Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 25 July 2022
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in Understanding the Nature and Features of Misophonia View all 24 articles

The neurobiology of misophonia and implications for novel, neuroscience-driven interventions

  • 1Duke Center for Misophonia and Emotion Regulation, Duke Brain Stimulation Research Center, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, School of Medicine, Duke University, Durham, NC, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, United States
  • 3Department of Philosophy, Duke University, Durham, NC, United States

Decreased tolerance in response to specific every-day sounds (misophonia) is a serious, debilitating disorder that is gaining rapid recognition within the mental health community. Emerging research findings suggest that misophonia may have a unique neural signature. Specifically, when examining responses to misophonic trigger sounds, differences emerge at a physiological and neural level from potentially overlapping psychopathologies. While these findings are preliminary and in need of replication, they support the hypothesis that misophonia is a unique disorder. In this theoretical paper, we begin by reviewing the candidate networks that may be at play in this complex disorder (e.g., regulatory, sensory, and auditory). We then summarize current neuroimaging findings in misophonia and present areas of overlap and divergence from other mental health disorders that are hypothesized to co-occur with misophonia (e.g., obsessive compulsive disorder). Future studies needed to further our understanding of the neuroscience of misophonia will also be discussed. Next, we introduce the potential of neurostimulation as a tool to treat neural dysfunction in misophonia. We describe how neurostimulation research has led to novel interventions in psychiatric disorders, targeting regions that may also be relevant to misophonia. The paper is concluded by presenting several options for how neurostimulation interventions for misophonia could be crafted.

Introduction to misophonia

Misophonia is a disorder characterized by distress when faced with specific sounds or with the context surrounding such sounds (Brout et al., 2018; Swedo et al., 2021). Sound or visual stimuli, labeled as “triggers,” lead to negative emotional, physiological, and behavioral responses that are more intense than in the general population. Triggers tend to be repetitive and more often than not are generated by another’s human body. Sounds such as chewing, eating, slurping (Vitoratou et al., 2021), throat clearing, and breathing/sniffing are common triggers for misophonia. [See consensus definition for a more comprehensive characterization of trigger sounds (Swedo et al., 2021)]. Once triggered, children and adults with misophonia experience intense distress and have difficulty disengaging from the stimulus (Brout et al., 2018). While triggers may vary from person to person, the typical response involves increased autonomic arousal (muscle tension, increased heart rate, and skin conductance) and self-reported experience of anger, disgust, and anxiety (Siepsiak and Dragan, 2019). This discomfort may translate into behavioral or verbal aggression in the moment, and extreme avoidance behaviors outside of the moment (Swedo et al., 2021).

Misophonia is hypothesized to develop in early adolescence and does not improve with time (Brout et al., 2018; Swedo et al., 2021). More research is needed to better understand the onset of misophonia, with emerging studies suggesting genetics (Sanchez and Silva, 2018; Kılıç et al., 2021), and maladaptive learning (Schröder et al., 2013; Dozier, 2015; Dozier and Morrison, 2017) as possible mechanisms through which this disorder develops. Several studies document that misophonia leads to impairment in functioning and negatively affects interpersonal relationships (Wu et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2017; Brout et al., 2018; Swedo et al., 2021). The severity of the misophonic reaction depends on the context in which it occurs, the perceived controllability, and the relationship between the individual and the source of the trigger (Swedo et al., 2021). Increased environmental stressors worsen misophonic distress and reduce the ability to downregulate arousal when faced with trigger sounds (Ferrer-Torres and Giménez-Llort, 2021). In addition, misophonic triggers significantly reduce the participant’s cognitive control and ability to engage in goal directed behavior (Daniels et al., 2020).

Misophonia may overlap with other auditory conditions such as tinnitus, hyperacusis (Aazh et al., 2019), autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR; McErlean and Banissy, 2018; Palumbo et al., 2018; Rouw and Erfanian, 2018), or with psychiatric conditions (Quek et al., 2018; Swedo et al., 2021). One study found a 52.4% overlap with obsessive compulsive personality disorder (OCPD; Cavanna and Seri, 2015) while in other samples this overlap was lower (26%; Jager et al., 2020). Other typical comorbidities are mood disorders (10–48%; Erfanian et al., 2019; Claiborn et al., 2020; Jager et al., 2020), attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; 12%), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; 12–15%; Rouw and Erfanian, 2018; Erfanian et al., 2019; Claiborn et al., 2020), and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD; 15–21%; Erfanian et al., 2019; Claiborn et al., 2020). Comorbidities with eating disorders have also been reported (10% in one study; Erfanian et al., 2019). One study found that PTSD alone was related to misophonia severity (Rouw and Erfanian, 2018). These studies should be seen as preliminary because they either had small samples (Erfanian et al., 2019), or they asked participants to report what diagnoses they received or thought they had (Claiborn et al., 2020), which may lack accuracy. Nevertheless, research suggests that misophonia is independent from these comorbidities and shouldn’t be diagnosed if the presenting problems are better explained by one of these more established disorders (Swedo et al., 2021). For instance, up to 50% of those who describe misophonic distress do not have any other mental health disorders (Rouw and Erfanian, 2018) indeed, emerging research is painting the picture of a distinct problem that does not fit neatly within a diagnosable disorder (Brout et al., 2018).

Structure and function of neural regions that may be connected to misophonic distress

One clear way in which misophonia can be distinguished from other disorders is by identifying how exactly this dysfunction translates into aberrant neural function and connectivity. In this section, we introduce regions of interest for misophonia, and describe their broad structure, function, and connectivity patterns. This section is focused on brain regions that have been identified by at least two neuroscientific studies as showing abnormalities in misophonia (see Table 1 and section “The neurobiology of misophonia”). We chose to include review articles coupled with relevant research findings to highlight typical function in these regions in healthy adults. We also highlight, when available, findings of dysfunction in these regions in adults or adolescents who meet criteria for the disorders that most commonly co-occur with misophonia. An exhaustive literature review of all the findings relevant to these regions is beyond the scope of this article. Other regions not detailed below but found in section “The neurobiology of misophonia” may be important areas of intervention and should continue to be investigated.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of brain regions relevant to misophonia, their established function, and the specific alterations in structure and function identified in neuroimaging studies to date.

Insula

The insular cortex (see Figure 1B) can be found in the lateral sulcus, underneath the frontal and temporal lobes (Naidich et al., 2004). It is highly connected with several regions including the frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes, the brainstem, and limbic structures such as the amygdala, thalamus, cingulate gyrus, and basal ganglia (Flynn, 1999). Therefore, the insula is involved in autonomic, self-awareness, and emotional processing functions (Gu et al., 2013). On each side, the insula can be divided into anterior, middle, and posterior sections. Of primary interest to misophonia is the anterior insula, which is a specialized region, involved in autonomic and interoceptive functions (Flynn, 1999). A unique feature of the insula is that it includes a cluster of spindle-shaped von Economo neurons (VENs; Economo, 1926), that are larger than other neurons, and are used for rapid integration of information between the frontal and insular cortices (Gu et al., 2013). The insula is critical for emotional awareness, and is especially involved in the interoceptive “feeling” of the emotion (Gu et al., 2013). The function of the anterior insula has been connected with heart rate and respiration changes, pain, feeling of touch, awareness of temperature, risk, emotional processing, trust, and norm violation (Gu et al., 2013; Droutman et al., 2015). Therefore, this structure is responsible for perceived awareness of one’s physiological state and needs. Insults to the anterior insular cortex (AIC) result in either heightened, or significantly impaired, perceived wellbeing, and emotional awareness. Studies examining the AIC show that it integrates cognitive and motivational information with emotional inputs (Gu et al., 2013). Hyperacussis has also been described in case reports of three individuals with insular damage, denoting that this structure may be critical in the perception and modulation of sound intensity (Boucher et al., 2015).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Position of the supplementary motor area (SMA), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), ventral premotor cortex (PMv), superior temporal cortex (STC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) on an MNI template brain segmented using the AAL atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Masks for the dlPFC and vmPFC were extracted from the Mindboggle segmentation (Klein et al., 2005). (B) Coronal, sagittal, and transversal views of subcortical regions relevant to misophonia extracted from Mindboggle and from the FSL Harvard-Oxford sub-cortical structural segmentations. The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is blue, the amygdala is green, and the insular cortex is colored rainbow.

Degeneration of VENs as well as hypoactivity in the anterior insula have been connected to alexithymia, or deficits in emotional awareness (Bird et al., 2010; Seeley, 2010). Hypoactivity in the bilateral mid-insula has also been connected with depression severity (Avery et al., 2014). Functional hyperactivity of the insula has generally been related to tasks where negative emotional stimuli are elicited in psychiatric disorder (Schienle et al., 2005; Groenewold et al., 2013). One study found hyperactivity in the right AIC when engaging in downregulation of distress in adults who have excessive weight versus matched controls (Steward et al., 2016) as well as in depressed adults (Beauregard et al., 2006). Another study identified hyperactivation in ADHD adolescent boys when compared to typically developing boys during exposure to negative-valenced stimuli (Vetter et al., 2018). In anxiety disorders, it has been shown that anticipating negative stimuli with unpredictable aversiveness is associated with insula hyperactivity (Simmons et al., 2011; Gorka et al., 2014).

Tractography studies observe widespread connections between the insula and the frontal and temporal lobes. Specifically, structural connectivity to superior medial frontal gyri (SMFG), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and the auditory cortex (Ghaziri et al., 2017). Furthermore, the anterior insula connects to the anterior cingulate (ACC) and midcingulate cortices (Ghaziri et al., 2017). Studies examining connectivity dysfunction in PTSD patients found increased right AIC-DMN resting state connectivity 2 days after trauma exposure (Wang et al., 2012), and in right AIC-amygdala functional connectivity while listening to trauma reminders (Cisler et al., 2014). A decrease in the functional connectivity between the right AIC and the OFC/ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) at rest was found to be uniquely associated with lack of insight in OCD (Fan et al., 2017) and with alexithymia in smoking adults (Sutherland et al., 2013).

Orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal cortices

The vmPFC (Figure 1A) is located at the bottom of the frontal lobe and is critical for evaluating risk, and regulating emotions, such as fear, by directly influencing the amygdala (Motzkin et al., 2015). The OFC is located just above the orbits, therefore in a similar anatomical location, and encompassing similar Brodmann (BA) areas as the vmPFC (Phillips and Della Sala, 1998). The OFC functions to assist in decision making and emotional processing, and is connected to other prefrontal cortex (PFC) regions associated with decision making (Kahnt et al., 2012). In healthy adults, the OFC has also been associated with assigning a reward value or emotion to primary reinforcers such as taste, texture, or facial expressions (Rolls and Grabenhorst, 2008). In order to make these evaluations, the OFC has connections to the primary taste, inferior temporal, primary olfactory, and somatosensory cortical areas (Rolls, 1996). The OFC also receives input from the amygdala and is responsible for emotional enhancement in memory processing (Rolls, 1996; Kumfor et al., 2013). In one study, atrophy in the OFC was connected in patients with fronto-temporal dementia to then inability to use negative emotions to retrieve memories (Séguin, 2004). Reduced engagement of the OFC has also been connected with difficulty adjusting behaviors after negative feedback in OCD (Cisler et al., 2014) and ADHD patients (Itami and Uno, 2002).

Hyperconnectivity between the OFC and other brain regions can be seen in OCD and obesity (Black et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2022). For example, OFC-ACC/and OFC-caudate hyperconnectivity has been documented in OCD (Liu et al., 2022), and lateral OFC-left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) hyperconnectivity in obese children when compared to controls (Black et al., 2014). Hyperconnectivity between the vmPFC and striatum, frontal and motor cortices are also connected to OCD, with vmPFC-caudate hyperconnectivity being correlated with OCD severity (Apergis-Schoute et al., 2018).

Cingulate cortex

The cingulate cortex lies underneath the frontal cortex, in the medial portion of the cerebral hemisphere and is comprised of Brodmann Areas (BA) 23–26 and 29–31 (Stanislav et al., 2013). It mediates the pathway from the thalamus to the hippocampus (Chauhan et al., 2021) and is implicated in emotional processing and regulation, with specific areas (anterior and posterior cingulate cortices) assumed to have differentiated functions. The ACC (Figure 1B) sits at the front of the cingulate cortex, wrapping around the head of the corpus callosum (Monosov et al., 2020). Various examinations implicate the ACC in several aspects of emotion and cognition, including conflict processing, reinforcement learning, motivation, error detection, action selection (Holroyd and Yeung, 2011), management of aggressive behaviors (van Heukelum et al., 2021), and processing of social pain (Eisenberger, 2015). The ACC has also been broken into different subsections, each with differential functions. The rostral ACC (rACC) is closely connected with the amygdala and is thought to play a role in empathy and emotional processing (Singer et al., 2004; De Brito et al., 2009). For example, loss of white matter tracts connecting the rACC with the amygdala diminishes the ACC’s ability to inhibit amygdala activation, leading to increased fear responses seen in PTSD (O’Doherty et al., 2018). The dorsal ACC (dACC), also labeled the midcingulate cortex (MCC; Stevens et al., 2011), is connected to the dlPFC, supplementary motor area (SMA), supramarginal gyrus, and insula revealing its involvement in social cognitive and motor processes (Overbeek et al., 2021; Tuovinen et al., 2022). The MCC was found to mediate decision making, executive control (Mattavelli et al., 2022), and emotion regulation (Li et al., 2014) with increased activation in the anterior MCC being associated with decreased negative emotion (Stevens et al., 2011). Increased ACC activity during a task has been associated with improved emotion regulation (Tang et al., 2016).

