Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 02 November 2023
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods

The effects of trauma on feedback processing: an MEG study

  • 1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-4), Forschungszentrum Jülich GmbH, Jülich, Germany
  • 2Faculty of Medicine, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 3Palestinian Neuroscience Initiative, Al-Quds University, Abu Dis, Palestine
  • 4Faculty of Mathematics, Computer Science and Natural Sciences, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 5Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-11), Jülich Aachen Research Alliance (JARA), Forschungszentrum Jülich GmbH, Jülich, Germany
  • 6Jülich Aachen Research Alliance (JARA)-Brain – Translational Medicine, Aachen, Germany
  • 7Department of Neurology, University Hospital RWTH Aachen, Aachen, Germany
  • 8Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 9Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, United States

The cognitive impact of psychological trauma can manifest as a range of post-traumatic stress symptoms that are often attributed to impairments in learning from positive and negative outcomes, aka reinforcement learning. Research on the impact of trauma on reinforcement learning has mainly been inconclusive. This study aimed to circumscribe the impact of psychological trauma on reinforcement learning in the context of neural response in time and frequency domains. Two groups of participants were tested - those who had experienced psychological trauma and a control group who had not - while they performed a probabilistic classification task that dissociates learning from positive and negative feedback during a magnetoencephalography (MEG) examination. While the exposure to trauma did not exhibit any effects on learning accuracy or response time for positive or negative feedback, MEG cortical activity was modulated in response to positive feedback. In particular, the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortices (mOFC and lOFC) exhibited increased activity, while the insular and supramarginal cortices showed decreased activity during positive feedback presentation. Furthermore, when receiving negative feedback, the trauma group displayed higher activity in the medial portion of the superior frontal cortex. The timing of these activity changes occurred between 160 and 600 ms post feedback presentation. Analysis of the time-frequency domain revealed heightened activity in theta and alpha frequency bands (4–10 Hz) in the lOFC in the trauma group. Moreover, dividing the two groups according to their learning performance, the activity for the non-learner subgroup was found to be lower in lOFC and higher in the supramarginal cortex. These differences were found in the trauma group only. The results highlight the localization and neural dynamics of feedback processing that could be affected by exposure to psychological trauma. This approach and associated findings provide a novel framework for understanding the cognitive correlates of psychological trauma in relation to neural dynamics in the space, time, and frequency domains. Subsequent work will focus on the stratification of cognitive and neural correlates as a function of various symptoms of psychological trauma. Clinically, the study findings and approach open the possibility for neuromodulation interventions that synchronize cognitive and psychological constructs for individualized treatment.

1. Introduction

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 5th edition, (DSM-5), exposure to psychological trauma can induce four clusters of symptoms: intrusive re-experiencing of events related to the traumatic event, persistent avoidance of stimuli or memories associated with the traumatic event, negative alteration in cognition and mood and symptoms of arousal (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). For a diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) to be made, the severity and frequency of these post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) must reach a threshold for diagnosis (Megías et al., 2007). However, it is also recognized that subthreshold PTSS are debilitating in their own right (Schnurr et al., 2000; Brancu et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2020). Compared to individuals without trauma exposure, people with PTSS may experience social and functional impairments, suicidal ideations, and other psychiatric problems, including anxiety and depression (Marshall et al., 2001; Zlotnick et al., 2002; Friedman et al., 2011; Morgan-López et al., 2020).

Classically, PTSS were attributed to impairments in Pavlovian conditioning, extinction, or recall of fear (Lissek and van Meurs, 2015). However, limited research has examined reinforcement learning and the associated neural dynamics following exposure to trauma. Traumatic experiences are known to reduce positive feedback expectancy and satisfaction (Hopper et al., 2008), decrease the ability of the person to exploit information about positive feedback in their environment (Hanson et al., 2017) and increase their sensitivity to negative stimuli (Sawyer et al., 2016). However, little is known about the dynamics of brain signals during feedback processing following traumatic experiences and how it relates to learning from positive and negative feedback.

Exposure to trauma has been shown to affect the neural circuitry for feedback processing, including multiple cortical and subcortical structures. Indeed, exposure to trauma has been associated with decreased activation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; encodes stimulus value; Sescousse et al., 2013; Purves et al., 2018), higher activation in the amygdala (important for fear conditioning; Greco and Liberzon, 2016), lower connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; assessing feedback based on choices; Purves et al., 2018) and hippocampus (memory formation; Alvarez and Squire, 1994), and increased connectivity between the insula (integration of internal and somatic states; Craig, 2002; Sescousse et al., 2013; Purves et al., 2018) and other regions in the salience network (Sripada et al., 2012). Furthermore, trauma exposure has also been associated with a decrease in the size of the amygdala, insula, ACC, and mPFC (Ganzel et al., 2008). However, although response to positive and negative feedback can explain some trauma-related symptoms, it is still unclear how trauma exposure affects online processing of feedback with temporal and spatial precision.

