 33.163; p < 0.001; partial eta2 = 0.364], with decreased RTs during performance of each target valence, flanker valence, and congruency condition over the course of the experiment. However, there was no significant group by time effect [F(1, 3) = 0.128; p = 0.943; partial eta2 = 0.002]. Analysis of a congruency effect show significance [F(1, 1) = 127.753; p < 0.001; partial eta2 = 0.688], but we did not observe a significant group by congruency interaction [F(1, 1) = 0.071; p = 0.791; partial eta2 = 0.001]. Complimentary analysis of the flanker valence conditions (negative, neutral) shows no significant effect [F(1, 1) = 1.176; p = 0.283; partial eta2 = 0.020] nor an interaction effect with group [F(1, 1) = 0.007; p = 0.932; partial eta2 = 0.000]. Analysis of target valence conditions (negative, positive) yields no significant effect [F(1, 1) = 3.582; p = 0.063; partial eta2 = 0.058] and no group interaction [F(1, 1) = 0.386; p = 0.537; partial eta2 = 0.007] (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Timeline of behavioral performance during 1-week of escitalopram-intake and after a 1-month wash-out period for each task condition: We measured RT [in milliseconds (ms)] performance on the mEFT in both the escitalopram and placebo groups at baseline, single dose, after 1-week of continuous intake, and following a 1-month wash-out period. We assessed performance in each of the (A) congruent and (B) incongruent conditions, (C) the negative and (D) neutral flanker conditions, and (E) the negative target and (F) positive target conditions. While results indicate a significant effect of time, with decreasing RT between the baseline and follow-up assessments, we did not observe any indication of a difference between groups on any measure of performance.

Correlation analyses

Results of a bivariate correlation analyses within the escitalopram group only do not suggest any apparent relationship between peripheral plasma escitalopram levels, acquired at the final day of intake, with behavioral performance in each of the specific task conditions at the seventh day of intake. Across six correlation analyses for each condition within the congruency, flanker valence, and target valence conditions, peripheral plasma escitalopram did not correlate with performance (all p > 0.008) (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlation analyses at day seven of escitalopram intake for each task condition: We assessed a potential correlation between peripheral measures of plasma escitalopram and behavioral performance in each task condition category with a bivariate Pearson’s correlation. Results for the congruency condition [left column (A,D)], flanker condition [middle column (B,E)] and target condition [right column (C,F)] show no evidence of a correlation. Results considered significant at a Bonferroni-corrected p-value of < 0.008 due to multiple testing.

Bayesian analyses

A series of Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs show moderate to strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis for each cognitive and emotional behavioral outcome, compared to the alternative hypothesis (Table 3). Model comparisons show increasing likelihood in favor of the null hypothesis for each additional model contribution (i.e., of the group and group by time interaction terms). Consistent with frequentist repeated measures analyses, no evidence for the null hypothesis is observed for the time factor.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of Bayesian analysis for each cognitive and valence domain: Repeated measures Bayesian analyses show moderate to strong evidence for the null hypothesis when considering the group and group by time interaction terms for each cognitive and valence-dependent condition.

Discussion

In the present study, we assessed whether 1-week administration of escitalopram modulates the attention-emotion interaction in healthy female participants with regular OC use. Using a mEFT, we tested the hypothesis that escitalopram intake would facilitate the disengagement from negatively valenced task distractors (Harmer et al., 2009a). Against our hypothesis, our results do not suggest an effect of escitalopram on emotional and attentional distraction, either after single dose, or after 7 days continuous intake, or at a 1-month follow-up assessment. While we observed a significant improvement in task performance over time with a decrease in RT to task stimuli, there was no observed significant difference in performance between groups. Moreover, Bayesian analyses yield moderate to strong support in favor of the null, relative to the alternative, hypothesis. Consequently, these results do not suggest an effect of 1-week escitalopram intake on selective attention and inhibition of negatively valenced distractors in this sample of healthy female participants on OCs.