Ventral premotor cortex

The ventral premotor cortex (PMv; Figure 1A) is located within the frontal cortex overlapping with the Brocca region and including mirror neurons involved in mimicking and predicting the intentions of others (Binkofski and Buccino, 2006). The PMv is a receptacle for multi-sensory inputs including tactile, auditory, and visual, and is highly connected with adjacent premotor cortical regions (Boussaoud, 2001; Pardo-Vazquez et al., 2009). Time and sensorimotor integration of inputs have been causally connected to the PMv using an inhibitory neurostimulation paradigm (Ruspantini et al., 2011). In synchrony with adjacent areas, the PMv calculates the optimal motor response given learned consequences of several possible options (Pardo-Vazquez et al., 2008; Lemus et al., 2009). Individuals with PMv impairment have difficulty adjusting motor responses based on feedback (Berthier et al., 2017).

Supplementary motor area

The SMA (Figure 1A) of the brain is located on the medial surface of the cortex anterior to the pre central sulcus and is divided into two sub-regions (Kaas and Stepniewska, 2002). The preSMA connects predominantly with areas that are fundamental to a wide spectrum of functions such as planning complex movements, response selection, conflict resolution, word selection, and decision making (Tremblay and Gracco, 2009). The SMA proper occupies a significant portion of the superior frontal gyrus, taking up roughly one third of its territory, with a primary function of planning complex movements and deliberate motor execution (Tremblay and Gracco, 2009). Lesions within the SMA have resulted in an impaired ability to produce self-initiated or unique motion; however, motions that have a strong conditional response based upon memory are maintained as they may be generated largely by using previously associated sensory cues (Kaas and Stepniewska, 2002).

The rich connections of the SMA to the somatosensory regions allow this region to function as more than simply a point of translation of sensory stimuli into motor activity (Kaas and Stepniewska, 2002). SMA neurons receive sensory inputs and interpret them for the purpose of planning and controlling the timing of a motor response so that movement is deliberate and planned and not simply reflexive in nature. In connection with the aMCC and the left anterior insula, SMA has also been hypothesized to underline the experience of empathy for other people’s fear or disgust emotions (Fan et al., 2011). Examinations of the SMA in psychopathology show deficits in activation and in integration of sensory input in ADHD and other developmental disorders (Piek and Dyck, 2004; Mostofsky et al., 2006). Therefore, unlike other brain regions, hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity in this region is uncommon in psychopathology.

The superior temporal cortex

The superior temporal cortex (STC; Figure 1A) comprises roughly 17% of the total cerebral cortex in humans and includes areas with auditory, olfactory, vestibular, visual, and linguistic functions (Kiernan, 2012). The predominant role of the STC is to identify the source of a sound and interpret it as human, dog, car, etc. (Kiernan, 2012). The STC can be divided into two regions with some functional overlap: the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and the superior temporal sulcus (STS). Likely most relevant to misophonia, the STG is fundamental to human understanding of language and sound. It provides the ability to analyze facial expressions and changes in terms of the inherent emotional messaging (Bigler et al., 2007). Additionally, the STG has a functional role in auditory processing within the context of ambient noise. Specifically, this region becomes more active in social situations when a person strains to hold conversation amid a background information (Vander Ghinst et al., 2016). The tempo-parietal junction (TPJ), which captures part of the STG, has also been connected with the ability to engage successfully in emotional distancing if the context requires it (Powers et al., 2020a,b; Powers and LaBar, 2019). Hyperactivity of the STG during exposure to trauma has been one of the brain abnormalities distinguishing adults diagnosed with PTSD from controls (Lanius et al., 2002).

Amygdala

The amygdala (Figure 1B) is a key structure in the brain for processing emotions. It lies in the medial temporal lobe in front of the hippocampus and is a part of the limbic system. Extended research has found strong connectivity between the amygdala and the cingulate cortex, OFC, insular cortex, dlPFC, and parahippocampal gyrus (Stein et al., 2007). The amygdala also interacts with the vmPFC when we engage in affective decision-making. The hypothesized role of the amygdala is to assess the affective components of the situation and the of the vmPFC to lead to a judgment based on amygdala inputs (Shenhav and Greene, 2014). Differences between the roles of the right and left amygdala have been identified and extensively researched. In brief, the right amygdala is naturally larger than its left counterpart in healthy adults, resulting in asymmetrical lateralization (Murphy et al., 1987). It uniquely connects to the dorsomedial (dm)PFC, a neural pathway that has been connected to the regulation of negative emotions (Baeken et al., 2014). On a behavioral level, the right amygdala is more engaged in processing negative emotional stimuli as opposed to the left amygdala which is involved primarily in processing positive emotional stimuli (Yoshimura et al., 2009). More prominent involvement in habituation (Wright et al., 2001) and fear conditioning (Baker and Kim, 2004) led to the hypothesis that the right amygdala is part of a system that detects dynamic emotional stimuli, while the left amygdala may be more involved in evaluating a continuous stimulus. Higher volume in the left amygdala has also been connected with enhanced empathy (Goerlich-Dobre et al., 2015). The pervasive amygdala dysfunction found in psychopathology tends to be associated with hyperactivation or atypical connectivity of the right amygdala (Abercrombie et al., 1998; Chen et al., 2004; Gilboa et al., 2004; Kleinhans et al., 2010; Spoletini et al., 2011; Pico-Perez et al., 2017; Thorsen et al., 2018; Picó-Pérez et al., 2019) or insufficient activation in the left amygdala (Thorsen et al., 2018).

The neurobiology of misophonia

Initial conceptualizations of misophonia hypothesized neural hyper-connectivity between auditory and limbic systems (Jastreboff and Hazell, 2008) based on prior examinations of heightened noise sensitivity which found volumetric enhancements in left auditory areas, bilateral hippocampus, and right anterior insula (Kliuchko et al., 2018)1. Therefore, initial examinations of the neurobiology of misophonia have focused on the auditory and emotional neural networks. In a preliminary investigation, 10 misophonic adults, and 7 matched healthy controls were exposed to 25 s audio-video clips of neutral, aversive, or misophonic trigger sounds (San Giorgi, 2015). Participants with other mental health disorders were excluded. Increased activity in the left amygdala was found for those diagnosed with misophonia versus controls when being exposed to trigger versus aversive videos (San Giorgi, 2015). This preliminary finding suggested that emotional neural networks, rather than auditory over-responsivity, might be key in understanding misophonia. Specifically, given the hypothesis that the left amygdala may be more involved in evaluating a continuous stimulus as negative, trigger sounds may be overly identified as negative experiences in misophonic contexts. Furthermore, given that the left amygdala tends to under perform in the majority of psychiatric disorders, hyperactivation in misophonia points to a unique neural signature for this disorder.

Kumar et al. (2017) examined 20 adults diagnosed with misophonia and 22 controls (non-misophonic) using a functional neuroimaging paradigm. Participants had to report being bothered by sounds of eating, breathing and chewing regardless of whom the person engaging in these behaviors was. Control participants were briefly exposed to misophonic triggers and only enrolled if they did not describe a misophonic response to these sounds. No additional clinical characterization of the two groups was performed. Exposing participants to 15 s audio clips of misophonic sounds led to significantly higher activation in the bilateral AIC in adults diagnosed with misophonia versus controls. The researchers concluded that trigger sounds may have been perceived with heightened salience in those who experience misophonia. This enhanced activation mediated increases in heart rate (HR) and galvanic skin response in adults with misophonia. Furthermore, during trigger sounds alone, the AIC (especially in the left hemisphere) had enhanced functional connectivity with core parts of the default mode network (DMN; the vmPFC, posteromedial cortex), with the hippocampus, and with the amygdala. This hyperconnectivity suggests that individuals with misophonia have difficulty disconnecting default mode memories of similar contexts and thoughts from the active situation, increasing the salience of the experience (Kumar et al., 2017). The enhanced connectivity to the amygdala is also seen in PTSD during trauma exposure (Cisler et al., 2014), suggesting that perception of trigger sounds may be akin to trauma exposure.

Using a similar paradigm but changing the stimuli to include both video and audio, Schröder et al. (2019) compared 21 adults with misophonia with 23 controls. Unlike Kumar et al. (2017), this study excluded participants with psychiatric comorbidities (e.g., anxiety, mood, or substance use disorders) and did not find differences between groups when comparing misophonic (lip smacking, loud breathing) with aversive triggers. Nevertheless, when comparing misophonic with neutral sounds, the study found significant higher activation of the right insula, ACC, and STC in adults with misophonia versus controls. Increased heart rate and self-reported anger, disgust and sadness in response to misophonic triggers also differentiated adults diagnosed with misophonia from controls. Furthermore, repeated exposure to the same triggers amplified the salience network activity which the authors hypothesized indicated a conditioned response, augmented by enhanced vigilance (Schröder et al., 2019).

A novel finding in this study is the increased ACC function during symptom provocation which may suggest that adults diagnosed with misophonia may engage in enhanced emotion regulation during perception of misophonic trigger sounds (Schröder et al., 2019; Cerliani and Rouw, 2020). Based on the function of the ACC presented in the last section, is also possible that increased ACC activation during misophonic triggers can be a marker of higher perceived social pain (or rejection) when sounds produced by another person are experienced. In addition, hyperactivation of the STC during trigger sounds in misophonia may suggest that misophonic sounds are involuntarily brought into the forefront of the context, enhancing their salience.

Cerliani and Rouw (2020) compared 19 participants with misophonia and 20 controls. Twelve-second audio-visual stimuli that were either neutral, aversive, or misophonic triggers were presented during an fMRI paradigm. Each stimulus was preceded by a 2 s text description. Significantly higher brain activity was found during perception of triggers versus aversive sounds in misophonic adults versus controls in the SMA, MCC, visual cortex (V1/V2), and right ventrolateral premotor cortex (PMvl), including the right anterior insula. In addition to enhanced activation, researchers also found hyperconnectivity between MCC/SMA/PMvl with the primary auditory cortex (A1), and the lateral OFC, a signature that appears to be unique to misophonia. Interestingly, these differences were not found when comparing trigger sounds with neutral sounds.

The areas of enhanced activation are responsible for planning motor behavior and may represent the misophonic adult’s preparation to avoid or physically react to the sound. The authors hypothesize that the misophonic reaction is not a direct auditory-limbic response, rather a more complex process mediated by higher order processes. The important role of the lateral OFC connects misophonia to difficulties with behavioral inhibition and with learning how to adjust responses to misophonic triggers based on context. Specifically, adults with misophonia may have difficulty reassessing the unnecessary negative response associated with this particular type of innocuous auditory stimuli (Cerliani and Rouw, 2020). This finding supports the high co-occurrence between misophonia and compulsive disorders. Concerning the SMA, the heightened connectivity with the auditory cortex can suggest higher salience of the auditory input when determining upcoming motor action. The higher SMA activation during trigger versus aversive sounds may suggest a stronger impulse to escape, or to overly identify with the person engaged in the actions that yield misophonic sounds.

Concerning the AIC, two studies showed hyperactivity during exposure to trigger sounds in the absence of regulation instructions (Kumar et al., 2017; Schröder et al., 2019). However, in one study when the unpredictability of the trigger may have been reduced by announcing the type of sound with a written description, the hyperactivity seen in the insula was reduced (Cerliani and Rouw, 2020). This may suggest that reducing ambiguity may lessen the insula reactivity, although a direct comparison is needed to support such a conclusion. Therefore, it is possible that the unpredictability of how a misophonic situation might unfold (i.e., how much worse the trigger sound will get), negative emotions associated with the sounds, and automatic engagement in maladaptive emotion regulation may be responsible for the insula hyperactivity seen in misophonia. It is also possible that hyperactivity in the insula in misophonia may be connected to deficits in attention (Eijsker et al., 2019).

Eijsker et al. (2019) administered a stop signal task to 25 adults with misophonia and 25 matched controls. Participants were excluded if they met criteria for bipolar I, psychosis, substance use disorders, autism, as well as other neurological conditions. The majority of the sample had no comorbidities except one case with ADHD, and one case with borderline personality disorder. There were no differences in successful engagement in the task between the two groups. Nevertheless, misophonic adults evidenced reduced inhibition success-related activation of left dlPFC and heightened activation in this region during correct going trials when compared to controls. Furthermore, misophonic adults only activated the SMFG less during inhibition success compared to failure, suggesting that feedback that may normally be passed to the insula adjusts its activity may be missing or insufficient. Controls only showed inhibition success-related activity in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC).

The authors interpret these findings as suggesting strategic delay of responses in misophonic adults who may prefer accuracy over speed (e.g., perfectionism) during this task coupled with overly negative self-reflection in response to failure and an inability to adjust strategy employed in response to success and failure information. Others have also suggested that perfectionism may be at play within misophonic patients (Jager et al., 2020; Natalini et al., 2020), using self-reports, personality assessments, and clinical examples, although the research data is mixed (Szykowny, 2020). One recent experimental study highlights that disproportionate attention to detail coupled with cognitive inflexibility (which can also be construed as perfectionism) is more prevalent in misophonic versus non-misophonic adults (Simner et al., 2022).

Resting state connectivity studies also offer insight into brain differences that are related to misophonia. In a separate assessment of the sample presented above, Eijsker et al. (2021b) examined differences at rest between misophonic adults and matched controls. A multivariate connectivity analysis with the bilateral amygdalae as seed regions showed hyperconnectivity during resting state with the cerebellum for patients when compared to controls. Patients also showed stronger connectivity within the right frontal cortex (IFC) and TPJ at rest. The authors hypothesize that these abnormalities reflect a tendency to enhance sensory processing of emotional information and may lead to reflex-like behaviors. Enhanced connectivity between the TPJ and the frontal cortex at rest could also be connected with difficulty recruiting this area for successful distancing from distressing noise in misophonic contexts (Powers and LaBar, 2019).