Studying the temporal and spectral aspects of feedback processing can provide a better understanding of the cognitive effects of trauma exposure. Given that feedback components follow reproducible patterns of activity that are time- and frequency-dependent (Bernat et al., 2015), studying these components can shed more light into the processes underlying them. Previous studies have relied on electroencephalography (EEG) to investigate the effect of trauma on the temporal dynamics of feedback processing following trauma (Pechtel and Pizzagalli, 2013; Lieberman et al., 2017). However, while EEG provides a high temporal resolution, when compared to other modalities, such as fMRI, it has very low spatial resolution (Glover, 2011). To bridge the gap between temporal and spatial resolution, magnetoencephalography (MEG) would be the ideal resort (Hämäläinen et al., 1993). To date, only a handful of studies have used MEG to characterize feedback signal components in healthy individuals and compared them to EEG literature (Miltner et al., 2003; Keil et al., 2010; Talmi et al., 2012). MEG studies addressing psychological trauma have mainly focused on studying resting state (Huang et al., 2014; James et al., 2021, 2022), face-processing tasks (Badura-Brack et al., 2018) or working-memory tasks (McDermott et al., 2016). However, to the best of the authors’ knowledge, no MEG study has investigated feedback processing in the context of psychological trauma.

We hypothesize that traumatic experiences affect feedback processing, which will be reflected as differences in activity of the involved regions in the temporal and frequency domains. To test this, we examine the spatio-temporal and spectro-temporal facets of feedback processing in the context of trauma exposure. A feedback-based learning task was administered to two groups of individuals - a group with a history of trauma exposure and a control group without such history - during an MEG examination. As a difference in the cortical brain regions involved in the processing of positive and negative feedback was expected between the two groups, feedback processing differences in the temporal and spectral domains were further investigated. This is the first study to examine the potential changes in spatial, spectral, and temporal aspects of feedback processing following traumatic events.

2. Methods

2.1. Experimental paradigm

Participants were administered a probabilistic classification task that dissociates learning from positive and negative feedback (Herzallah et al., 2017). On each trial, one of four fractal images was presented to the participant, and they were asked to guess whether a fractal image (stimulus) predicts weather as “sun” or “rain.” The stimulus remained visible until the participant responded. A red line below the “sun” or “rain” image indicated the chosen answer for 700 milliseconds, followed by a 300 ms blank screen (Figure 1). Feedback was presented for a variable time of between 900 and 1,000 ms as either a green smiley face with the text “+25,” representing positive feedback; a red frowny face with the text “-25,” representing negative feedback; or a gray circle without text, representing no feedback. Two of the stimuli had a 90% probability of predicting “sun” and a 10% chance of predicting “rain.” The other two stimuli predicted “rain” with a 90% probability and “sun” with a 10% probability. For feedback type, two stimuli (positive-feedback stimuli) were associated with positive feedback when answered optimally and no feedback when answered non-optimally. The other two stimuli (negative-feedback stimuli) were associated with negative when answered non-optimally and no feedback when answered optimally. Task structure of the task is shown in Table 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm of the probabilistic task. The task paradigm for negative-feedback stimuli is illustrated in the upper portion. Participants received negative feedback when they provided an incorrect answer and no feedback when they gave a correct answer. The lower portion depicts the paradigm for positive-feedback stimuli, where participants received positive feedback for correct answers and no feedback for incorrect answers.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Structure of the probabilistic classification task.

The task consisted of three blocks, each containing 160 trials, and the participants needed an average duration of 24.7 ± 3.8 min to finish the task. Stimulus presentation was performed using the Python package PsychoPy 2.0 (Peirce et al., 2019). The visual stimuli were presented on an MEG-compatible screen using a Barco FL35 WUXGA projector with a resolution of 1980 × 1,200 pixels and a frequency of 60 Hz.

2.2. Participants

Seventy-nine participants between the ages of 21 and 43 were recruited using flyers, social media, and snowball sampling. All participants were interviewed by a trained researcher who administered the mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998). Using the MINI, the presence of trauma was determined, and participants were divided into two groups, the trauma-exposed group (Trauma) and the no-trauma-exposed control group (Control). The study was conducted according to the criteria of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the ethics committee of RWTH Aachen University Hospital, Germany. Following an explanation of the procedure, written informed consent was collected from every participant at the beginning of the session.

For inclusion in the Trauma group, participants were required to have had a traumatic event (criterion A) as defined by the MINI. The rest were assigned to the Control group. Using the MINI, comorbid psychiatric conditions were determined. The presence of psychiatric disorders, as defined by the MINI, was only accepted in the Trauma group. From the sample taken, 14 participants in the Trauma group met the criteria of either major depressive disorder, dysthymia, suicidality, social phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, or generalized anxiety disorder. The inclusion criteria for the Control group constituted the absence of any traumatic event and the absence of any psychiatric disorders at the time of testing, which was confirmed using the MINI. The exclusion criteria for all groups were the current use of psychotropic drugs, left-handedness, inability to understand the computer-based task, and major neurological or medical illnesses, including endocrine disorders. After the exclusion of three participants, the sample included 76 participants: 56 in the Trauma group and 20 in the Control group. Participant demographics are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographics of the tested sample.

To improve data accuracy and reduce noise, any trials with high signal amplitudes (above 5 picotesla) were excluded from the MEG analysis. As a result, one participant was excluded from the positive-feedback analysis, and two participants were excluded from the negative-feedback analysis. The final number of participants included in each of the analyses is shown in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Number of participants included in each of the analyses.