One possible reason for our findings may be the specificity of our task demands. Specifically, unlike previous emotional processing tasks, the mEFT combines emotional and attentional domains using peripheral distractors that simultaneously present emotional and spatial information (Lichtenstein-Vidne et al., 2012). Previous studies, however, typically employed tasks that considered these domains individually, such as a facial recognition task (Harmer et al., 2003) or an attentional network task (Drueke et al., 2009). Furthermore, previous findings were mainly obtained using tasks that present distracting emotional information inside the focus of attention, such as the emotional Stroop task and the dot-probe task. For example, Browning et al. (2007a), employed a visual probe task to show an increase in attention to socially relevant positive words after a single dose of citalopram. In another study, Murphy et al. (2009b) showed reduced attentional orienting to threatening stimuli after 1-week citalopram intake with the use of an attentional probe task. Unlike these paradigms, however, our task presented the distracting emotional information outside the focus of attention. Therefore, while previous studies provide evidence for a modulation of an attentional bias toward distracting emotional content that is presented inside the focus of attention, we investigated the effect of escitalopram on an attentional bias to task-irrelevant emotional content, when task settings do not encourage distractors’ processing. Investigating such an effect is also of special interest, considering that only a small portion of the visual stimuli in everyday life appear at the visual center (Wandell, 1995). Consequently, our results do not indicate an effect of escitalopram on selective attentional processing of task irrelevant emotional and task relevant spatial information in healthy females on OCs. Our findings are consistent with the concept that emotional processing is not automatic, but highly task and stimuli dependent. Future studies comparing the difference of task-relevant and irrelevant distractors in emotional processing tasks are required, however, to directly assess this possibility.

Another possible reason may be a product of the emotional stimuli presented as part of the mEFT. Evidence suggests that emotional recognition tasks that employ facial stimuli, as used by Ahmed et al. (2021), may be particularly sensitive to changes in emotional processing compared to that of the mEFT, which primarily presents situational stimuli. This interpretation is consistent with other findings that did not suggest an effect of linguistic stimuli (Browning et al., 2019; Ahmed et al., 2021) on emotional processing, stimuli which are thought to have a lower emotional impact relative to pictorial stimuli (Lees et al., 2005). While such illustrations have been described as being less emotionally salient compared to the real-life pictures, presented in our task (Okon-Singer et al., 2013), it is likely that facial stimuli may uniquely stimulate specific brain regions (Britton et al., 2006). In sum, our findings, while not replicating previous findings in healthy volunteers, are consistent with the hypothesis that certain emotional stimuli maybe somewhat specific to certain task demands. Larger studies with a wider array of emotionally salient stimuli are needed to further explore this possibility, however.

Given our longitudinal design and the fact that participants performed the task multiple times, our results may also show a practice or habituation effect. Though we observed no difference in performance between groups, we did observe a significant effect of time with decreased RT, suggesting that all participants responded faster over the course of the experiment. Participants were asked to respond via a simple button press, making the task relatively easy to perform. With a healthy sample showing no aberrant emotional processing (as measured by our baseline replication analysis), it is possible that participants simply adapted or habituated to the presented stimuli. One possible counterpoint, however, is that the presented stimuli where randomized across each measurement, suggesting that it is likely that different stimuli were presented at each assessment day. However, as this randomization of stimuli was random, we cannot rule out the possibility that similar stimuli were shown or repeated at different times during the experiment. As a result, the simpler nature of our task in the presence of a healthy sample may have dampened an already small effect of the SSRI, thus leading to our observed outcomes. However, it is also worth mentioning that our task procedure was specifically designed in this less complex manner, given that existing evidence has shown that increased task difficulty may conflict with emotional processing (Pessoa, 2005; Cohen et al., 2011). Regardless, a simple task contributing to habituation and practice effects in a healthy sample present a viable interpretation for our null result, an interpretation which future studies in patients and with more demanding tasks may address.