Taken together, these studies highlight the neural networks involved in misophonia and do not support the hypothesis that misophonia is simply a noise sensitivity problem. Rather, these studies suggest that brain networks involved in emotion, salience, attention to detail, and cognitive flexibility may display aberrant function and connectivity in adults with misophonia (See Supplementary Table 1 for specific locations for neural differences found in the reviewed studies). Evidence supports amplified physiological reactivity to misophonic cues that seems to be connected to emotion much more than to heightened responsivity of the auditory cortex. Similar to other clinical disorders, prefrontal areas may be hypoactive, or connected to these limbic structures in a dysfunctional way (Kumar et al., 2017) leading to difficulties downregulating this exaggerated arousal response. The specific ways in which the insula connectivity is altered and the presence of hyperactivity in motor areas during symptom provocation highlight that a unique neural signature of misophonia exists. Small sample sizes and differences in inclusion/exclusion criteria suggest the need for replication for these findings in more tightly controlled samples, including comparisons with clinical not just with healthy controls. The wide range of functional differences found from controls in different studies also points to the necessity of a large trial that can clarify with more accuracy which of these candidate regions shows the most robust evidence for dysfunction in misophonia. Interestingly, all studies point toward hyperfunction in motor areas as being connected to misophonic responses.

Along these lines, Kumar et al. (2021) proposed a new mechanism for misophonia that involves the activity of mirror neurons found in premotor areas. Using a resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) paradigm, the team showed that function in the right secondary auditory and the PMv cortices are significantly more correlated at rest in 17 adults with misophonia than in 20 controls. A similarly increased functional connectivity at rest was found between the right secondary visual cortex (V2) and the PMv. The right anterior insula also demonstrated heightened functional connectivity at rest with the right V2 and the left primary visual cortex (V1) in adults diagnosed with misophonia versus controls. When exposed to 15 s audio clips, 19 participants with misophonia showed again a stronger connectivity between the auditory cortex and the PMv regardless of the sounds being played. The PMv demonstrated higher activity in misophonic adults when compared to controls for trigger sounds only, and the magnitude of activation in this area correlated with the self-reported distress induced by the sound. The authors identify that the area that demonstrates hyperactivity within the PMv is responsible for engaging in or observing mouth and lip movements. They conclude that this pattern indicates hyperactivity of mirror neurons in areas responsible for orofacial actions.

This neural mirroring may involve behavioral mirroring, likely done with enhanced awareness, and inability to disconnect or distract from this activity (Kumar et al., 2021). Given that the PMv is also activated when observing lip movements of others (Buccino et al., 2001), it is possible that misophonia includes an inability to disengage from sensory cues related to others orofacial movements. Given the function of the PMv, this pattern of results may also suggest over-preparedness for the reactions of others and higher importance given to the motor movements involved in trigger sounds versus other sensory inputs. This hypothesized mechanism doesn’t explain why some trigger sounds, like clicking, elicit misophonic reactions (Hansen et al., 2021), but it offers a comprehensive explanation for neuroimaging findings presented in the literature thus far. A possibility exists that different subtypes of misophonia may exist, or that different mechanisms through which the misophonic reaction is triggered may be at play.

Structural abnormalities

The first structural abnormality related to misophonia was reported by Kumar et al. (2017) who found higher myelination in the vmPFC. Increased myelination of the vmPFC can also be seen in monkeys who are exposed to early mild life stressors and learn to cope with such stressors (Katz et al., 2009), suggesting that this brain difference may be a marker of resilience. More recent studies failed to replicate this finding when using voxel-based morphometry along with structural MRI; nevertheless, new findings emerged (Eijsker et al., 2021a,b). When compared to controls (n = 25) patients with misophonia (n = 24) may have larger gray matter volume in the right amygdala (Eijsker et al., 2021b), a finding believed to be connected to the increased emotional reactivity when being exposed to trigger sounds. The same team also showed that adults with misophonia may have greater white matter volumes in the left frontal cortex (the left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, and the left body of the corpus callosum; Eijsker et al., 2021a). This finding again points toward a unique neural characteristic in misophonia, given that the majority of mental health dysfunctions have been connected to lower, not higher, white matter volumes (Thomason and Thompson, 2011). Patients may also have lower diffusivities, which reflects higher myelination. The affected regions involve tracts connecting the amygdala with the occipital cortex, and the OFC with the frontal pole and the dlPFC (Eijsker et al., 2021a). The authors interpret these findings as possibly underlying processes responsible for disengaging attention away from the aversive stimulus (Eijsker et al., 2021a). The regions affected are also involved in emotional empathy and recognizing facial emotions (Philippi et al., 2009; Oishi et al., 2015), processes that have not been studied in misophonia but which may play a role.

Future directions for the neuroscience of misophonia

The rapid advancements in understanding the neuroscience behind misophonia are promising. One drawback of the current literature is the little overlap in findings, despite similar paradigms. Therefore, one important future direction may be to examine differences in much larger samples, using more stringent controls for comorbidities. In addition, employing tasks that elicit activation in the insula or the PMv in misophonic patients, in the absence of misophonic triggers, may shed light onto the specificity or generalizability of the observed dysfunction. For example, examining differences in empathy or emotional awareness in adults with and without misophonia (Philippi et al., 2009; Oishi et al., 2015), may provide additional insight into insula hyperactivation.

Future research should also examine neural differences between misophonia and clinical controls during emotion regulation. The rich body of literature of neural underpinnings and plasticity of emotion regulation (Gross, 2013; Powers and LaBar, 2019) may provide important additional avenues for misophonia interventions. An additional important future direction is to examine the developmental trajectory of neural changes in misophonia. Imaging studies in misophonic children do not yet exist, and longitudinal examinations that show the trajectory of dysfunction over time are also lacking.

Neurostimulation can be a helpful tool in understanding causality and development. Therefore, paradigms that attempt to temporarily enhance insula activity and measure sensitivity to triggers in non-misophonic adults may answer questions about misophonic mechanisms. Temporarily altering the function of other brain regions related to misophonia could also provide future insight into the causality of these dysfunctions in relation to misophonia distress.

Last, but not least, misophonic triggers are context specific. In other words, the sound must elicit specific visual imagery, and must come from a specific set of people in order to trigger a misophonic reaction. Neuroimaging examinations that separate these different components of the trigger experience are also needed.

The neuroscience of overlapping disorders

Knowledge of neurological dysfunction that can be seen in comorbid conditions may broaden our understanding of the neurobiology of misophonia (see Table 2). We chose to focus this section of the review on psychiatric comorbidities, while acknowledging that there is much to learn from misophonia’s overlap with tinnitus, ASMR, or hyperacusis [see McGeoch and Rouw (2020) for an example]. Our review is restricted to psychiatric comorbidities because the solutions proposed for neuroscience-based interventions were primarily developed for psychiatric conditions. We focus on OCPD/OCD, mood disorders, ADHD, and PTSD because several papers have supported their co-occurrence with misophonia, and the rates of overlap appear to be over 10% (Cavanna and Seri, 2015; Quek et al., 2018; Rouw and Erfanian, 2018; Erfanian et al., 2019; Claiborn et al., 2020; Jager et al., 2020; Swedo et al., 2021). Other comorbidities may also occur and have relevance to the neurobiology of misophonia. Nevertheless, existing data either point to low co-occurrence [e.g., autism was reported by 3% of participants with misophonia in a large study (Claiborn et al., 2020)], or there needs to be replication to ascertain the relevance of a comorbidity to misophonia [e.g., for eating disorders (Erfanian et al., 2019)].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of alterations in brain regions that are relevant to misophonia in disorders who have been shown to have comorbidity with misophonia.

Obsessive compulsive personality disorders/obsessive compulsive disorder

Much of the neural mechanisms and dysfunctions in OCPD are difficult to distinguish from OCD. OCD is characterized as a chronic compulsive disorder, while OCPD is a behavioral disorder defined by immoderate perfectionism (Gordon et al., 2013). Emerging examinations of OCPD neural dysfunction highlight altered activity during resting state in the bilateral caudate, left insula, and left medial SFG areas when compared to healthy controls (Lei et al., 2020). These findings highlight higher engagement in self-perception and future planning at rest, which may play into the perfectionistic tendencies seen in this disorder. OCPD patients also have decreased gray matter volume in the prefrontal, cingulate, and insular cortices (Reetz et al., 2008). The decreased gray matter volume of these areas most obviously affects decision-making and correlates with anxious tendencies such as avoidance behavior (Reetz et al., 2008). Furthermore, OCPD patients have decreased amygdala, hippocampus (Gurok et al., 2019), and OFC volumes, and higher volume in the thalamus (Atmaca et al., 2019), suggesting difficulties with memory, emotional processing, and conscious attention. An fMRI study showed greater functional connectivity within the precuneus, a region that controls memory retrieval and manipulation, in OCPD adults when compared to healthy controls (Coutinho et al., 2016). Thus, the neural dysfunction seen in OCPD does not overlap with any of the current findings of neural dysfunction in misophonia, although direct comparisons are warranted. Interestingly, OCPD is characterized by reduced amygdala volumes, with in misophonia amygdala volumes may be increased.

Similar to OCPD, meta-analytic findings in OCD highlight increased activation of the right caudate, putamen, and insula as well as decreased activation of the left OFC, caudate, and MCC when compared to controls (Yu et al., 2022). During resting state, it has also been found in patients with OCD that connectivity within the dmPFC-thalamus-caudate loop is decreased, while connectivity between the caudate and the superior and middle temporal gyrus, middle and inferior occipital gyrus, and SMA is increased (Chen et al., 2016). These neural findings have been associated with disruptions in processing during distress, dysfunctional memory formation, and impairments in cognitive and behavioral regulation. Increased activity in the insula (Schienle et al., 2005) and connectivity with the dmPFC was found as well (Beucke et al., 2014), highlighting the insula’s critical role in emotional processing and feelings of disgust often seen in OCD. Symptom provocation in OCD also shows hyperactivity in the left amygdala (Simon et al., 2014). Studies examining how patients with an OCD diagnosis learn from errors and inhibit behaviors show a hyperactive error processing mechanism and an impaired ability to engage in inhibitory control (Norman et al., 2019). These alterations have been connected with hyperactivity in the dACC, SMA, pre-SMA, right anterior insula, and anterior lateral PFC during error processing, and hypoactivity in the rACC, OFC, and right anterior insula during inhibitory control (Norman et al., 2019). Structural examinations find that patients with OCD have smaller gray matter volume in the ACC compared to controls, suggesting deficits with motor control and visuospatial function (Peng et al., 2012).

Unlike with OCPD, there is some overlap in neural dysfunction between OCD and misophonia. Specifically, hyperactivity in the insula and amygdala is seen during symptom provocation in both disorders as well as hyperconnectivity between the insula and frontal regions, although the specific aberrant connectivity is different between OCD (dmPFC-insula) and misophonia (vmPFC-insula). Hyperactivity in the ACC is found in both disorders, during trigger sound exposure in misophonia and while engaging in an error processing task in OCD. It would be interesting to examine whether exposure to disgust and fear, as well as error processing ’lead to hyperactivity in the insula in misophonia, like they do in OCD. In other networks, OCD and misophonia neuroimaging results diverge. For example, the function of the OFC shows impairment in OCD, while in misophonia the connectivity of the OFC seems to be primarily affected. During symptom provocation, the MCC is hyperactive in misophonia and hypoactive in OCD. Inhibitory control shows dysfunction in the ACC in OCD and the PCC in misophonia. The function of SMA is differentially affected in both disorders: in misophonia trigger sounds lead to SMA hyperactivity, while in OCD error processing tasks lead to SMA hypoactivity. The STC shows altered connectivity at rest in both disorders, but with very different brain regions. Therefore, misophonia is unlikely to be a variant of OCD given that the neuroscientific results point primarily to differences and not to overlapping patterns.

Mood disorders

There has been considerable effort to characterize neural dysfunction in mood disorders in the recent years. Summaries of this literature point to decreased activation in the dlPFC during exposure to negative stimuli and sustained activation in the amygdala in adults diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD) when compared to controls (Groenewold et al., 2013). Hyperactivation in the OFC when presented with positive stimuli and hyperactivity in the ACC (Groenewold et al., 2013) [particularly the subgenual ACC (sgACC)] (Gray et al., 2020) and insula when being exposed to negative stimuli (Groenewold et al., 2013) or when engaging in emotion regulation (Beauregard et al., 2006), are also markers of depression. Severity of depression and cognitive dysfunction has been associated with alterations in functional connectivity between the nucleus accumbens, the OFC, ACC, SMA, and caudate (Gong et al., 2017). Decreased thickness in the left premotor cortex is characteristic of depression, and indicative of a positive response to antidepressants 8 weeks later (Liu et al., 2021).

The markers of psychopathology in bipolar disorder are somewhat different. During emotional processing and regulation, those who meet criteria for bipolar disorder show hyperactivity in the amygdala, hypoactivity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and OFC, and decreased connectivity between these regions when compared to healthy controls (Phillips and Swartz, 2014). Reduced insula and amygdala volumes may be a precursor for bipolar disorder (Bechdolf et al., 2012). Furthermore, during cognitive interference trials when compared to controls, bipolar adults evidence deactivation in the anterior insula and the ACC with altered connectivity with the DMN (Ellard et al., 2019). The hypoactivity in the dlPFC remains a consistent marker of both unipolar and bipolar mood disorders (Townsend and Altshuler, 2012). Structural studies find reduced gray matter volume in the right SMA in both MDD and bipolar adults when compared to clinical and non-clinical controls (Chang M. et al., 2018).

Just like with OCD, there is overlap between misophonia and mood disorders in the neural dysfunction seen during symptom provocation, which in mood disorders takes the form of exposure to negative emotional stimuli. In both groups, symptom provocation leads to hyperactivity in the amygdala and insula, as well as in the ACC. Interestingly, exposure to positive stimuli leads to hypoactivity in these regions, a phenomenon that would be interesting to test in misophonia also. The OFC and SMA function and connectivity are altered in both mood disorders and misophonia, but in very different ways, pointing toward divergence between these disorders. Emotion regulation tasks within mood disorders also lead to a pattern of aberrant function and connectivity, but have not yet been studied in misophonia. Thus, examinations of emotion regulation and positive emotional processing are warranted, although they are unlikely to alter the current conclusion that misophonia has a different neural signature than mood disorders.

Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder

Meta-analyses of neuroimaging findings aimed to capture dysfunction in ADHD found decreased activity in the bilateral SFG and left dlPFC during working memory tasks as well as in the bilateral inferior frontal gyri, right SFG, and right dlPFC during response inhibition tasks (McCarthy et al., 2014). In addition, in tasks testing selective motor response inhibition, less activation in the left dlPFC and right caudate was found (McCarthy et al., 2014). The decreased activity in frontal regions correlate to behavioral symptoms of decreased working memory capacity, inhibitory control, self-regulation, and impulsivity control (McCarthy et al., 2014). Dysfunction in reward processing has also been connected to ADHD, specifically to hypoactivity in the OFC when compared to controls (Van Den Heuvel et al., 2005). Underperformance in the SMA and the basal ganglia during neurocognitive tasks, and poor deactivation in the DMN when switching to response inhibition have also been found to differentiate ADHD patients from controls (Albajara Sáenz et al., 2019).

Similar to other misophonia comorbidities, in ADHD when compared to controls, exposure to negative stimuli leads to higher activation in the insula (Vetter et al., 2018). In a rigorous analysis of over 140 participants, investigators concluded that the right anterior insula is likely the hub for altered emotion regulation function in ADHD young adults (Viering et al., 2021). Another study examined learning of fear cues in ADHD and found diminished activation in the dACC when unlearning an instructed fear cue as well as increased activation in the amygdala when being exposed to a neutral cue in the absence of fear instructions (Maier et al., 2014). The authors interpreted these findings as impairments in processing of verbally aversive information in ADHD. Unlike other comorbid disorders, dysfunction in motor areas has been associated with ADHD impairments. In one study, boys diagnosed with ADHD had reduced premotor cortex areas when compared to typically developing children (Dirlikov et al., 2015). Dysfunction in the premotor cortex was also evidenced by an fMRI study showing insufficient recruitment of this area when trying to suppress distractions (Vaidya et al., 2005). When attempting response inhibition children with an ADHD diagnosis recruited the right STC unlike comparison children, who recruited a front-striatal network for this task (Vaidya et al., 2005).

Alterations in the dlPFC function during response inhibition tasks as well as hyperactivity in the insula when presented with upsetting stimuli appear to be commonalities between misophonia and ADHD. The STC is hyperactive in misophonia during trigger sounds, and appears to be recruited as a compensatory mechanism to handle response inhibition in ADHD. These findings may point to overlapping mechanisms of these disorders, although a direct comparison is needed to examine this hypothesis. On the other hand, alterations in other brain regions appear to be very different between misophonia and ADHD. For example, difficulties ignoring distractors are connected to PMv hypoactivity in ADHD, while trigger sounds lead to PMv hyperactivity in misophonia. OFC function appears to be altered in ADHD at least in one domain, while in misophonia evidence primarily points toward dysfunctional connectivity. Thus, misophonia and ADHD are likely to be very distinct disorders.

Post-traumatic stress disorder

Post-traumatic stress disorder is a disorder that develops in a subset of children and adults who experience a traumatic event (Kessler et al., 2005). A review study conducted in 2016 summarized a decade’s worth of literature regarding the neurobiological basis for PTSD and concluded that the amygdala, the mPFC, and the hippocampus played important roles in the development and maintenance of this disorder (Shin et al., 2006). Specifically, this review concludes that patients with PTSD evidence a heightened amygdala responsivity during trauma exposure, which is positively associated with symptom severity. The mPFC is typically volumetrically smaller amongst patients with PTSD and is hypo-responsive during symptomatic states, with an inverse correlation to symptom severity (Shin et al., 2006). The insula may also have a critical role in PTSD. Resting state scans were used in one study to classify with 80% accuracy participants with PTSD versus non-clinical controls based on insula connectivity patterns (Harricharan et al., 2020). Structural studies also show that in patients with PTSD compared to controls, the premotor cortex (Rocha-Rego et al., 2012) insula (Kunimatsu et al., 2020), and hippocampus (Shin et al., 2006) have diminished volumes, with the hippocampus showing aberrant functional, and neuronal integrity (Shin et al., 2006). Newer findings also suggest diminished gray matter volume in the right dlPFC, OFC, and SMA in 30 females with PTSD based on childhood trauma versus controls (Thomaes et al., 2010).

When comparing the neural signature of misophonia and PTSD, similarities emerge in insula dysfunction. In resting state scans, participants with both misophonia and PTSD display PFC-insula hyperconnectivity. Furthermore, symptom provocation evidences hyperconnectivity between the insula and the amygdala as well as hyperactivity in the insula in both PTSD and misophonia. Nevertheless, PTSD seems to be characterized by reduced volumes in many of the structures of interest, a neural abnormality that has not been related to misophonia yet. Future studies should examine in more detail volumetric reductions in key brain regions in misophonia to further elucidate differences or similarities to PTSD. Furthermore, insula hyperactivity is not characteristic of PTSD dysfunction, but is characteristic of misophonia. Similarly, OFC dysfunction is characteristic of PTSD but not of misophonia. Thus, there appear to be several differences in PTSD and misophonia neural signatures to highlight the uniqueness of each disorder.

Summary

Taken together (see Table 2), these findings highlight that, while there is overlap in neural dysfunction in misophonia and comorbid disorders, no other disorder can fully explain the alterations seen in misophonia. Of all comorbid disorders discussed here, PTSD and OCD neural dysfunction come closest to misophonia, although the hallmark of these disorders are the amygdala and the OFC, not the insula. When tasks relevant to the disorder examined are employed (e.g., symptom provocation, exposure to negative stimuli, emotion regulation in mood disorders, planning in OCD, working memory in ADHD), across comorbid disorders and misophonia, hyperactivity in the insula, amygdala, ACC, and hypoactivity in the dlPFC can be found. Therefore, the hyperactivity in subcortical regions seen in misophonia may be related to aberrant processing of, or hypersensitivity to, negative emotions. Furthermore, in misophonia and beyond, the dlPFC may be a general marker of problematic allocation of resources to respond to challenging contexts.

Findings related to the OFC and the motor cortex appear to be unique to misophonia. The dysfunction seen in the OFC appears to vary depending on the disorder under investigation with a unifying theme across comorbid disorders of functional hypoactivity and hypoconnectivity when disorder-relevant tasks are employed. The pattern of hyperconnectivity with other frontal and premotor regions appears to be uniquely related to misophonia. Across comorbid disorders, there was little examination of the role of the premotor cortex in psychiatric presentations. While hyperactivity in both SMA and PMv was found in misophonia during symptom provocation, other disorders were characterized by either unremarkable performance in these regions or by hypoactivity. Taken together, these findings strongly suggest a unique neural signature for misophonia, supporting an independent problem in need of novel personalized solutions.

Neuroscience-informed interventions: Brain stimulation

In order to most rapidly identify an intervention for misophonia, a disorder for which there is currently no consensus for an optimal treatment [see Aazh et al. (2019) for a review of evidence for cognitive-behavioral interventions], neuroscientific dysfunctions that are unique to this problem should be directly targeted and altered. Thus, translating findings from basic neuroscience studies into innovative therapies can be the quickest way to finding a cure for misophonia. Non-invasive neurostimulation (i.e., the purposeful modulation of neural circuitry) is a powerful tool that resulted in novel interventions for several treatment resistant conditions, such as treatment refractory MDD (Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015), OCD (Trevizol et al., 2016), smoking (Maiti et al., 2017), and PTSD (Kan et al., 2020).

Initially, neurostimulation targeted cortical regions at the surface of the brain which were reachable by the generated e-Field (which generally has a 2-cm depth of penetration below the scalp; Deng et al., 2013). Nevertheless, research findings suggest that the effect of neurostimulation can be seen throughout entire networks. A recent systematic review of over 33 rTMS studies found that active rTMS induces significant changes in resting state functional connectivity in a variety of targeted networks (Beynel et al., 2020). Furthermore, functional and structural networks can then be used to alter connectivity and activity in subcortical structures, such as the insula (Addicott et al., 2019) amygdala (Baeken et al., 2010), or ACC (Vink et al., 2018). In this way, neurostimulation can be used to remediate dysfunctional brain circuits regardless of their location.

Overview of neurostimulation approaches

Because the brain is an electric organ, communications within neural networks may be altered with the use of magnetic and electric fields that induce brief activity in targeted brain cells. Magnetic stimulation relies on an electromagnetic coil, while electric stimulation passes direct current through the cortex in order to achieve neuromodulatory effects (Larrivee, 2020). In this section, we introduce several types of neurostimulation applications, focusing on those that have been successfully used in interventions for psychiatric disorders (see Table 3 for a summary).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview, advantages, and risks of various neurostimulation techniques.

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) employs rapidly alternating magnetic pulses that induce an electric current in the underlying cortex. It can be applied in single or repetitive pulses to either activate, enhance, or inhibit activation in a superficial neural target. The induced current depolarizes cortical neurons and alters the excitability of neural tissue (Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015). High-frequency (HF; up to 20 Hz) rTMS has been associated with more excitability resulting in enhanced activity, and low frequency (≤1 Hz) with less excitability, and inhibited activity (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994; Chen et al., 1997; Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015).

Repetitive TMS (rTMS) is the most traditional therapeutic application of brain stimulation (Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015). It uses a figure-8 coil that is placed over a predetermined location of the head (called a target). The coil generates a magnetic field that induces electricity in the target by delivering magnetic pulses (or trains) on and off, at specific intervals inter-train intervals (ITI). An additional parameter important for rTMS is the motor threshold (MT). This parameter represents the lowest output of the rTMS machine needed to reliably elicit a motor movement when stimulating the motor area (i.e., the lowest output needed to reach the brain; Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015). As an example, a typical rTMS treatment session for depression will contain 75 trains, each train being 4 s long, with an ITI of 26 s, delivered at 120% MT (Holtzheimer and McDonald, 2014). HF-rTMS has been FDA approved as a treatment for depression since 2008 (Holtzheimer and McDonald, 2014). Furthermore, HF-rTMS over the dlPFC and superior medial frontal cortex, has been found to significantly inhibit activity in the right insula (Li et al., 2017b; Chang D. et al., 2018). Therefore, rTMS is a successful approach to changing function in structures relevant to misophonia.

Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation (dTMS) aims to target “deeper” regions of the brain by using an H shaped coil pattern. This coil is inserted in a spherical helmet placed on the patient’s head. Generally, dTMS follows the same parameters as rTMS, with the difference being that dTMS is less precise in hitting its target. The FDA recently approved dTMS in conjunction with symptom provocation as a treatment for OCD in 2018 (Roth et al., 2021) and for smoking cessation in 2020 (Young et al., 2021). DTMS has also been employed in the treatment of PTSD (Isserles et al., 2021) and has evidence of successful use in interventions to target both cortical and subcortical structures (Beynel et al., 2021; Roth et al., 2021).

Intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) is also delivered via a figure-8 coil, using triple pulses at a higher frequency to deliver unique, high-energy frequency of stimulation. The same effect that rTMS achieves with 75 trains, can be achieved with only 20 trains of iTBS, each train lasting 2 s, 8 s ITI. Therefore, iTBS session can be completed in about 3 min versus rTMS sessions which take 35–40 min (Pabst et al., 2022). Given that the majority of currently approved treatment protocols include 20–30 sessions [for depression for example (Sonmez et al., 2019)], iTBS can save significant time for patients. Recently, an iTBS protocol has obtained approval for treatment-resistant depression intervention (Blumberger et al., 2018). TBS has also been used successfully to alter amygdala activity via its connectivity with the STS (Pitcher et al., 2017), highlighting that this approach can successfully alter cortical and subcortical brain structures, including areas indicated in misophonia.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is an alternative type of brain stimulation that uses direct electrical currents to stimulate a specific brain network. A constant small current (1–2.5 mA) passes through two electrodes placed over the head in order to modulate neuronal activity. Anodal stimulation acts to excite the network it targets while cathodal stimulation reduces neuronal activity (Fregni et al., 2015). TDCS has not yet been cleared by the FDA in the treatment of psychiatric disorders because mechanisms through which tDCS operates need to be better understood (Fregni et al., 2015). Nevertheless, it is widely researched for its therapeutic applications, because of the low cost and ease to administer. TDCS interventions are thought to be probably efficacious for non-drug resistant depression (Lefaucheur et al., 2017), currently unsuccessful with tinnitus (positive response for 15% of 602 participants across six studies; Santos et al., 2018), and potentially relevant for remediating aberrant amygdala hyperactivity (Ironside et al., 2019).

Neurostimulation interventions have primarily been developed and successful for adults with psychiatric disorders who did not respond to other treatments. Meta-analyses of therapeutic applications within these samples find effect sizes that are small-to-moderate when compared to placebo (Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015). Findings suggest that these effect sizes could be improved by using connectivity-driven targeting (Fox et al., 2012; Opitz et al., 2016; Fox, 2018), neuroimaging and neuro-navigation (Neacsiu and Lisanby, 2015; Beynel et al., 2019) and employing electric field (E-field) modeling (Bungert et al., 2017; Opitz et al., 2016). In addition, fusing neuromodulation with behavioral practice (Tsagaris et al., 2016) can enhance efficacy. For example, combining HF-rTMS with active emotion regulation practice yields behavioral improvements in emotion regulation up to a week after a single session when compared to emotion regulation practice alone in transdiagnostic clinical adults (Neacsiu et al., 2022b,a). In addition, combinations of rTMS with 16–20 sessions of psychotherapy demonstrate feasibility (Neacsiu et al., 2018), with enhanced effects over psychotherapy alone (Kozel et al., 2018), or over cognitive training alone (Cunningham et al., 2015).

Both researchers and consumers should be aware of the potential risks involved with neurostimulation. The most severe, but rare, risk is for seizure, which in prior studies occurred in about 0.2% of research subjects (Rossi et al., 2021). The most common side effects are headaches or muscle tension which can occur in up to 30% of those receiving neurostimulation, depending on the protocol and target (Rossi et al., 2009). Discomfort at the site of neurostimulation during the procedure can happen in up to 40% of patients (Rossi et al., 2009). Nevertheless, less than 2% of research participants quit because of pain and discomfort, and the majority of participants experienced habituation to this discomfort over time (Rossi et al., 2009). Other, less common, side effects are temporary changes in mood, dizziness, and hearing impairment. ’Another potential risk is that compensatory rather than dysfunctional networks may be impacted by this particular treatment. Several guidelines have been established to guide development and application of neurostimulation interventions in order to maximize safety (Rossi et al., 2009, Rossi et al., 2021).