2.3. Behavioral analysis

In order to investigate whether trauma affects feedback processing and perception, learning accuracy and reaction time was analyzed for both positive and negative feedback. In order to avoid the multiple comparison problem, a one-way Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was initially performed with positive feedback accuracy, negative feedback accuracy, positive feedback reaction time, and negative feedback reaction time as dependent variables; and Group as the independent variable.

Due to time constraints for some participants and the tight schedule for testing, not all participants completed all 480 trials. Sixty-nine participants completed all 480 trials, one participant completed 416 trials, five participants completed 320 trials, and one participant only completed 160 trials. To compensate for the difference in the number of trials completed, behavioral analysis was performed once on the full data set for each participant and once on the first 160 trials only.

In order to obtain greater insight into the effect of trauma on feedback-based learning, the percentages of “learners” and “non-learners” were calculated for both positive- and negative-feedback trials. “Learners” of a particular feedback were defined as those who had more than 65% correct responses (Myers et al., 2013) for either of the feedback cards. The percentage of learners between the two groups was compared using the chi-square test.

2.4. MEG data acquisition

MEG data were collected using the Magnes-3600WH MEG system from 4d-Neuroimaging (San Diego, United States of America). Brain activity was measured with a sampling rate of 1017.25 Hz using 248 magnetometers. Additionally, cardiac and ocular activity were collected using the BrainAmp ExG MR amplifier (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany) with a sampling frequency of 5,000 Hz. This signal was down-sampled to 1017.25 Hz and merged with the MEG signal into a single recording. Electrooculography (EOG) signals were collected by attaching two electrodes lateral to the eyes and two electrodes above and below one of the eyes to record eye movements and eye blinks, respectively. Electrocarduigraphy (ECG) signals were collected by attaching an electrode at the middle of the right clavicle, one electrode at the left side below the apex of the heart, and a third electrode at the left leg. Finally, three head location coils were attached to the head to measure its relative position in space before and after each experiment.

2.5. MEG-MRI co-registration

To obtain anatomical information about the head and brain, MR measurements were taken from all participants at either a 3 T PRISMA scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using the MPRAGE sequence (Mugler and Brookeman, 1990) or at a 7 T MAGNETOM Terra scanner (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany) using the MP2RAGE sequence. As the participants of the Trauma group were also included in another study that used the 7 T MR system, images obtained at 7 T were used for this group to avoid further inconvenience. A sanity check was manually performed for each MEG file after the co-registration step to ensure that the use of the two MR systems did not affect the source localization of the signal. This was conducted by recording the mean error of co-registration and ensuring it was less than 3 mm.

MEG brain activity was aligned with the structural information by means of source (i.e., brain) space construction utilizing the FreeSurfer package (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999). For source localization, dynamic statistical parametric mapping (dSPM) was applied with a depth weighting of 0.8 (Dale et al., 2000). The individual source estimates were then projected onto the average template brain, as provided by FreeSurfer, using 8,196 vertices with an average distance between vertices of about 5 mm. Finally, the source activity was divided into anatomical regions based on the areas defined by the Desikan-Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., 2006). Co-registration from the MEG to the MRI coordinate space and the solving of the forward and inverse problems was performed using the MNE-Python library (Gramfort et al., 2014).

Three participants did not have an MR scan or had a corrupt file, so the average MR brain shape provided by Freesurfer (fsaverage template brain) was used instead. To ensure the quality of co-registration for all participants, including the three participants with missing files, the mean error of co-registration was recorded to be less than 3 mm using mne-coreg interface (Gramfort et al., 2013).

2.6. Preprocessing the MEG signal

Strong artifacts in MEG channels were identified using an in-house algorithm based on density-based spatial clustering of applications with noise (DBSCAN), as implemented in scikit- learn (Ester et al., 1996; Pedregosa et al., 2011). This was followed by visual inspection to identify noisy channels missed by this function. The signal of the identified “bad” channels was replaced by an interpolated signal from surrounding channels (Perrin et al., 1989). Environmental noise and powerline noise was removed by subtracting individually weighted reference channels from each of the MEG channels (Robinson, 1989).

Biological noise (i.e., ocular and cardiac signals) was removed using independent component analysis (ICA; Hyvärinen and Oja, 2000). The signal was split into segments with an average of 155.1 s per segment, then bandpass filtered (1–45 Hz) to improve the quality of the ICA decomposition (Winkler et al., 2015). Ocular activity was detected by finding components with a Pearson’s correlation of 0.3 or more with the EOG channel, while cardiac activity was detected using cross-trial phase statistics (CTPS; Dammers et al., 2008). A visual inspection of the results was finally performed to ensure all EOG and ECG signals were removed. The components were then projected back to the data. The average number of removed components was found to be 7.0 for all participants. Dividing the groups into Trauma and Control, the average number of removed components for the Trauma group is 7.1, while the average for the Control group is 6.5, with no significant difference between the two groups (p-value = 0.453).