One final, though admittedly more speculative possibility, is the influence of downregulated endogenous sex hormones via OC use and the associated effects on SSRI responsivity. As we confirmed OC- induced suppression of LH and FSH in comparison to natural cycling women and to control for sex hormone fluctuations during the menstrual cycle (Mihm et al., 2011), it is arguable that this induced downregulation of endogenous sex hormones (Givens et al., 1976) may have dampened, at least in part, the effects of escitalopram on our primary outcome measure, the behavioral response to the mEFT. Evidence in favor of this possibility comes from previous studies showing endogenous estradiol modulates serotonergic transmission, SSRI responsivity and affect (Amin et al., 2005; Michopoulos et al., 2011; Ocampo Rebollar et al., 2017). However, we stress that, as we did not investigate estradiol or an interaction effect of SSRIs, OCs and time on task performance, this interpretation should be taken with great caution. Future interventional studies with multiple groups are required to explore this possibility further.

There are also several limitations to this study that should be considered. First, our results are limited to healthy participants only as we did not include any patients in our sample. This decision was deliberate, however, as we aimed to replicate previous findings in healthy participants and to extend these findings to an underrepresented demographic. While future studies in clinical populations are crucial, our analyses in healthy female participants with long term OC-use provides a much needed extension of previous preclinical studies. Secondly, we cannot exclude that a longer drug administration duration would have resulted in differential effects given that, in patients, antidepressants often take up to 3–4 weeks to exhibit clinically relevant changes in mood (Frazer and Benmansour, 2002). Nevertheless, we again explicitly chose this time frame of administration in order to test our hypothesis of earlier alterations in attentional processing in response to SSRIs, which is hypothesized to precede the changes in mood often seen at later stages of administration (Harmer and Cowen, 2013). Thirdly, the interpretation of our findings may be limited to escitalopram and thus, we cannot make any comments regarding the potentially differential effects of other SSRIs as escitalopram, unlike other common SSRIs such as paroxetine or fluoxetine, exhibits a unique allosteric binding affinity for the serotonin transporter (Klein et al., 2006). As a result, future studies are needed to assess whether our findings extend to a class-effect or are specific to escitalopram at this specific dose. Fourth, we acknowledge that more experimental groups, in which naturally cycling females are investigated with their endogenous sex hormone fluctuations, would be necessary to clarify whether OC-use, specifically, contributed to our findings. As such, we can only speculate, with great caution, that this is a contributing factor. Future studies with different SSRIs, varying doses, hormonal measures and administration regimes (Frye, 2006), and multiple groups that are specifically designed to assess this possibility, are necessary in order to further discuss this possibility. Finally, our analyses are limited to behavioral outcomes only, which differs to previous studies that assessed neural responses during combined SSRI intake and task performance (Harmer et al., 2006; Murphy et al., 2009a; Norbury et al., 2009). In contrast, we did not assess neural responses to escitalopram during mEFT performance. Future studies employing similar samples (and patient populations) should also consider functional neuroimaging to investigate regional and global effects of escitalopram intake at the neural level during attentional and emotional processing.

In conclusion, our results do not indicate an effect of 7 days escitalopram intake on attentional-emotional interaction in healthy female participants taking OCs. While these outcomes may be a result of the specific task requirements of the modified emotional flanker task, or of our chosen sample of healthy volunteers, another possible explanation may be practice effects resulting from simple task requirements. Nevertheless, our findings provide much needed data on a highly relevant, yet underrepresented sample, thus making a critical contribution toward refining the attention-emotion cognitive model of antidepressant action in healthy volunteers. As a result, these results provide a solid platform for studies in patients, with the ultimate goal of improving personalized treatment for depression.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics committee of the medical faculty of the University of Leipzig. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