In addition to risks, it is important to highlight that there continue to be many unknowns with regards to this technology. There is limited data on the long-term effects of neurostimulation interventions (Marangell et al., 2007) and the parameter space (frequency, intensity, coil positioning, and orientation; Beynel et al., 2019). Furthermore, the exact mechanism through which neurostimulation changes psychopathology in the brain is still unknown (Bestmann and Feredoes, 2013). Other types of neurostimulation exist, such as transcranial photobiomodulation, alternating current stimulation, or focused ultrasound. While these technologies can also offer promise to treatments in general, they will not be discussed in this review because of their limited existing evidence for broad therapeutic effects in psychiatric disorders. Therefore, researchers and clinicians interested in neurostimulation approaches should continue to follow the literature for best practices and new approaches to increase safety and efficacy.

Neurostimulation as a treatment for misophonia: design considerations

A neurostimulation treatment for misophonia could be constructed through several avenues. On the one hand, based on resting state misophonia findings, one could develop a neurostimulation intervention that attempts to reduce hyperconnectivity between IFC-TPJ (Eijsker et al., 2021b), PMv-insula, or other such hyperactive circuits (Kumar et al., 2021). In order to reduce hyperconnectivity, inhibitory neurostimulation may be attempted as a first choice of intervention. Existing studies highlight that inhibitory neurostimulation can be successfully applied over the TPJ (Powers et al., 2020a) and the PMv (Tremblay et al., 2012). To enhance the efficacy of neurostimulation, resting state functional imaging data should be collected prior to treatment administration in order to identify the regions within the TPJ and the PMv that are connected to IFC and the insula, respectively. These regions should then be exposed to repetitive inhibitory neurostimulation using either rTMS or continuous TBS (Huang et al., 2005). Accelerated neurostimulation (Baeken, 2019), or several consecutive sessions should be examined to determine the optimal amount of neurostimulation to elicit changes in misophonia.

On the other hand, given the therapeutic synergy between behavioral and neuromodulatory interventions, one could alter context either before or during neuromodulation. One approach would be to expose participants to misophonic triggers before administering neurostimulation, similar to the FDA approved paradigm for OCD using dTMS (Roth et al., 2021). The rationale behind the “symptom provocation” is that it activates the circuitry involved in misophonia leaving these networks more amenable to change. This provocation could be followed with excitatory neurostimulation targeted toward regions that downregulate the amygdala, the insula, or the ACC (such as the dlPFC or MPFC), as well as inhibitory neurostimulation over regions like the SMA, PMv, STG, or vmPFC. An alternative approach would be to expose participants to trigger sounds during neurostimulation. Options for brain stimulation while sounds are being played would be similar as previously described (e.g., excitatory over dlPFC/mPFC, or inhibitory over SMA/PMv/vmPFC/STG).

An additional option, following research in emotion regulation (Neacsiu et al., 2022a), would be to coach the use of an emotion regulation skill [e.g., distancing (Powers and LaBar, 2019)] while misophonic triggers are presented. Excitatory neurostimulation could then be concurrently administered over a node of the emotion regulation network (e.g., the dlPFC). Alternatively, inhibitory neurostimulation could be administered concurrently over a hyperactive area in misophonia (e.g., SMA). Yet another option could be to enhance dlPFC or SMFG activation using HF-rTMS during a stop signal task, to correct differences from healthy subjects found in misophonia (Eijsker et al., 2019).

While regions like the dlPFC or mPFC are mentioned several times as options for stimulation, the specific target within the dlPFC/mPFC might vary depending on the rationale for its selection. For low resource approaches, the structural dlPFC could be identified using the beam method (Beam et al., 2009), a targeting approach that uses scalp measurements to identify the optimal stimulation spot. A more precise approach would involve neuroimaging, either structural, to identify regions with more anatomic specificity, or functional, to identify specific dysfunctional networks that may be candidates for neurostimulation. For example, one might expose participants to trigger versus neutral sounds in the scanner. Using neuroimaging analysis software, the next step would be to compute a contrast in activation between these two different auditory experiences. For surface targets, the region with the highest activation within the vmPFC or within the PMv within this contrast could be extracted. For connectivity targets, the highest activation within the amygdala, insula, or ACC could be extracted, followed by an analyses to help identify a surface region with functional connectivity to one of these subcortical regions of specific activity.

Similar approaches have been successfully used in other disorders. For example, in adolescents with MDD, decreased amygdala volumes were normalized using HF-rTMS to the left dlPFC (Seewoo et al., 2022). Furthermore, HF rTMS to the dlPFC was found to significantly increase ACC activity (Tremblay et al., 2012), and resting state left dlPFC – ACC connectivity (Huang et al., 2005), and decrease right insula activity (Li et al., 2017a). In MDD and PTSD patients, 5 Hz TMS to the dlPFC was also found to reduce the problematic hyperconnectivity between the sgACC, the insula and the DMN (Philip et al., 2018). In borderline personality disorder, HF-rTMS over the right dlPFC yielded a decrease in connections between the amygdala/insula and precuneus, PCC, and parietal lobules (Sverak et al., 2021). Similarly, tDCS over the left dlPFC led to increased long-term cerebral blood flow to the ACC (Jog et al., 2021), and reduced activation of the amygdala (Ironside et al., 2019). Therefore, the dlPFC is a successful target for changing activity and connectivity of subcortical structures such as the insula and the ACC. These changes occur independent of the type of targeting employed, although more precise, connectivity-driven targeting is likely to have a more powerful effect than anatomically driven targeting (Neacsiu et al., 2018).

Different regions of the mPFC and TPJ have also been targeted successfully with documented downstream effects. For the treatment of OCD, after rTMS to the right OFC, PET scans revealed decreased metabolism in the ACC (Nauczyciel et al., 2014), implying deactivation in this region. Furthermore, 5 Hz rTMS over the mPFC led to an increase in functional connectivity between the mPFC and amygdala (Beynel et al., 2021). In MDD adults, HF rTMS over the dmPFC led to reduced dmPFC – insula and sgACC-caudate connectivity (Salomons et al., 2014). TDCS between the left TPJ and left PFC reduced TPJ-insula and TPJ-SMA functional connectivity in schizophrenia patients (Mondino et al., 2016) and in non-clinical adults (Dalong et al., 2021). Taken together, these studies offer insight into the feasibility and initial parameter set up necessary to engage other cortical targets in neurostimulation intervention.

Researchers are encouraged to examine the efficacy and optimal parameters necessary for misophonia interventions that utilize neurostimulation alone, or in conjunction with a behavioral intervention (see Table 4 for examples of the parameters to use.). It is important to highlight that the possible avenues for intervention presented in this paper are by no means exhaustive. Researchers should continue to examine the therapeutic potential of altering other circuits and brain regions as new theoretical findings and refined hypotheses emerge.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Examples of specific parameters that are based on research findings or other protocols where similar goals (e.g., reducing hyperconnectivity) were accomplished for different types of neurostimulation discussed in this review.

In conclusion, a novel neurostimulation intervention could be an effective way to help sufferers. For example, rTMS clinics are available in all 50 states, and over 250 million Americans have insurance plans that cover this approach to treatment (Neurostar, 2021). TDCS equipment is affordable and accessible, which has led to exciting innovations in administering tDCS interventions remotely, by sending devices at home and teaching patients how to independently use them (Charvet et al., 2015; Hordacre, 2018). In addition, TBS is emerging as the most efficient way to administer neurostimulation in a very short amount of time, allowing for massed sessions (Chung et al., 2016; Pabst et al., 2022). Furthermore, the funding for neurostimulation is skyrocketing, with hundreds of millions of dollars being raised worldwide to fund technology advancements (Albert, 2020). In short, neurostimulation is becoming the frontier for developing novel treatments worldwide and misophonia treatment research should harness this enthusiasm.

Author contributions

AN conceptualized the review, summarized the research on misophonia, and wrote the section on neurostimulation treatment considerations. The co-authors each contributed several paragraphs to the other sections of the review. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

REAM Misophonia Grant (248771), supported part of AN effort during the writing of this review.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the Duke Misophonia and Emotion Regulation Center and the Ream Foundation for their support with our examination of the misophonia literature.

Supplementary materiala

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.893903/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ It is important to highlight that misophonia per se was not measured in this study.

References

Aazh, H., Landgrebe, M., Danesh, A. A., and Moore, B. C. (2019). Cognitive behavioral therapy for alleviating the distress caused by tinnitus, hyperacusis and misophonia: current perspectives. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 12:991. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S179138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abercrombie, H. C., Schaefer, S. M., Larson, C. L., Oakes, T. R., Lindgren, K. A., Holden, J. E., et al. (1998). Metabolic rate in the right amygdala predicts negative affect in depressed patients. Neuroreport 9, 3301–3307. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Addicott, M., Luber, B., Nguyen, D., Palmer, H., Lisanby, S., and Appelbaum, L. (2019). Low and high frequency rTMS effects on resting-state functional connectivity between the postcentral gyrus and the insula. Brain Connect. 9, 322–328. doi: 10.1089/brain.2018.0652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albajara Sáenz, A., Villemonteix, T., and Massat, I. (2019). Structural and functional neuroimaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 61, 399–405.

Google Scholar

Albert, H. (2020). Investment is Pouring into Neuromodulation Treatments. Berlin: Labiotech.

Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Schubert, F., Bohm, F., Ittermann, B., Aydin, S., Hayek, D., et al. (2017). tDCS-induced modulation of GABA levels and resting-state functional connectivity in older adults. J. Neurosci. 37, 4065–4073.

Google Scholar

Apergis-Schoute, A. M., Bijleveld, B., Gillan, C. M., Fineberg, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2018). Hyperconnectivity of the vmPFC in OCD. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2, 1–10. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1609194114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atmaca, M., Korucu, T., Tabara, M. F., Yildirim, H., and Kılıç, M. C. (2019). Volumetric MRI study of orbito-frontal cortex and thalamus in obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. J. Clin. Neurosci. 64, 89–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2019.03.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avery, J. A., Drevets, W. C., Moseman, S. E., Bodurka, J., Barcalow, J. C., and Simmons, W. K. (2014). Major depressive disorder is associated with abnormal interoceptive activity and functional connectivity in the insula. Biol. Psychiatry. 76, 258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baeken, C. (2019). Accelerated neurostimulation in major depression: insights from brain imaging. Brain Stimul. 12:464.

Google Scholar

Baeken, C., De Raedt, R., Van Schuerbeek, P., Vanderhasselt, M.-A., De Mey, J., Bossuyt, A., et al. (2010). Right prefrontal HF-rTMS attenuates right amygdala processing of negatively valenced emotional stimuli in healthy females. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 450–455. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baeken, C., Marinazzo, D., Van Schuerbeek, P., Wu, G.-R., De Mey, J., Luypaert, R., et al. (2014). Left and right amygdala-mediofrontal cortical functional connectivity is differentially modulated by harm avoidance. PLoS One 9:e95740. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, K. B., and Kim, J. J. (2004). Amygdalar lateralization in fear conditioning: evidence for greater involvement of the right amygdala. Behav. Neurosci. 118:15. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.1.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beam, W., Borckardt, J. J., Reeves, S. T., and George, M. S. (2009). An efficient and accurate new method for locating the F3 position for prefrontal TMS applications. Brain Stimul. 2, 50–54. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauregard, M., Paquette, V., and Le, J. (2006). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotional self-regulation in major depressive disorder. Neuroreport 17, 843–846.

Google Scholar

Bechdolf, A., Wood, S. J., Nelson, B., Velakoulis, D., Yücel, M., Takahashi, T., et al. (2012). Amygdala and insula volumes prior to illness onset in bipolar disorder: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Res. 201, 34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthier, M. L., Torres-Prioris, M. J., and López-Barroso, D. (2017). Thinking on treating echolalia in aphasia: recommendations and caveats for future research directions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:164. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bestmann, S., and Feredoes, E. (2013). Combined neurostimulation and neuroimaging in cognitive neuroscience: past, present, and future. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1296, 11–30. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beucke, J. C., Sepulcre, J., Eldaief, M. C., Sebold, M., Kathmann, N., and Kaufmann, C. (2014). Default mode network subsystem alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 205, 376–382.

Google Scholar

Beynel, L., Appelbaum, L. G., Luber, B., Crowell, C. A., Hilbig, S. A., Lim, W., et al. (2019). Effects of online repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on cognitive processing: a meta-analysis and recommendations for future studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 107, 47–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beynel, L., Campbell, E., Naclerio, M., Galla, J. T., Ghosal, A., Michael, A. M., et al. (2021). The effects of functionally guided, connectivity-based rTMS on amygdala activation. Brain Sci. 11:494. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11040494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beynel, L., Powers, J. P., and Appelbaum, L. G. (2020). Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on resting-state connectivity: a systematic review. Neuroimage. 211:116596.

Google Scholar

Bigler, E. D., Mortensen, S., Neeley, E. S., Ozonoff, S., Krasny, L., Johnson, M., et al. (2007). Superior temporal gyrus, language function, and autism. Dev. Neuropsychol. 31, 217–238.

Google Scholar

Binkofski, F., and Buccino, G. (2006). The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 396–405.

Google Scholar

Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., and Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain 133, 1515–1525. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, W. R., Lepping, R. J., Bruce, A. S., Powell, J. N., Bruce, J. M., Martin, L. E., et al. (2014). Tonic hyper-connectivity of reward neurocircuitry in obese children. Obesity 22, 1590–1593. doi: 10.1002/oby.20741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumberger, D. M., Vila-Rodriguez, F., Thorpe, K. E., Feffer, K., Noda, Y., Giacobbe, P., et al. (2018). Effectiveness of theta burst versus high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with depression (THREE-D): a randomised non-inferiority trial. Lancet 391, 1683–1692.