2.7. Creating epochs

In order to analyze the data in the spatial, temporal and spectral domains, epochs were extracted around the event of interest (i.e., positive or negative feedback) starting 250 ms prior to the event and extending to 600 ms after the event, with the time 0 representing the feedback onset. The interval − 250 – 0 ms was chosen as a baseline. This means that, for each of the analyses, the mean of the signal in the baseline interval was subtracted from the entire signal of the epoch. The signal was then divided by the standard deviation of the baseline signal.

To ensure comparable signal-to-noise ratios between the two groups, the number of trials was equalized between the two groups by removing trials from the participants with larger trial counts until the density distribution of trials was equal. This was done by dividing the data into 20 bins, each containing one participant from the Control group and 2–3 participants from the Trauma group. Within each bin, the number of trials was equalized to match the participant with the lowest number of trials. For positive-feedback trials, the average number of trials became similar to those in the trauma group (Control: min = 24, max = 215, mean = 155; Trauma: min = 24, max = 215, mean = 154). The case was also similar for negative-feedback trials (Control: min = 15, max = 65 mean = 35 for both groups). The results of the density matching are illustrated in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Trial density matching for (A) positive-feedback and (B) negative-feedback trials. Trauma and Control groups were matched in trials in order to remove the bias that might result from the difference in trial count per group.

2.8. Regions of interest

Regions of interest were defined as regions that showed significantly-different activity in space and time between the two groups (Control vs. Trauma) in positive and negative-feedback trials separately. The Monte-Carlo-based non-parametric spatio-temporal cluster permutation test (SCPT) was performed to extract differences between the two groups (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). Since SCPT only performs a single statistical test on the entire data set, the multiple comparison problem does not apply. This means that differences across conditions directly reflect the significance level (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007).

SCPT was performed using a two-sample permutation t-test with 104 permutations. In order to choose the cluster threshold, the difference in trial numbers between the two conditions was taken into account. As the number of trials was much smaller for negative-feedback trials (35 vs. 154), the significant clusters were likely to have a shorter duration of activity. Therefore, the cluster threshold for positive-feedback trials was chosen to be equivalent to a t-value at an alpha level of 0.05 (corresponding to a threshold value of 1.99), while for negative-feedback trials, it was chosen to be equivalent to a t-value at an alpha level of 0.001 (corresponding to a threshold value of 3.43). The choice of cluster threshold does not affect the false alarm rate, but it does affect the sensitivity of the test (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). Finally, the significance threshold for both types of comparisons was set to 0.05.

To ensure that the resulting areas did not include outliers in space or time, clusters with 15 vertices or less in the template brain (corresponding to an active cortical area of about 2.29 cm2) were excluded. Similarly, temporally short clusters with a duration of less than 20 ms were discarded from the analysis. Finally, vertices within the medial wall of the brain were also excluded from the analysis. As previously stated, this test was conducted for both positive-feedback and negative-feedback trials separately. The resulting regions of interest were used to further conduct the time-course analysis and the spectro-temporal analysis.

2.9. Time-course analysis

In order to investigate possible changes in the temporal dynamics of feedback processing after trauma, positive feedback-related activity for Control vs. Trauma was compared on positive-feedback trials. For this comparison, a representative source time course (rSTC) was computed for each of the ROIs identified in the previous step using the mean activation of all “significant” vertices within the respective ROI. These representative time courses represent the average of epochs and vertices for a given ROI. Afterwards, all rSTCs were z-scored using the mean and standard deviation from the pre-stimulus interval.

To identify differences in the temporal dynamics between the groups, a cluster permutation test was applied in a similar way to the SCPT described above, but only in the temporal domain, so as to investigate differences between the ROI activation time courses (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). Cluster permutation tests were applied using a paired t-test with a clustering threshold using a critical alpha level of 0.05, which corresponds to a t-threshold of 1.99, and a significance level of p < 0.05. The number of permutations used was set to 104 permutations.

To study how the activity in brain regions affect learning, the two groups are divided into “learners,” with more than 65% accuracy, and “non-learners.” The signal within the significant time intervals (time intervals of interest; TOIs) of each rSTC was extracted for each subject to be compared between the two groups. This was conducted separately for Trauma and Control groups to control for trauma-related effects.

2.10. Spectro-temporal analysis

In addition to the statistical analysis in the temporal domain, differences between groups were also investigated in the time-frequency domain using a spectro-temporal cluster permutation test based on the previously identified ROIs. This test was conducted on the same principles as used in the SCPT, with the only change that clusters are built across time and frequency (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). For this analysis, power spectral density (PSD) was computed following positive and negative feedback was compared between the two groups for each region of interest using spectro-temporal cluster permutation test.

The complex Morlet wavelet transform was applied using a frequency range from 4 to 45 Hz with a spectral resolution of 1 Hz. The number of cycles for each frequency (f) was set to f/3. The results were z-scored using the mean and standard deviation of the 250 ms preceding feedback. For both types of comparisons (positive feedback and negative feedback), a paired t-test was used to identify clusters, with a clustering threshold using a critical alpha level of 0.05, corresponding to a t-threshold of 2.0, and a significance level of p < 0.05. The number of permutations used was set to 104 permutations.