NB, EM, RZ, and TR contributed to data analysis. NB, EM, RZ, TR, JS, and HO-S contributed on manuscript writing process. NB, EM, RZ, JS, HO-S, FP, US, TR, GZ, RR, and AV contributed on reviewing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Branco Weiss Fellowship—Society in Science, the National Association for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) Young Investigator Grant 25032—Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, the Minerva Research Group Grant—Max Planck Society (awarded to JS), the Doctoral Scholarship—FAZIT Foundation (awarded to EM), the Fellowship—Joachim Herz Foundation (awarded to RZ), and the Doctoral Stipend from Leipzig University (awarded to NB).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahmed, N., Bone, J. K., Lewis, G., Freemantle, N., Harmer, C. J., Duffy, L., et al. (2021). The effect of sertraline on emotional processing: Secondary analyses of the PANDA randomised controlled trial. Psychol. Med. 1–8. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720004985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amin, Z., Canli, T., and Epperson, C. N. (2005). Effect of estrogen-serotonin interactions on mood and cognition. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 4, 43–58. doi: 10.1177/1534582305277152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artigas, F., Nutt, D. J., and Shelton, R. (2002). Mechanism of action of antidepressants. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 36(Suppl. 2), 123–132.

Google Scholar

Barth, C., Villringer, A., and Sacher, J. (2015). Sex hormones affect neurotransmitters and shape the adult female brain during hormonal transition periods. Front. Neurosci. 9:37. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastianelli, C., Farris, M., Rosato, E., Brosens, I., and Benagiano, G. (2018). Pharmacodynamics of combined estrogen-progestin oral contraceptives 3. Inhibition of ovulation. Expert Rev. Clin. Pharmacol. 11, 1085–1098. doi: 10.1080/17512433.2018.1536544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., and Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion 1, 276–298. doi: 10.1037//1528-3542.1.3.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, J. C., Taylor, S. F., Sudheimer, K. D., and Liberzon, I. (2006). Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: Common and differential networks. Neuroimage 31, 906–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M., Grol, M., Ly, V., Goodwin, G. M., Holmes, E. A., and Harmer, C. J. (2011). Using an experimental medicine model to explore combination effects of pharmacological and cognitive interventions for depression and anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2689–2697. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M., Kingslake, J., Dourish, C. T., Goodwin, G. M., Harmer, C. J., and Dawson, G. R. (2019). Predicting treatment response to antidepressant medication using early changes in emotional processing. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 29, 66–75.