Google Scholar

Boucher, O., Turgeon, C., Champoux, S., Ménard, L., Rouleau, I., Lassonde, M., et al. (2015). Hyperacusis following unilateral damage to the insular cortex: a three-case report. Brain Res. 1606, 102–112. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boussaoud, D. (2001). Attention versus intention in the primate premotor cortex. Neuroimage 14, S40–S45.

Google Scholar

Brout, J. J., Edelstein, M., Erfanian, M., Mannino, M., Miller, L. J., Rouw, R., et al. (2018). Investigating misophonia: a review of the empirical literature, clinical implications, and a research agenda. Front. Neurosci. 12:36. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 400–404.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bungert, A., Antunes, A., Espenhahn, S., and Thielscher, A. (2017). Where does TMS stimulate the motor cortex? Combining electrophysiological measurements and realistic field estimates to reveal the affected cortex position. Cereb. Cortex 27, 5083–5094. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cash, R. F., Dar, A., Hui, J., De Ruiter, L., Baarbé, J., Fettes, P., et al. (2017). Influence of inter-train interval on the plastic effects of rTMS. Brain Stimul. 10, 630–636.

Google Scholar

Cavanna, A. E., and Seri, S. (2015). Misophonia: current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 11:2117.

Google Scholar

Cerliani, L., and Rouw, R. (2020). Increased orbitofrontal connectivity in misophonia. bioRxiv [Preprint] doi: 10.1101/2020.10.29.346650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, D., Zhang, J., Peng, W., Shen, Z., Gao, X., Du, Y., et al. (2018). Smoking cessation with 20 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) applied to two brain regions: a pilot study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:344. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M., Womer, F. Y., Edmiston, E. K., Bai, C., Zhou, Q., Jiang, X., et al. (2018). Neurobiological commonalities and distinctions among three major psychiatric diagnostic categories: a structural MRI study. Schizophr. Bull. 44, 65–74. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charvet, L. E., Kasschau, M., Datta, A., Knotkova, H., Stevens, M. C., Alonzo, A., et al. (2015). Remotely-supervised transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for clinical trials: guidelines for technology and protocols. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:26. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chauhan, P., Jethwa, K., Rathawa, A., Chauhan, G., and Mehra, S. (2021). “The anatomy of the hippocampus,” in Cerebral Ischemia, ed. R. Pluta (Brisbane, AU: Exon Publications).

Google Scholar

Chen, B. K., Sassi, R., Axelson, D., Hatch, J. P., Sanches, M., Nicoletti, M., et al. (2004). Cross-sectional study of abnormal amygdala development in adolescents and young adults with bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 399–405. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, R., Classen, J., Gerloff, C., Celnik, P., Wassermann, E., Hallett, M., et al. (1997). Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 48, 1398–1403.

Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Juhás, M., Greenshaw, A. J., Hu, Q., Meng, X., Cui, H., et al. (2016). Abnormal resting-state functional connectivity of the left caudate nucleus in OCD. Neurosci. Lett. 623, 57–62.

Google Scholar

Chung, S. W., Hill, A. T., Rogasch, N. C., Hoy, K. E., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2016). Use of TBS in changing excitability of motor cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 63, 43–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., Steele, J. S., Lenow, J. K., Smitherman, S., Everett, B., Messias, E., et al. (2014). Functional reorganization of neural networks during repeated exposure to the traumatic memory in posttraumatic stress disorder: an exploratory fMRI study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 48, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claiborn, J. M., Dozier, T. H., Hart, S. L., and Lee, J. (2020). Self-identified misophonia phenomenology, impact, and clinical correlates. Psychol. Thought 13, 349–375.

Google Scholar

Coutinho, J., Goncalves, O. F., Soares, J. M., Marques, P., and Sampaio, A. (2016). Alterations of the default mode network connectivity in OCPD: a pilot study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 256, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, D. A., Varnerin, N., Machado, A., Bonnett, C., Janini, D., Roelle, S., et al. (2015). Stimulation targeting higher motor areas in stroke rehabilitation: a proof-of-concept, randomized, double-blinded placebo-controlled study of effectiveness and underlying mechanisms. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 33, 911–926. doi: 10.3233/RNN-150574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalong, G., Jiyuan, L., Yubin, Z., Yufei, Q., Jinghua, Y., Cong, W., et al. (2021). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the right temporoparietal junction suppresses its functional connectivity and reduces contralateral spatial and temporal perception. Front. Neurosci. 15:629331. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.629331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daniels, E. C., Rodriguez, A., and Zabelina, D. L. (2020). Severity of misophonia symptoms is associated with worse cognitive control when exposed to misophonia trigger sounds. PLoS One 15:e0227118. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Brito, S. A., Mechelli, A., Wilke, M., Laurens, K. R., Jones, A. P., Barker, G. J., et al. (2009). Size matters: increased grey matter in boys with conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits. Brain 132, 843–852.

Google Scholar

Deng, Z. D., Lisanby, S. H., and Peterchev, A. V. (2013). Electric field depth-focality tradeoff in transcranial magnetic stimulation: simulation comparison of 50 coil designs. Brain Stimul. 6, 1–13.

Google Scholar

Dirlikov, B., Rosch, K. S., Crocetti, D., Denckla, M. B., Mahone, E. M., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Distinct frontal lobe morphology in girls and boys with ADHD. Neuroimage 7, 222–229.

Google Scholar

Dozier, T. H. (2015). Treating the initial physical reflex of misophonia with the neural repatterning technique: a counterconditioning procedure. Psychol. Thought 8, 189–210.

Google Scholar

Dozier, T. H., and Morrison, K. L. (2017). Phenomenology of misophonia: initial physical and emotional responses. Am. J. Psychol. 130, 431–438.

Google Scholar

Droutman, V., Bechara, A., and Read, S. J. (2015). Roles of the different sub-regions of the insular cortex in various phases of the decision-making process. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:309. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutta, P., Dhyani, M., Garg, S., Tikka, S. K., Khattri, S., Mehta, S., et al. (2021). Efficacy of intensive orbitofrontal continuous theta burst stimulation (iOFcTBS) in obsessive compulsive disorder: a randomized placebo controlled study. Psychiatry Res. 298:113784.

Google Scholar

Economo, C. V. (1926). Eine neue Art Spezialzellen des Lobus cinguli und Lobus insulae. Zeitschrift Gesamte Neurol. Psychiatr. 100, 706–712.

Google Scholar

Eijsker, N., Schröder, A., Smit, D. J., van Wingen, G., and Denys, D. (2019). Neural basis of response bias on the Stop Signal Task in misophonia. Front. Psychiatry 10:765. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eijsker, N., Schröder, A., Liebrand, L. C., Smit, D. J., van Wingen, G., and Denys, D. (2021a). White matter abnormalities in misophonia. Neuroimage 32:102787. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eijsker, N., Schröder, A., Smit, D. J., van Wingen, G., and Denys, D. (2021b). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in misophonia. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 52, 62–71.

Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I. (2015). Meta-Analytic Evidence for the Role of the Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Social Pain. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1–2.

Google Scholar

Ellard, K. K., Gosai, A. K., Felicione, J. M., Peters, A. T., Shea, C. V., Sylvia, L. G., et al. (2019). Deficits in frontoparietal activation and anterior insula functional connectivity during regulation of cognitive-affective interference in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 21, 244–258. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erfanian, M., Kartsonaki, C., and Keshavarz, A. (2019). Misophonia and comorbid psychiatric symptoms: a preliminary study of clinical findings. Nordic J. Psychiatry 73, 219–228. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2019.1609086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., Zhong, M., Zhu, X., Gan, J., Liu, W., Niu, C., et al. (2017). Resting-state functional connectivity between right anterior insula and right OFC correlate with insight level in OCD. Neuroimage Clin. 15, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., and Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 903–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeser, M., Prehn, K., Kazzer, P., Mungee, A., and Bajbouj, M. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances cognitive control during emotion regulation. Brain Stimul. 7, 105–112.

Google Scholar

Ferrer-Torres, A., and Giménez-Llort, L. (2021). Sounds of silence in times of COVID-19: distress and loss of cardiac coherence in people with misophonia caused by real, imagined or evoked triggering sounds. Front. Psychiatry 12:638949. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.638949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flynn, F. G. (1999). Anatomy of the insula functional and clinical correlates. Aphasiology 13, 55–78.

Google Scholar

Fox, M. D. (2018). Mapping symptoms to brain networks with the human connectome. N. Engl. J. Med. 379, 2237–2245.

Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Buckner, R. L., White, M. P., Greicius, M. D., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2012). Efficacy of TMS targets for depression is related to intrinsic functional connectivity with the subgenual cingulate. Biol. Psychiatry. 72, 595–603.

Google Scholar

Fregni, F., Nitsche, M., Loo, C., Brunoni, A., Marangolo, P., Leite, J., et al. (2015). Regulatory considerations for the clinical and research use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): review and recommendations from an expert panel. Clin. Res. Regul. Aff. 32, 22–35. doi: 10.3109/10601333.2015.980944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghaziri, J., Tucholka, A., Girard, G., Houde, J.-C., Boucher, O., Gilbert, G., et al. (2017). The corticocortical structural connectivity of the human insula. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1216–1228.

Google Scholar

Gilboa, A., Shalev, A. Y., Laor, L., Lester, H., Louzoun, Y., Chisin, R., et al. (2004). Functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 263–272.

Google Scholar

Goerlich-Dobre, K. S., Lamm, C., Pripfl, J., Habel, U., and Votinov, M. (2015). The left amygdala: a shared substrate of alexithymia and empathy. Neuroimage 122, 20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, L., Yin, Y., He, C., Ye, Q., Bai, F., Yuan, Y., et al. (2017). Disrupted reward circuits is associated with cognitive deficits and depression severity in major depressive disorder. J. Psych. Res. 84, 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, O. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Oldfield, V. B., and Carter, N. (2013). The association between obsessive compulsive disorder and obsessive compulsive personality disorder: prevalence and clinical presentation. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 52, 300–315.

Google Scholar

Gorka, S. M., Nelson, B. D., Phan, K. L., and Shankman, S. A. (2014). Insula response to unpredictable and predictable aversiveness in individuals with panic disorder & comorbid depression. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 4:9. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-4-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. P., Müller, V. I., Eickhoff, S. B., and Fox, P. T. (2020). Multimodal abnormalities of brain structure and function in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Am. J. Psych. 177, 422–434. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19050560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groenewold, N. A., Opmeer, E. M., de Jonge, P., Aleman, A., and Costafreda, S. G. (2013). Emotional valence modulates brain functional abnormalities in depression: evidence from a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 152–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2013). Handbook of Emotion Regulation, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press, 669.

Google Scholar

Gu, X., Hof, P. R., Friston, K. J., and Fan, J. (2013). Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 3371–3388.

Google Scholar

Gurok, M. G., Korucu, T., Kilic, M. C., Yildirim, H., and Atmaca, M. (2019). Hippocampus and amygdalar volumes in patients with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. J. Clin. Neurosci. 64, 259–263. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2019.03.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, H. A., Leber, A. B., and Saygin, Z. M. (2021). What sound sources trigger misophonia? Not just chewing and breathing. J. Clin. Psychol. 77, 2609–2625.

Google Scholar

Harricharan, S., Nicholson, A. A., Thome, J., Densmore, M., McKinnon, M. C., Théberge, J., et al. (2020). PTSD and its dissociative subtype through the lens of the insula: anterior and posterior insula resting-state functional connectivity and its predictive validity using machine learning. Psychophysiology 57:e13472. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holroyd, C. B., and Yeung, N. (2011). “An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function: option selection in hierarchical reinforcement learning,” in Neural Basis of Motivational & Cognitive Control, eds R. B. Mars, J. Sallet, M. F. S. Rushworth, and N. Yeung (Cambridge, MA: MIT), 333–349.

Google Scholar

Holtzheimer, P. E., and McDonald, W. (2014). A Clinical Guide to TMS. Oxford: Oxford Press.

Google Scholar

Hordacre, B. (2018). The role of telehealth to assist in-home tDCS: opportunities, promising results and acceptability. Brain Sci. 8:102. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8060102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, J. C., Mathews, J., Demitrack, M. A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). The NeuroStar TMS device: conducting the FDA approved protocol for treatment of depression. J. Vis. Exp. 45:e2345.

Google Scholar

Huang, Y.-Z., Edwards, M. J., Rounis, E., Bhatia, K. P., and Rothwell, J. C. (2005). Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45, 201–206.

Google Scholar

Ironside, M., Browning, M., Ansari, T. L., Harvey, C. J., Sekyi-Djan, M. N., Bishop, S. J., et al. (2019). Effect of prefrontal cortex stimulation on regulation of amygdala response to threat in individuals with trait anxiety: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 76, 71–78. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isserles, M., Tendler, A., Roth, Y., Bystritsky, A., Blumberger, D. M., Ward, H., et al. (2021). Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation combined with brief exposure for post-traumatic stress disorder–a prospective multisite randomized trial. Biol. Psychiatry 90, 721–728. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itami, S., and Uno, H. (2002). Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder revealed by reversal and extinction tasks. Neuroreport 13, 2453–2457. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200212200-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jager, I., de Koning, P., Bost, T., Denys, D., and Vulink, N. (2020). Misophonia: phenomenology, comorbidity and demographics in a large sample. PLoS One 15:e0231390. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastreboff, P. J., and Hazell, J. W. (2008). Tinnitus Retraining Therapy: Implementing the Neurophysiological Model. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Jog, M. S., Kim, E., Anderson, C., Kubicki, A., Kayathi, R., Jann, K., et al. (2021). In-vivo imaging of targeting and modulation of depression-relevant circuitry by transcranial direct current stimulation: a randomized clinical trial. Transl. Psychiatry 11:138. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01264-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaas, J. H., and Stepniewska, I. (2002). “Motor cortex,” in Encyclopedia of the Human Brain, Vol. 2, ed. V. S. Ramachandran (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 159–169.