Similar to the previous section, in order to study how time-frequency clusters affect learning, the “learners” and “non-learners” in our sample were compared on the average activity within each significant cluster (time-frequency cluster of interest; TFOI). This was done by extracting the PSD within the significant time-frequency clusters for each participant and averaging it over time and frequency for each group (i.e., “learners” vs. “non-learners”). This was performed separately for Trauma and Control groups to understand the signal’s contribution to learning accuracy in time and frequency.

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral analysis

In order to test whether trauma affects feedback-based learning, the two groups (Control vs. Trauma) were compared in terms of their learning accuracy and reaction time. This analysis was done on the complete data set from all subjects. First, MANOVA was used to test the effect of group (Trauma vs. Control) on positive feedback accuracy, negative feedback accuracy, positive feedback reaction time, and negative feedback reaction time. Using Wilks’ lambda, the results showed no significant effect of group on the dependent variables (F(4,71) = 0.8567, p = 0.4943), leading to the conclusion that there is no difference between the Trauma and Control groups in terms of accuracy or reaction time for positive or negative feedback types. Secondly, to avoid any ceiling effect due to the seven participants who completed less than 480 trials (see Methods section), the analysis was repeated using only the first 160 trials, and the results were found to be similar, with no significant effect for the group on any of the dependent variables (results not shown).

To confirm the findings, the ratio of learners to non-learners was compared in the two groups of comparison. In the tested sample, all participants, except for one, learnt the negative feedback cards more than with more than 65%. Thus, negative feedback is excluded from this analysis. As for positive feedback trials, no significant difference in the learners’ proportions was found between the two groups (χ2(1) = 0.0019, p-value = 0.9651). This further supports the notion that feedback-based learning is not affected in trauma survivors. Behavioral results are shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Feedback-learning accuracy and response time. Left: learning accuracy (left) and response time (right) for positive- and negative-feedback trials for Control vs. Trauma groups. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

To ensure the absence of confounding effects from other variables in subsequent analyses, the “learners” and “non-learners” groups were compared in terms of gender, age, education level and the 14 modules of the MINI interview. These include major depressive disorder, dysthymia, suicidality, mania, panic attacks, agoraphobia, social phobia, obsessive compulsive disorder, alcoholism, substance use, psychosis, anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and anxiety. There was no significant difference between the two groups in terms of the proportions of males and females (χ2 = 0.983, p-value = 0.3215), no significant difference in age (learners = 29.23 ± 6.44, non-learners = 32.23 ± 7.89, t = −1.6703, p-value = 0.0993) and years of education (learners = 17.05 ± 4.02, non-learners = 15.62 ± 4.47, t = 1.2895, p-value = 0.2018). As for the MINI modules, there were no significant differences between the two groups in any of the modules (smallest p-value = 0.2433).

3.2. ROI analysis

For positive feedback, results from the spatio-temporal cluster permutation test (SCPT) revealed two main clusters that showed significant differences between the two groups in space and time (Figure 4). The first cluster covers the right insula and part of the right supramarginal area as well as a small portion of the lateral part of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. The activity in this cluster was higher for the Control group (p-value = 0.0024). The second cluster covered a large portion of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex as well as the medial orbitofrontal cortex. The activity in this cluster was higher for the Trauma group (p-value = 0.0294). By projecting the clusters to brain regions defined by the Desikan-Killiany atlas and eliminating small regions with areas equal to or smaller than 2.29 cm2 and regions with short activity duration, four regions were assigned to be the positive-feedback ROIs: supramarginal, lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) and medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC). One cluster was identified in the medial portion of the superior frontal gyrus for negative feedback. This cluster had higher activity in the Trauma group (p-value of 0.022). The identified region is considered to be the negative-feedback ROI.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Regions of interest are areas that exhibit a significant difference between the Control group and the Trauma group during positive or negative-feedback trials. Vertices in red indicate higher activity in the Trauma group, while the blue color indicates higher activity in the Control group. Regions #1–4 pertain to positive-feedback clusters, while region #5 pertains to the negative-feedback cluster.

Table 4 details the location of each region and the percentage of the covered area according to the Desikan-Killiany atlas.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regions of interest.

3.3. Temporal dynamics of feedback-related activity

In order to gain more insight into the temporal aspect of the differences in signal processing, representative time courses were extracted from the five ROIs and compared between the two groups. The cluster-permutation test in the temporal domain revealed significant clusters in all five ROIs. For positive-feedback trials, the differences span the time interval 160–600 ms after feedback onset. The midway time point for positive-feedback trials was 390 ms (SD = 35 ms). The results of the temporal analysis showed higher activity for the Control group in the insula and the supramarginal cortices, while the Trauma group exhibited higher activity in the lOFC, mOFC, and the medial portion of the superior frontal cortex. This direction of differences is similar to that found using SCPT in the previous section, which confirms the findings reported above. Differences in the temporal dynamics between the two groups and the five brain areas are depicted in Figure 5. The results are also summarized in Table 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Temporal course of feedback processing of Trauma vs. Control groups. The figure illustrates brain activity in regions that showed significant differences in the time domain for (A) positive-feedback trials and (B) negative-feedback trials. The light blue shaded area represents the time interval with a significant difference. The gray shaded area around the timeseries represents the standard error of the mean.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of temporal analysis.