Google Scholar

Browning, M., Reid, C., Cowen, P. J., Goodwin, G. M., and Harmer, C. J. (2007a). A single dose of citalopram increases fear recognition in healthy subjects. J. Psychopharmacol. 7, 684–690. doi: 10.1177/0269881106074062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M., Reid, C., Cowen, P. J., Goodwin, G. M., and Harmer, C. J. (2007b). A single dose of citalopram increases fear recognition in healthy subjects. J. Psychopharmacol. 21, 684–690. doi: 10.1177/0269881106074062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipriani, A., Furukawa, T. A., Salanti, G., Geddes, J. R., Higgins, J. P., Churchill, R., et al. (2009). Comparative efficacy and acceptability of 12 new-generation antidepressants: A multiple-treatments meta-analysis. Lancet 373, 746–758. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(09)60046-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleare, A., Pariante, C. M., Young, A. H., Anderson, I. M., Christmas, D., Cowen, P. J., et al. (2015). Evidence-based guidelines for treating depressive disorders with antidepressants: A revision of the 2008 british association for psychopharmacology guidelines. J. Psychopharmacol. 29, 459–525. doi: 10.1177/0269881115581093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, N., Henik, A., and Mor, N. (2011). Can emotion modulate attention? Evidence for reciprocal links in the attentional network test. Exp. Psychol. 58, 171–179. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, N., Margulies, D. S., Ashkenazi, S., Schaefer, A., Taubert, M., Henik, A., et al. (2016). Using executive control training to suppress amygdala reactivity to aversive information. Neuroimage 125, 1022–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Graaf, L. E., Roelofs, J., and Huibers, M. J. (2009). Measuring dysfunctional attitudes in the general population: The dysfunctional attitude scale (form A) revised. Cognit. Ther. Res. 33, 345–355. doi: 10.1007/s10608-009-9229-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell’Osso, L., Armani, A., Rucci, P., Frank, E., Fagiolini, A., Corretti, G., et al. (2002). Measuring mood spectrum: Comparison of interview (SCI-MOODS) and self-report (MOODS-SR) instruments. Compr. Psychiatry 43, 69–73. doi: 10.1053/comp.2002.29852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derntl, B., Kryspin-Exner, I., Fernbach, E., Moser, E., and Habel, U. (2008). Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated with cycle phase. Horm. Behav. 53, 90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drueke, B., Baetz, J., Boecker, M., Moeller, O., Hiemke, C., Gründer, G., et al. (2009). Differential effects of escitalopram on attention: A placebo-controlled, double-blind cross-over study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 207, 213–223. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1649-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drueke, B., Boecker, M., Schlaegel, S., Moeller, O., Hiemke, C., Gründer, G., et al. (2010). Serotonergic modulation of response inhibition and re-engagement? Results of a study in healthy human volunteers. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 25, 472–480. doi: 10.1002/hup.1141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fales, C. L., Barch, D. M., Rundle, M. M., Mintun, M. A., Mathews, J., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2009). Antidepressant treatment normalizes hypoactivity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during emotional interference processing in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 112, 206–211. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B. (2002). The DSM series and experience with DSM-IV. Psychopathology 35, 67–71. doi: 10.1159/000065121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazer, A., and Benmansour, S. (2002). Delayed pharmacological effects of antidepressants. Mol. Psychiatry 7(Suppl. 1), S23–S28. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frye, C. A. (2006). An overview of oral contraceptives: Mechanism of action and clinical use. Neurology 66(6 Suppl. 3), S29–S36. doi: 10.1212/wnl.66.66_suppl_3.s29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnock-Jones, K. P., and McCormack, P. L. (2010). Escitalopram: A review of its use in the management of major depressive disorder in adults. CNS Drugs 24, 769–796. doi: 10.2165/11204760-000000000-00000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasbarri, A., Pompili, A., d’Onofrio, A., Cifariello, A., Tavares, M. C., and Tomaz, C. (2008). Working memory for emotional facial expressions: Role of the estrogen in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 964–972. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, S. J. (1997). The measurement of mood states in older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 52, 167–174. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52b.4.p167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Givens, J. R., Andersen, R. N., Wiser, W. L., Umstot, E. S., and Fish, S. A. (1976). The effectiveness of two oral contraceptives in suppressing plasma androstenedione, testosterone, LH, and FSH, and in stimulating plasma testosterone-binding capacity in hirsute women. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 124, 333–339. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(76)90089-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, J. M., Jerram, M., Poldrack, R., Ahern, T., Kennedy, D. N., Seidman, L. J., et al. (2005). Hormonal cycle modulates arousal circuitry in women using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 25, 9309–9316. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2239-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldzieher, J. W., Kleber, J. W., Moses, L. E., and Rathmacher, R. P. (1970). A cross-sectional study of plasma FSH and LH levels in women using sequential, combination or injectable steroid contraceptives over long periods of time. Contraception 2, 225–248.