Google Scholar

Kahnt, T., Chang, L. J., Park, S. Q., Heinzle, J., and Haynes, J.-D. (2012). Connectivity-based parcellation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 6240–6250.

Google Scholar

Kan, R. L., Zhang, B. B., Zhang, J. J., and Kranz, G. S. (2020). Non-invasive brain stimulation for posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 10:168. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0851-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, M., Liu, C., Schaer, M., Parker, K. J., Ottet, M.-C., Epps, A., et al. (2009). Prefrontal plasticity and stress inoculation-induced resilience. Dev. Neurosci. 31, 293–299. doi: 10.1159/000216540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 593–602.

Google Scholar

Kiernan, J. (2012). Anatomy of the temporal lobe. Epilepsy Res. Treat. 2012:176157.

Google Scholar

Kılıç, C., Öz, G., Avanoğlu, K. B., and Aksoy, S. (2021). The prevalence and characteristics of misophonia in Ankara, Turkey: population-based study. BJPsych open 7:e144. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2021.978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, A., Mensh, B., Ghosh, S., Tourville, J., and Hirsch, J. (2005). Mindboggle: automated brain labeling with multiple atlases. BMC Med. Imaging. 5:7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2342-5-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinhans, N. M., Richards, T., Weaver, K., Johnson, L. C., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., et al. (2010). Association between amygdala response to emotional faces and social anxiety in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 48, 3665–3670.

Google Scholar

Kliuchko, M., Puoliväli, T., Heinonen-Guzejev, M., Tervaniemi, M., Toiviainen, P., Sams, M., et al. (2018). Neuroanatomical substrate of noise sensitivity. Neuroimage 167, 309–315.

Google Scholar

Kozel, F. A., Motes, M. A., Didehbani, N., DeLaRosa, B., Bass, C., Schraufnagel, C. D., et al. (2018). Repetitive TMS to augment cognitive processing therapy in combat veterans of recent conflicts with PTSD: a randomized clinical trial. J. Affect. Disord. 229, 506–514. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.12.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, S., Dheerendra, P., Erfanian, M., Benzaquén, E., Sedley, W., Gander, P. E., et al. (2021). The motor basis for misophonia. J. Neurosci. 41, 5762–5770.

Google Scholar

Kumar, S., Tansley-Hancock, O., Sedley, W., Winston, J. S., Callaghan, M. F., Allen, M., et al. (2017). The brain basis for misophonia. Curr. Biol. 27, 527–533.

Google Scholar

Kumfor, F., Irish, M., Hodges, J. R., and Piguet, O. (2013). The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in emotional enhancement of memory: evidence from the dementias. Brain 136, 2992–3003. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunimatsu, A., Yasaka, K., Akai, H., Kunimatsu, N., and Abe, O. (2020). MRI findings in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 52, 380–396.

Google Scholar

Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Boksman, K., Densmore, M., Gupta, M., Neufeld, R. W., et al. (2002). Brain activation during script-driven imagery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 305–311.

Google Scholar

Larrivee, D. (2020). “Introductory chapter: neurostimulation and the structural basis of brain activity,” in Neurostimulation and Neuromodulation in Contemporary Therapeutic Practice, eds D. Larrivee and S. M. Rayegani (London: IntechOpen).

Google Scholar

Lefaucheur, J.-P., Antal, A., Ayache, S. S., Benninger, D. H., Brunelin, J., Cogiamanian, F., et al. (2017). Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of tDCS. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 56–92.

Google Scholar

Lei, H., Huang, L., Li, J., Liu, W., Fan, J., Zhang, X., et al. (2020). Altered spontaneous brain activity in obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 96:152144. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2019.152144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemus, L., Hernández, A., and Romo, R. (2009). Neural encoding of auditory discrimination in ventral premotor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 14640–14645. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907505106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., Metzger, C. D., Li, W., Safron, A., van Tol, M.-J., Lord, A., et al. (2014). Dissociation of glutamate and cortical thickness is restricted to regions subserving trait but not state markers in major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 169, 91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Du, L., Sahlem, G. L., Badran, B. W., Henderson, S., and George, M. S. (2017a). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the dlPFC reduces resting-state insula activity and modulates functional connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex in cigarette smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 174, 98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Du, L., Sahlem, G. L., Badran, B. W., Henderson, S., and George, M. S. (2017b). RTMS of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reduces resting-state insula activity and modulates functional connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex in cigarette smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 174, 98–105.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Cao, L., Li, H., Gao, Y., Bu, X., Liang, K., et al. (2022). Abnormal resting-state functional connectivity in patients with OCD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 135: 104574.

Google Scholar

Liu, X., Hou, Z., Yin, Y., Xie, C., Zhang, H., Zhang, H., et al. (2021). Decreased cortical thickness of left premotor cortex as a treatment predictor in major depressive disorder. Brain Imaging Behav. 15, 1420–1426. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00341-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeda, F., Keenan, J. P., Tormos, J. M., Topka, H., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2000). Interindividual variability of the modulatory effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical excitability. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 425–430.

Google Scholar

Maier, S., Szalkowski, A., Kamphausen, S., Feige, B., Perlov, E., Kalisch, R., et al. (2014). Altered cingulate and amygdala response towards threat and safe cues in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol. Med. 44, 85–98. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maiti, R., Mishra, B. R., and Hota, D. (2017). Effect of high-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on craving in substance use disorder: a meta-analysis. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 29, 160–171.

Google Scholar

Marangell, L., Martinez, M., Jurdi, R., and Zboyan, H. (2007). Neurostimulation therapies in depression: a review of new modalities. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 116, 174–181.

Google Scholar

Mattavelli, G., Lo Presti, S., Tornaghi, D., and Canessa, N. (2022). High-definition transcranial direct current stimulation of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex modulates decision-making and executive control. Brain Struct. Funct. 227, 1565–1576. doi: 10.1007/s00429-022-02456-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, H., Skokauskas, N., and Frodl, T. (2014). Identifying a consistent pattern of neural function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 869–880.

Google Scholar

McErlean, A. B. J., and Banissy, M. J. (2018). Increased misophonia in self-reported autonomous sensory meridian response. PeerJ 6:e5351. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGeoch, P. D., and Rouw, R. (2020). How everyday sounds can trigger strong emotions: ASMR, misophonia and the feeling of wellbeing. Bioessays 42:2000099. doi: 10.1002/bies.202000099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondino, M., Jardri, R., Suaud-Chagny, M.-F., Saoud, M., Poulet, E., and Brunelin, J. (2016). Effects of fronto-temporal tDCS on auditory verbal hallucinations and resting-state functional connectivity of the left temporo-parietal junction in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 318–326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monosov, I. E., Haber, S. N., Leuthardt, E. C., and Jezzini, A. (2020). Anterior cingulate cortex and the control of dynamic behavior in primates. Curr. Biol. 30, R1442–R1454.

Google Scholar

Mostofsky, S. H., Rimrodt, S. L., Schafer, J. G., Boyce, A., Goldberg, M. C., Pekar, J. J., et al. (2006). Atypical motor and sensory cortex activation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of simple sequential finger tapping. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Wolf, R. C., Baskaya, M. K., and Koenigs, M. (2015). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 276–284.

Google Scholar

Murphy, G. Jr., Inger, P., Mark, K., Lin, J., Morrice, W., Gee, C., et al. (1987). Volumetric asymmetry in the human amygdaloid complex. J. Hirnforsch. 28, 281–289.

Google Scholar

Naidich, T. P., Kang, E., Fatterpekar, G. M., Delman, B. N., Gultekin, S. H., Wolfe, D., et al. (2004). The insula: anatomic study and MR imaging display at 1.5 T. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 25, 222–232.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Natalini, E., Dimaggio, G., Varakliotis, T., Fioretti, A., and Eibenstein, A. (2020). Misophonia, maladaptive schemas and personality disorders: a report of three cases. J. Contemp. Psychother. 50, 29–35.

Google Scholar

Nauczyciel, C., Le Jeune, F., Naudet, F., Douabin, S., Esquevin, A., Vérin, M., et al. (2014). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the orbitofrontal cortex for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a double-blind, crossover study. Transl. Psychiatry 4:e436. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neacsiu, A. D., and Lisanby, S. H. (2015). “Magnetic stimulation for depression: subconvulsive and convulsive approaches,” in Neuromodulation in Psychiatry, eds C. Hamani, P. Holtzheimer, A. M. Lozano, and H. Mayberg (Oxford: Wiley Blackwell), 155–180.

Google Scholar

Neacsiu, A. D., Beynel, L., Graner, J. L., Szabo, S. T., Appelbaum, L. G., Smoski, M. J., et al. (2022a). Enhancing cognitive restructuring with concurrent fMRI-guided neurostimulation for emotional dysregulation: a randomized controlled trial. J. Affect. Disord. 301, 378–389. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.01.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neacsiu, A. D., Beynel, L., Powers, J. P., Szabo, S. T., Appelbaum, L. G., Lisanby, S. H., et al. (2022b). Enhancing cognitive restructuring with concurrent repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for transdiagnostic psychopathology: a proof of concept randomized controlled trial. Psychother. Psychosom. 91, 94–106. doi: 10.1159/000518957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neacsiu, A. D., Luber, B. M., Davis, S. W., Bernhardt, E., Strauman, T. J., and Lisanby, S. H. (2018). On the concurrent use of self-system therapy and functional magnetic resonance imaging-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation as treatment for depression. J. ECT 34, 266–273. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

NeuroStar (2021). Find a Doctor. Malvern, PA: NeuroStar.

Google Scholar

Norman, L. J., Taylor, S. F., Liu, Y., Radua, J., Chye, Y., De Wit, S. J., et al. (2019). Error processing and inhibitory control in OCD: a meta-analysis using statistical parametric maps. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 713–725. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Doherty, D. C. M., Ryder, W., Paquola, C., Tickell, A., Chan, C., Hermens, D. F., et al. (2018). White matter integrity alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1327–1338.

Google Scholar

Oishi, K., Faria, A. V., Hsu, J., Tippett, D., Mori, S., and Hillis, A. E. (2015). Critical role of the right uncinate fasciculus in emotional empathy. Ann. Neurol. 77, 68–74. doi: 10.1002/ana.24300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opitz, A., Fox, M. D., Craddock, R. C., Colcombe, S., and Milham, M. P. (2016). An integrated framework for targeting functional networks via transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 127, 86–96.

Google Scholar

Overbeek, G., Gawne, T. J., Reid, M. A., Kraguljac, N. V., and Lahti, A. C. (2021). A multimodal neuroimaging study investigating resting-state connectivity, glutamate and GABA at 7 T in first-episode psychosis. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 46, E702–E710. doi: 10.1503/jpn.210107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pabst, A., Proksch, S., Médé, B., Comstock, D. C., Ross, J. M., and Balasubramaniam, R. (2022). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the efficacy of intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) on cognitive enhancement. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 135:104587. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palumbo, D. B., Alsalman, O., De Ridder, D., Song, J.-J., and Vanneste, S. (2018). Misophonia and potential underlying mechanisms: a perspective. Front. Psychol. 9:953. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardo-Vazquez, J. L., Leboran, V., and Acuna, C. (2008). Neural correlates of decisions and their outcomes in the ventral premotor cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 12396–12408.

Google Scholar

Pardo-Vazquez, J. L., Leboran, V., and Acuña, C. (2009). A role for the ventral premotor cortex beyond performance monitoring. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 18815–18819. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910524106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., Valls-Solé, J., Wassermann, E. M., and Hallett, M. (1994). Responses to rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex. Brain 117, 847–858.

Google Scholar

Peng, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., Cheung, E. F. C., Jin, Z., Miao, G., Jing, J., et al. (2012). Brain structural abnormalities in OCD: converging evidence from white matter and grey matter. Asian J. Psych. 5, 290–296.

Google Scholar

Philip, N. S., Barredo, J., van ’t Wout-Frank, M., Tyrka, A. R., Price, L. H., and Carpenter, L. L. (2018). Network mechanisms of clinical response to transcranial magnetic stimulation in posttraumatic stress disorder and major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philippi, C. L., Mehta, S., Grabowski, T., Adolphs, R., and Rudrauf, D. (2009). Damage to association fiber tracts impairs recognition of the facial expression of emotion. J. Neurosci. 29, 15089–15099. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0796-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, L. H., and Della Sala, S. (1998). Aging, intelligence, and anatomical segregation in the frontal lobes. Learn. Individ. Differ. 10, 217–243.

Google Scholar

Phillips, M. L., and Swartz, H. A. (2014). A critical appraisal of neuroimaging studies of bipolar disorder: toward a new conceptualization of underlying neural circuitry and a road map for future research. Am. J. Psychiatry 171, 829–843. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13081008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picó-Pérez, M., Ipser, J., Taylor, P., Alonso, P., López-Solà, C., Real, E., et al. (2019). Intrinsic functional and structural connectivity of emotion regulation networks in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety 36, 110–120. doi: 10.1002/da.22845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pico-Perez, M., Radua, J., Steward, T., Menchon, J. M., and Soriano-Mas, C. (2017). Emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of fMRI cognitive reappraisal studies. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 79(Pt. B), 96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piek, J. P., and Dyck, M. J. (2004). Sensory-motor deficits in children with developmental coordination disorder, ADHD and autistic disorder. Hum. Mov Sci. 23, 475–488. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2004.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Japee, S., Rauth, L., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2017). The superior temporal sulcus is causally connected to the amygdala: a combined TBS-fMRI Study. J. Neurosci. 37, 1156–1161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0114-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. P., and LaBar, K. S. (2019). Regulating emotion through distancing: a taxonomy, neurocognitive model, and supporting meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 96, 155–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. P., Davis, S. W., Neacsiu, A. D., Beynel, L., Appelbaum, L. G., and LaBar, K. S. (2020a). Examining the role of lateral parietal cortex in emotional distancing using TMS. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 20, 1090–1102. doi: 10.3758/s13415-020-00821-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. P., Graner, J. L., and LaBar, K. S. (2020b). Multivariate patterns of posterior cortical activity differentiate forms of emotional distancing. Cereb. Cortex 30, 2766–2776. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quek, T. C., Ho, C. S., Choo, C. C., Nguyen, L. H., Tran, B. X., and Ho, R. C. (2018). Misophonia in Singaporean psychiatric patients: a cross-sectional study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:1410. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15071410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reetz, K., Lencer, R., Steinlechner, S., Gaser, C., Hagenah, J., Büchel, C., et al. (2008). Limbic and frontal cortical degeneration is associated with psychiatric symptoms in PINK1 mutation carriers. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 241–247. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rocha-Rego, V., Pereira, M. G., Oliveira, L., Mendlowicz, M. V., Fiszman, A., Marques-Portella, C., et al. (2012). Decreased premotor cortex volume in victims of urban violence with PTSD. PLoS One 7:e42560. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (1996). The orbitofrontal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 351, 1433–1444.

Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T., and Grabenhorst, F. (2008). The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog. Neurobiol. 86, 216–244.

Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., and Pascual-Leone, A., and Safety of Tms Consensus Group (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of TMS in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Antal, A., Bestmann, S., Bikson, M., Brewer, C., Brockmöller, J., et al. (2021). Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: expert guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 132, 269–306. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, Y., Tendler, A., Arikan, M. K., Vidrine, R., Kent, D., Muir, O., et al. (2021). Real-world efficacy of deep TMS for OCD: post-marketing data collected from twenty-two clinical sites. J. Psych. Res. 137, 667–672. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouw, R., and Erfanian, M. (2018). A large-scale study of misophonia. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 453–479.

Google Scholar

Ruspantini, I., Mäki, H., Korhonen, R., D’Ausilio, A., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2011). The functional role of the PMv in a visually paced finger tapping task: a TMS study. Behav. Brain Res. 220, 325–330. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomons, T. V., Dunlop, K., Kennedy, S. H., Flint, A., Geraci, J., Giacobbe, P., et al. (2014). Resting-state cortico-thalamic-striatal connectivity predicts response to dorsomedial prefrontal rTMS in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 488–498. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

San Giorgi, R. (2015). Hyperactivity in amygdala and auditory cortex in misophonia: preliminary results of a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Amsterdam Brain Cogn. J. 2, 21–28.

Google Scholar

Sanchez, T. G., and Silva, F. E. D. (2018). Misofonia familiar ou síndrome da sensibilidade seletiva a sons: evidência de herança autossômica dominante? Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 84, 553–559.

Google Scholar

Santos, A. D. H. M., Santos, A. P. S., Santos, H. S., and Silva, A. C. D. (2018). The use of tDCS as a therapeutic option for tinnitus: a systematic review. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 84, 653–659.

Google Scholar

Schienle, A., Schäfer, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., and Vaitl, D. (2005). Neural responses of OCD patients towards disorder-relevant, generally disgust-inducing and fear-inducing pictures. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schröder, A., van Wingen, G., Eijsker, N., San Giorgi, R., Vulink, N. C., Turbyne, C., et al. (2019). Misophonia is associated with altered brain activity in the auditory cortex and salience network. Sci. Rep. 9:7542.

Google Scholar

Schröder, A., Vulink, N., and Denys, D. (2013). Misophonia: diagnostic criteria for a new psychiatric disorder. PLoS One 8:e54706. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W. (2010). Anterior insula degeneration in frontotemporal dementia. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 465–475.

Google Scholar

Seewoo, B. J., Rodger, J., Demitrack, M. A., Heart, K. L., Port, J. D., Strawn, J. R., et al. (2022). Neurostructural differences in adolescents with treatment resistant depression and treatment effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. Online ahead of print, doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyac007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Séguin, J. R. (2004). Neurocognitive elements of antisocial behavior: relevance of an orbitofrontal cortex account. Brain Cogn. 55, 185–197. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00273-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenhav, A., and Greene, J. D. (2014). Integrative moral judgment: dissociating the roles of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 34, 4741–4749. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3390-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, L. M., Rauch, S. L., and Pitman, R. K. (2006). Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD. Ann. N.Y Acad. Sci. 1071, 67–79.

Google Scholar

Siepsiak, M., and Dragan, W. (2019). Misophonia-a review of research results and theoretical concepts. Psychiatr. Pol. 53, 447–458.

Google Scholar

Simmons, A. N., Stein, M. B., Strigo, I. A., Arce, E., Hitchcock, C., and Paulus, M. P. (2011). Anxiety positive subjects show altered processing in the anterior insula during anticipation of negative stimuli. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1836–1846. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simner, J., Koursarou, S., Rinaldi, L., and Ward, J. (2022). Attention, flexibility, and imagery in misophonia: does attention exacerbate everyday disliking of sound? J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 43, 1006–1017. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2022.2056581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, D., Adler, N., Kaufmann, C., and Kathmann, N. (2014). Amygdala hyperactivation during symptom provocation in OCD and its modulation by distraction. Neuroimage 4, 549–557. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162.

Google Scholar

Sonmez, A. I., Camsari, D. D., Nandakumar, A. L., Voort, J. L. V., Kung, S., Lewis, C. P., et al. (2019). Accelerated TMS for depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 273, 770–781. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spoletini, I., Piras, F., Fagioli, S., Rubino, I. A., Martinotti, G., Siracusano, A., et al. (2011). Suicidal attempts and increased right amygdala volume in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 125, 30–40. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislav, K., Alexander, V., Maria, P., Evgenia, N., and Boris, V. (2013). Anatomical characteristics of cingulate cortex and neuropsychological memory tests performance. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 86, 128–133.

Google Scholar

Stein, J. L., Wiedholz, L. M., Bassett, D. S., Weinberger, D. R., Zink, C. F., Mattay, V. S., et al. (2007). A validated network of effective amygdala connectivity. Neuroimage 36, 736–745.

Google Scholar

Stevens, F. L., Hurley, R. A., and Taber, K. H. (2011). Anterior cingulate cortex: unique role in cognition and emotion. J. Neuropsych. Clin. Neurosci. 23, 121–125. doi: 10.1176/jnp.23.2.jnp121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steward, T., Pico-Perez, M., Mata, F., Martinez-Zalacain, I., Cano, M., Contreras-Rodriguez, O., et al. (2016). Emotion regulation and excess weight: impaired affective processing characterized by dysfunctional insula activation and connectivity. PLoS One 11:e0152150. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutherland, M. T., Carroll, A. J., Salmeron, B. J., Ross, T. J., and Stein, E. A. (2013). Insula’s functional connectivity with ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates the impact of trait alexithymia on state tobacco craving. Psychopharmacology 228, 143–155. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3018-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sverak, T., Linhartova, P., Gajdos, M., Kuhn, M., Latalova, A., Lamos, M., et al. (2021). Brain connectivity and symptom changes after transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with borderline personality disorder. Front. Psychiatry 12:770353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.770353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swedo, S., Baguley, D. M., Denys, D., Dixon, L. J., Erfanian, M., Fioretti, A., et al. (2021). A consensus definition of misophonia: using a delphi process to reach expert agreement. medRxiv [Preprint] doi: 10.1101/2021.04.05.21254951

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szykowny, N. (2020). The Relationship Between Misophonia and Perfectionism. Palo Alto, CA: Palo Alto University.

Google Scholar

Tang, Y.-Y., Tang, R., and Posner, M. I. (2016). Mindfulness meditation improves emotion regulation and reduces drug abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 163, S13–S18.

Google Scholar

Thomaes, K., Dorrepaal, E., Draijer, N., de Ruiter, M. B., van Balkom, A. J., Smit, J. H., et al. (2010). Reduced anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal volumes in child abuse-related complex PTSD. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71:17127. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04754blu

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomason, M. E., and Thompson, P. M. (2011). Diffusion imaging, white matter, and psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 7, 63–85.

Google Scholar

Thorsen, A. L., Hagland, P., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Kvale, G., Hansen, B., et al. (2018). Emotional processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 25 functional neuroimaging studies. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 563–571.

Google Scholar

Townsend, J., and Altshuler, L. L. (2012). Emotion processing and regulation in bipolar disorder: a review. Bipolar Disord. 14, 326–339.

Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Gracco, V. L. (2009). Contribution of the pre-SMA to the production of words and non-speech oral motor gestures, as revealed by rTMS. Brain Res. 1268, 112–124. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., Sato, M., and Small, S. L. (2012). TMS-induced modulation of action sentence priming in the ventral premotor cortex. Neuropsychologia 50, 319–326. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevizol, A. P., Shiozawa, P., Cook, I. A., Sato, I. A., Kaku, C. B., Guimarães, F. B., et al. (2016). Transcranial magnetic stimulation for OCD: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. J. ECT 32, 262–266.

Google Scholar

Tsagaris, K. Z., Labar, D. R., and Edwards, D. J. (2016). A framework for combining rTMS with behavioral therapy. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:82. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuovinen, N., Yalcin-Siedentopf, N., Welte, A.-S., Siedentopf, C. M., Steiger, R., Gizewski, E. R., et al. (2022). Neurometabolite correlates with personality and stress in healthy emerging adults: a focus on sex differences. Neuroimage 247:118847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turi, Z., Lenz, M., Paulus, W., Mittner, M., and Vlachos, A. (2021). Selecting stimulation intensity in repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation studies: a systematic review between 1991 and 2020. Eur. J. Neurosci. 53, 3404–3415.

Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289.

Google Scholar

Vaidya, C. J., Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Zalecki, C. A., Elliott, G. R., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2005). Altered neural substrates of cognitive control in childhood ADHD: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1605–1613. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valchev, N., Ćurčić-Blake, B., Renken, R. J., Avenanti, A., Keysers, C., Gazzola, V., et al. (2015). cTBS delivered to the left somatosensory cortex changes its functional connectivity during rest. Neuroimage 114, 386–397.

Google Scholar

Van Den Heuvel, O. A., Veltman, D. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Cath, D. C., Van Balkom, A. J., Van Hartskamp, J., et al. (2005). Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in OCD. Arch. Gen. Psych. 62, 301–309.

Google Scholar

van Heukelum, S., Tulva, K., Geers, F. E., van Dulm, S., Ruisch, I. H., Mill, J., et al. (2021). A central role for anterior cingulate cortex in the control of pathological aggression. Curr. Biol. 31, 2321–2333.e5.

Google Scholar

Vander Ghinst, M., Bourguignon, M., de Beeck, M. O., Wens, V., Marty, B., Hassid, S., et al. (2016). Left superior temporal gyrus is coupled to attended speech in a cocktail-party auditory scene. J. Neurosci. 36, 1596–1606. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1730-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetter, N. C., Buse, J., Backhausen, L. L., Rubia, K., Smolka, M. N., and Roessner, V. (2018). Anterior insula hyperactivation in ADHD when faced with distracting negative stimuli. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 2972–2986. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viering, T., Hoekstra, P. J., Philipsen, A., Naaijen, J., Dietrich, A., Hartman, C. A., et al. (2021). Functional network topology of the right insula affects emotion dysregulation in hyperactive-impulsive attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Sci Rep. 11:15045. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-94426-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vink, J. J. T., Mandija, S., Petrov, P. I., van den Berg, C. A. T., Sommer, I. E. C., and Neggers, S. F. W. (2018). A novel concurrent TMS-fMRI method to reveal propagation patterns of prefrontal magnetic brain stimulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4580–4592. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitoratou, S., Uglik-Marucha, N., Hayes, C., Erfanian, M., Pearson, O., and Gregory, J. (2021). Item response theory investigation of misophonia auditory triggers. Audiol. Res. 11, 567–581. doi: 10.3390/audiolres11040051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Sun, Y.-W., Wan, J.-Q., Su, S.-S., Zhou, Y., and Xu, J.-R. (2012). A preliminary study of alterations in default network connectivity in PTSD patients following recent trauma. Brain Res. 1484, 50–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, C. I., Fischer, H., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., and Rauch, S. L. (2001). Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Neuroreport 12, 379–383. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, M. S., Lewin, A. B., Murphy, T. K., and Storch, E. A. (2014). Misophonia: incidence, phenomenology, and clinical correlates in an undergraduate student sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 70, 994–1007. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshimura, S., Ueda, K., Suzuki, S.-I., Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., and Yamawaki, S. (2009). Self-referential processing of negative stimuli within the ventral anterior cingulate gyrus and right amygdala. Brain Cogn. 69, 218–225. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. R., Galla, J. T., and Appelbaum, L. G. (2021). Transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for smoking cessation: an introduction for primary care clinicians. Am. J. Med. 134, 1339–1343. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2021.06.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, J., Zhou, P., Yuan, S., Wu, Y., Wang, C., Zhang, N., et al. (2022). Symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a voxel-based meta-analysis and meta-analytic connectivity modeling. J. Psychiatr. Res. 146, 125–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Wu, M. S., and Storch, E. A. (2017). Misophonia symptoms among Chinese university students: Incidence, associated impairment, and clinical correlates. J. Obsess. Comp. Related Disord. 14, 7–12.

Google Scholar

Keywords: misophonia, neuroscience, neurostimulation, intervention, review

Citation: Neacsiu AD, Szymkiewicz V, Galla JT, Li B, Kulkarni Y and Spector CW (2022) The neurobiology of misophonia and implications for novel, neuroscience-driven interventions. Front. Neurosci. 16:893903. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.893903

Received: 11 March 2022; Accepted: 28 June 2022;
Published: 25 July 2022.

Edited by:

Julia Campbell, University of Texas at Austin, United States

Reviewed by:

Mercede Erfanian, University College London, United Kingdom
Ester Benzaquén, Newcastle University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Neacsiu, Szymkiewicz, Galla, Li, Kulkarni and Spector. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrada D. Neacsiu, andrada.neacsiu@duke.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.