To understand the potential correlations between brain activity within the selected ROIs and learning from positive or negative feedback, the MEG activity within each of the identified time intervals (times of interest; TOIs) was extracted and was used to compare “learners” and “non-learners” in the Trauma and Control groups separately. For the Trauma group, when comparing “learners” and “non-learners,” the results indicate significant differenes in lateral orbitofrontal cortex in the time period 415–600 ms (t(53) = −2.664, mean learners = 0.08, mean non-learners = 0.827, p-value = 0.01) and the supramarginal cortex in the time period 315–525 ms (t(53) = 2.559, mean learners = −0.077, mean non-learners = −1.102, p-value = 0.013). Taking the absolute values, we find that the “learners” group have lower activity in both regions compared to the “non-learners” group. No significant differences were found for any of the regions in the Control group (p-values >0.18). The results are shown in Figure 6.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Comparison between “learners” and “non-learners” subgroups of the Trauma group. The figure shows the average source amplitude for the significant time periods found in Figure 5 for both “learners” and “non-learners.” For the supramarginal region, the time period tested was 315–525 ms, and for the lOFC, the period tested was 415–600 ms. The comparison shows significant differences in lOFC and supramarginal cortex. No significant differences were found between “learners” and “non-learners” of the Control group. * p < 0.05.

3.4. Time-frequency analysis

In order to investigate differences between subject groups in the time-frequency domain, the five regions of interest were included to test for differences between Trauma and Control groups for both positive and negative-feedback ROIs. Cluster permutation tests done in the spectro-temporal domain for positive-feedback trials revealed one significant cluster in the lateral temporal lobe that covers the theta band while spanning the interval of 185–555 ms following feedback presentation. Running the analysis on the negative-feedback ROIs did not yield any significant cluster. An illustration of the spectro-temporal test can be found in Figure 7.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Time-frequency representations of Controls vs. Trauma group comparison. The cluster permutation test revealed a significant cluster (highlighted in red) with higher power in theta (4–8 Hz) and alpha (8–10) bands in the Trauma group. The cluster was found in the right lateral orbitofrontal region in the time window between approximately 185–555 ms where time zero represents the feedback presentation time.

The average power within identified TFOIs was compared with positive-feedback scores in for “learners” and “non-learners” in both groups. Trauma group showed a significant difference between “learners” (36) and “non-learners” (19) (t(53) = −3.953, mean learners = 9.964, mean non-learners = 14.16, p-value <0.001). No significant difference was found in the Control group (p-value = 0.353). The results are shown in Figure 8.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Average power comparison between “learners” and “non-learners” subgroups of the Trauma group. The figure shows the average PSD within the significant time-frequency cluster found in Figure 7 for both the “learners” and “non-learners” groups. The time period tested was 185–555 ms in the frequency range (4–10 Hz). The results show a significantly higher power in the “non-learner” group. No significant differences were found for the healthy controls. *** p < 0.001.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to understand the effect of trauma on the ability to learn from feedback by utilizing MEG to compare brain activity in individuals with a history of trauma with a control group. No significant differences were found in behavioral performance measures, such as accuracy or reaction time, between the two groups. However, by analyzing the brain activity related to feedback processing, significant differences were discovered in the spatial, temporal, and frequency domains. Specifically, when compared to the control group, individuals with a history of trauma displayed increased brain activity in regions such as the mOFC and lOFC and decreased activity in the supramarginal and insular cortices during positive feedback presentation. For negative feedback, the Trauma group exhibited increased activity in the medial part of the superior frontal cortex. These differences occurred relatively late after the presentation of feedback and were characterized by activity within the theta and alpha frequency ranges.

To study the cognitive effects of trauma, learning accuracy was compared between the two groups. The results showed that both groups learned similarly from both positive and negative feedback and exhibited similar reaction times, leading to the conclusion that that response to feedback is not affected in the trauma-exposed group at the crude behavioral level (see Figure 3). Additionally, dividing the sample into learners and non-learners showed no significant difference between the two groups in the positive-feedback trials. On the other hand, almost all participants learnt negative-feedback trials with more than 65% accuracy. This is can be simply attributed to the design of the task, where negative-feedback trials are associated with negative feedback when answered incorrectly, while positive-feedback trials are associated with no feedback. Having no feedback can be also interpreted as absence of negative feedback in our study design, which makes positive-feedback trials more prone to mistakes in learning. This result was the same for both Trauma and Control groups.

These results are consistent with previous studies showing comparable learning and reaction times between trauma-exposed groups and controls (Vythilingam et al., 2009). Other studies have also shown equal learning between a PTSD group and a group of controls (Levy-Gigi et al., 2012; Boukezzi et al., 2020), which suggests that even when the effect of trauma is strong enough to cross the severity threshold for PTSD, it does not seem to affect learning from feedback significantly. Thus, one can conclude that traumatic experiences do not affect the behavioral processing of feedback associated with a stimulus. However, response to feedback may be affected by the trauma at the neural level.

To determine which brain areas respond differently to positive and negative feedback, brain activity was measured in both the Trauma and Control groups during feedback processing using MEG. SCPT was used to compare activity between the two groups while controlling for multiple comparisons. The analysis focused separately on trials when participants received positive or negative feedback. The results showed that there were four regions exhibiting distinct activity between the groups during positive-feedback trials, namely the right supramarginal gyrus, the insula, mOFC, and lOFC. However, during negative-feedback trials, only the medial part of the superior frontal cortex showed a difference in activity.