Google Scholar

Guapo, V. G., Graeff, F. G., Zani, A. C., Labate, C. M., dos Reis, R. M., and Del-Ben, C. M. (2009). Effects of sex hormonal levels and phases of the menstrual cycle in the processing of emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1087–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guillén-Riquelme, A., and Buela-Casal, G. (2014). [Meta-analysis of group comparison and meta-analysis of reliability generalization of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Questionnaire (STAI)]. Rev. Esp. Salud Publica 88, 101–112. doi: 10.4321/s1135-57272014000100007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., and Cowen, P. J. (2013). ‘It’s the way that you look at it’–a cognitive neuropsychological account of SSRI action in depression. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368:20120407. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., Bhagwagar, Z., Perrett, D. I., Vollm, B. A., Cowen, P. J., and Goodwin, G. M. (2003). Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 148–152. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., de Bodinat, C., Dawson, G. R., Dourish, C. T., Waldenmaier, L., Adams, S., et al. (2011). Agomelatine facilitates positive versus negative affective processing in healthy volunteer models. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 1159–1167. doi: 10.1177/0269881110376689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., Duman, R. S., and Cowen, P. J. (2017). How do antidepressants work? New perspectives for refining future treatment approaches. Lancet Psychiatry 4, 409–418. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(17)30015-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., Goodwin, G. M., and Cowen, P. J. (2009a). Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Br. J. Psychiatry 195, 102–108. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., Mackay, C. E., Reid, C. B., Cowen, P. J., and Goodwin, G. M. (2006). Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 816–820. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., O’Sullivan, U., Favaron, E., Massey-Chase, R., Ayres, R., Reinecke, A., et al. (2009b). Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 1178–1184. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmer, C. J., Shelley, N. C., Cowen, P. J., and Goodwin, G. M. (2004). Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 1256–1263. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hieronymus, F., Lisinski, A., Nilsson, S., and Eriksson, E. (2018). Efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the absence of side effects: A mega-analysis of citalopram and paroxetine in adult depression. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1731–1736. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, N., Sacher, J., Geiss-Granadia, T., Attarbaschi, T., Mossaheb, N., Lanzenberger, R., et al. (2006). In vivo imaging of serotonin transporter occupancy by means of SPECT and [123I]ADAM in healthy subjects administered different doses of escitalopram or citalopram. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 188, 263–272. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0486-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knorr, U., Madsen, J. M., and Kessing, L. V. (2019). The effect of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in healthy subjects revisited: A systematic review of the literature. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 27, 413–432. doi: 10.1037/pha0000264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lees, A., Mogg, K., and Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health-threat cues. Cogn. Emot. 19, 453–462. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeGates, T. A., Kvarta, M. D., and Thompson, S. M. (2019). Sex differences in antidepressant efficacy. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 140–154. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0156-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, C. A., Mueller, K., Zsido, R. G., Reinelt, J., Regenthal, R., Okon-Singer, H., et al. (2021). A single dose of escitalopram blunts the neural response in the thalamus and caudate during monetary loss. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 46, E319–E327. doi: 10.1503/jpn.200121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., Henik, A., and Safadi, Z. (2012). Task relevance modulates processing of distracting emotional stimuli. Cogn. Emot. 26, 42–52. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.567055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Y. (2015). Neuropsychological mechanism underlying antidepressant effect: A systematic meta-analysis. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 311–319. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., and Martin, T. A. (2005). The NEO-PI-3: A more readable revised NEO personality inventory. J. Pers. Assess. 84, 261–270. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8403_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., and Droppelman, L. F. (1981). EdITS manual: Profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Michopoulos, V., Berga, S. L., and Wilson, M. E. (2011). Estradiol and progesterone modify the effects of the serotonin reuptake transporter polymorphism on serotonergic responsivity to citalopram. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 19, 401–408. doi: 10.1037/a0025008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mihm, M., Gangooly, S., and Muttukrishna, S. (2011). The normal menstrual cycle in women. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 124, 229–236. doi: 10.1016/j.anireprosci.2010.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, S. E., Norbury, R., O’Sullivan, U., Cowen, P. J., and Harmer, C. J. (2009a). Effect of a single dose of citalopram on amygdala response to emotional faces. Br. J. Psychiatry 194, 535–540. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.056093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, S. E., Yiend, J., Lester, K. J., Cowen, P. J., and Harmer, C. J. (2009b). Short-term serotonergic but not noradrenergic antidepressant administration reduces attentional vigilance to threat in healthy volunteers. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 169–179. doi: 10.1017/S1461145708009164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norbury, R., Taylor, M. J., Selvaraj, S., Murphy, S. E., Harmer, C. J., and Cowen, P. J. (2009). Short-term antidepressant treatment modulates amygdala response to happy faces. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 206, 197–204. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1597-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ocampo Rebollar, A., Menéndez Balaña, F. J., and Conde Pastor, M. (2017). Comparison of affect changes during the ovulatory phase in women with and without hormonal contraceptives. Heliyon 3:e00282. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2017.e00282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okon-Singer, H., Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., and Cohen, N. (2013). Dynamic modulation of emotional processing. Biol. Psychol. 92, 480–491. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Outhred, T., Das, P., Felmingham, K. L., Bryant, R. A., Nathan, P. J., Malhi, G. S., et al. (2014). Impact of acute administration of escitalopram on the processing of emotional and neutral images: A randomized crossover fMRI study of healthy women. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 39, 267–275. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 188–196. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilarczyk, J., Schwertner, E., Woloszyn, K., and Kuniecki, M. (2019). Phase of the menstrual cycle affects engagement of attention with emotional images. Psychoneuroendocrinology 104, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, N. (2007). The clinical pharmacokinetics of escitalopram. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 46, 281–290. doi: 10.2165/00003088-200746040-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roiser, J. P., Elliott, R., and Sahakian, B. J. (2012). Cognitive mechanisms of treatment in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 117–136. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, E. J., Simonotto, E., Spencer, E. P., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2006). The effects of escitalopram on working memory and brain activity in healthy adults during performance of the n-back task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 185, 339–347. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0334-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez, C., Reines, E. H., and Montgomery, S. A. (2014). A comparative review of escitalopram, paroxetine, and sertraline: Are they all alike? Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 29, 185–196. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0000000000000023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skandali, N., Rowe, J. B., Voon, V., Deakin, J. B., Cardinal, R. N., Cormack, F., et al. (2018). Dissociable effects of acute SSRI (escitalopram) on executive, learning and emotional functions in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 2645–2651. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0229-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sladky, R., Spies, M., Hoffmann, A., Kranz, G., Hummer, A., Gryglewski, G., et al. (2015). (S)-citalopram influences amygdala modulation in healthy subjects: A randomized placebo-controlled double-blind fMRI study using dynamic causal modeling. Neuroimage 108, 243–250. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Stahl, C. (2006). Software for generating psychological experiments. Exp. Psychol. 53, 218–232. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.3.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teichert, J., Rowe, J. B., Ersche, K. D., Skandali, N., Sacher, J., Aigner, A., et al. (2020). Determination of atomoxetine or escitalopram in human plasma by HPLC: Applications in neuroscience research studies?. Int. J. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 58, 426–438. doi: 10.5414/cp203705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uher, R., Farmer, A., Henigsberg, N., Rietschel, M., Mors, O., Maier, W., et al. (2009). Adverse reactions to antidepressants. Br. J. Psychiatry 195, 202–210. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.061960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wandell, B. A. (1995). Foundations of vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

Google Scholar

Weissman, A. N. (1979). The dysfunctional attitude scale: A validation study, thesis. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Graduate School of Arts and Science.

Google Scholar

Weissman, A. N., and Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation of the dysfunctional attitude scale: A preliminary investigation. Toronto, CA: Paper Presented at Meeting of the American Educational Research Association.

Google Scholar

Keywords: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, attention-emotion interaction, oral hormonal contraceptives, female mental health, emotional flanker task

Citation: Beinhölzl N, Molloy EN, Zsido RG, Richter T, Piecha FA, Zheleva G, Scharrer U, Regenthal R, Villringer A, Okon-Singer H and Sacher J (2022) The attention-emotion interaction in healthy female participants on oral contraceptives during 1-week escitalopram intake. Front. Neurosci. 16:809269. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.809269

Received: 04 November 2021; Accepted: 08 August 2022;
Published: 09 September 2022.

Edited by:

Ben Nephew, Worcester Polytechnic Institute, United States

Reviewed by:

Nicole J. Gervais, University of Toronto, Canada
Aisha Judith Leila Munk, University of Giessen, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Beinhölzl, Molloy, Zsido, Richter, Piecha, Zheleva, Scharrer, Regenthal, Villringer, Okon-Singer and Sacher. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nathalie Beinhölzl, beinhoelzl@cbs.mpg.de; Julia Sacher, sacher@cbs.mpg.de

ORCID: Nathalie Beinhölzl, orcid.org/0000-0001-9648-2830; Eóin N. Molloy, orcid.org/0000-0002-7804-2214; Rachel G. Zsido, orcid.org/0000-0002-3743-5871; Ralf Regenthal, orcid.org/0000-0002-7292-1859; Hadas Okon-Singer, orcid.org/0000-0002-5925-1686; Julia Sacher, orcid.org/0000-0003-0944-0558

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.