Previous reports have shown that the aforementioned ROIs are also involved in positive feedback-related processes (see Figure 4 and Table 4). For example, the mOFC and lOFC are involved in forming associations between the stimulus and the feedback (Rolls, 2004) and encoding values to stimuli (Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006; Purves et al., 2018), which is exhibited as higher activity when receiving positive feedback (Sescousse et al., 2013; Oldham et al., 2018; Nguyen et al., 2021). These areas receive input from various sensory regions that provide information about previous experiences to improve value estimation (Radcliffe Hospital et al., 2005; Purves et al., 2018). The insula is more generally activated when positive feedback is received (Wittmann et al., 2010) and is usually involved in the affective processing of positive stimuli and feedback (Sescousse et al., 2013; Nguyen et al., 2021). As part of a larger network, the insula sends information to the OFC to create a representation of the hedonic valence of stimuli (Craig, 2002; Purves et al., 2018; Nguyen et al., 2021). In turn, the supramarginal gyrus connects with the OFC, especially the lOFC (Du et al., 2020), and potentially modulates learning by increasing activity during the retrieval of information acquired through physical enactment (Russ et al., 2003). Accordingly, the supramarginal gyrus may be involved in the process of integrating new information with previous knowledge for future recall. In sum, the processing of positive feedback is a multidimensional process that engages a myriad of neural systems, and our results shed new light on the spatial facets of processing positive feedback with psychological trauma.

Conversely, in terms of the negative-feedback trials, the medial portion of the superior frontal cortex showed increased activity. In general, as a part of the brain’s performance-monitoring system, the superior frontal cortex is activated in response to errors in judgment and helps in avoiding further errors by influencing activity in other relevant brain areas and reducing distracting information (Danielmeier et al., 2011). In essence, one could argue that the increased activity seen in the superior frontal cortex might be the basis of the increased avoidance of negative encounters observed after exposure to trauma. However, in order for this to be proven, an examination of variability in the expression of avoidance symptoms in the Trauma group would be necessary.

The involvement of the insula and orbitofrontal cortices is also supported by the current neuroanatomical theory of PTSD, which indicates that trauma causes hypoactivity in the orbitofrontal cortex and medial prefrontal cortex in response to affective stimuli (Giustino and Maren, 2015; Stark et al., 2015). This could decrease the top-down regulation of amygdala activity and exacerbate symptoms of hyperarousal (Giustino and Maren, 2015; Stark et al., 2015). On the other hand, it has been reported that people who have experienced trauma display an increase in insular activity in response to emotional stimuli (Stein et al., 2007) and that both the insula and medial prefrontal cortex show a decrease in gray matter size following psychological trauma, even in the absence of PTSD (Ganzel et al., 2008).

Interestingly, in the current study, the Trauma group showed decreased activity in the insula but higher activity in the prefrontal, mOFC, and lOFC in response to positive feedback, which may initially seem counterintuitive. However, given that the sample consisted of trauma survivors with no or minimal PTSD symptoms at the time of testing, these results might reflect higher-than-average levels of appraisal of positive stimuli, which might be associated with a better response to trauma. This is further supported by previous studies, which found an increased valuation of positive stimuli in trauma survivors when compared to individuals with PTSD (Myers et al., 2013). This evidence suggests that trauma can cause long-lasting changes to cognition, including more positive feedback reappraisal, which might counter the effects of fear-related and avoidance symptoms. These differences are found at the neural activity level but do not seem to be prominent enough to affect crude measures of task performance at the behavioral level.

In order to further understand the differences in brain dynamics caused by trauma, activity in the regions of interest in the time domain were additionally analyzed (see Figure 5). Changes in processing positive feedback were evident between 165 and 600 ms, which covers two event-related potentials: the p300, and the late positive potential. The p300 is an event-related potential that is seen around 300 ms following feedback reported in EEG studies (Bernat et al., 2015) and usually correlates to secondary aspects of positive feedback, such as those requiring evaluation and comparison (Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Bernat et al., 2011, 2015). Conversely, the late positive potential starts at 300 ms, which is usually sustained until 2000 ms following positive feedback (Dennis and Hajcak, 2009), is linked to the selective processing of emotional stimuli and activation of emotional systems in response to positive stimuli (Cuthbert and Kozak, 2013). Thus, it is possible to conclude that the differences between the two groups are due to differences in the processing of secondary aspects of positive feedback as well as the emotional processing of positive stimuli. However, since our study design does not directly correlate the processing of secondary emotional aspects or emotional processing with the activity difference between the two groups, we suggest replicating our findings using experiments designed differently before definitively concluding such a correlation.

Analyzing the signal in the frequency domain provided more insight into feedback processing. Feedback processing consists of multiple components, each of which runs in a particular frequency band (Bernat et al., 2015). Using permutation-based clustering tests for the temporo-spectral domain, a cluster of differences was identified in the right lOFC (Figure 7). This cluster of activity spans the time interval of roughly 185–555 ms in the theta frequency band. Although the time interval should not be taken literally due to the nature of the permutation-based clustering test (Sassenhagen and Draschkow, 2019; Meyer et al., 2021), it should at least give an idea about the timing and frequency in which the significant differences were found.

Finally, the activity within each of the identified TOIs and TFOIs was compared between the “learners” and “non-learners” subgroups separately for the Trauma group and the Control group (Figure 6, Figure 8). This comparison was performed to determine whether the difference in learning is reflected in the activity within the TOIs and TFOIs in question. The results show that the activity within the TOIs was different between the two groups in the supramarginal (lower in the non-learners) and the lOFC (higher in the non-learners) regions. Similarly, the average power was significantly higher in the TFOI of the lOFC in the non-learner group. This result can be explained by one of two hypotheses. First, it is possible that the activity within these two regions is correlated with learning from positive feedback. Specifically, the activity in the lOFC region may have a negative impact on learning from positive feedback, while the activity within the supramarginal cortex region may have a positive impact on learning in the trauma group. Meanwhile, this effect is not noticeable on the behavioral level. Secondly, when examining the absolute values, the “learners” group have lower activity in the supramarginal and lOFC TOIs, as well as in the lOFC TFOI. This suggests that they have more attenuated activity in both regions. This can be due to the habituation effect, which refers to a decrease in response to the repeated presentation of a stimulus with emotional valence (Wright et al., 2001). Previous research supports these findings, where repeated presentation of an emotional stimulus resulted in lower brain activity in certain brain regions (Wright et al., 2001). Regardless of the explanation, these results confirm the correlation between brain activity in the lOFC and the supramarginal gyrus with receiving positive feedback. Furthermore, the “non-learners” subgroup seems to be the main contributor to the effects seen between Controls and Trauma in lOFC and supramarginal regions. However, since this learning effect is missing in the “non-learners” subgroup of the Controls, this suggests that the contribution of lOFC and supramarginal regions to learning is potentially perturbed following exposure to psychological trauma.

Taken together, no evidence to show that trauma-exposed individuals differ from individuals with no history of trauma was found at the behavioral level. However, differences between the groups were found at the neural level in the space, time, and frequency domains of cortical activity. These findings provide a deeper understanding of the cognitive processes that are affected as a result of trauma and demonstrate a novel framework for studying the underlying cognitive mechanisms that contribute to psychiatric symptoms by assessing the contribution of different domains of the brain signal on the targeted behavior. To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to combine the spatial, temporal, and spectral aspects of feedback processing in individuals with exposure to psychological trauma. It is anticipated that future work will build on these findings to focus on the potential of using cognitive and psychological constructs in assessing symptom improvement following trauma as part of individualized treatment plans.

A limitation of this study is that the impact of trauma was only assessed after exposure, which does not discount the presence of the reported differences before trauma exposure. To address this, future studies should employ a longitudinal design to identify any cognitive and neural differences that develop specifically as a result of exposure to trauma. Additionally, since the ROIs were determined using total signal and there is an inverse correlation between total signal and frequency, it is more likely that our ROIs will exhibit differences in low-frequency bands. This issue can be addressed in the future by selecting an alternative approach to identify the areas of interest.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee at the Medical Faculty of RWTH Aachen, Germany. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AS, CK, MH, and JD: conceptualization. AS and FB: data curation. AS: formal analysis, visualization, and writing – original draft preparation. NS and IN: funding acquisition. AS and FB: investigation. NS, IN, MH, and JD: project administration. NS, NK, and IN: resources. AS, CK, FB, MH, and JD: software. IN, MH, and JD: supervision. AS, MH, and JD: validation. MH and JD: writing – review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) under the Palestinian German Science Bridge and grant number 01DH16027. Open access publication was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – 491111487.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alvarez, P., and Squire, L. R. (1994). Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. Neurobiology 91, 7041–7045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.15.7041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013) ‘Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 2013 ’, Washington, DC: American psychiatric association

Google Scholar

Badura-Brack, A., McDermott, T. J., Heinrichs-Graham, E., Ryan, T. J., Khanna, M. M., Pine, D. S., et al. (2018). Veterans with PTSD demonstrate amygdala hyperactivity while viewing threatening faces: a MEG study. Biol. Psychol. 132, 228–232. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2018.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernat, E. M., Nelson, L. D., and Baskin-Sommers, A. R. (2015). Time-frequency theta and delta measures index separable components of feedback processing in a gambling task. Psychophysiology 52, 626–637. doi: 10.1111/PSYP.12390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernat, E. M., Nelson, L. D., Steele, V. R., Gehring, W. J., and Patrick, C. J. (2011). Externalizing psychopathology and gain/loss feedback in a simulated gambling task: dissociable components of brain response revealed by time-frequency analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 352–364. doi: 10.1037/A0022124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boukezzi, S., Baunez, C., Rousseau, P. F., Warrot, D., Silva, C., Guyon, V., et al. (2020). Posttraumatic stress disorder is associated with altered reward mechanisms during the anticipation and the outcome of monetary incentive cues. Neuroimage Clin. 25:102073. doi: 10.1016/J.NICL.2019.102073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brancu, M., Mann-Wrobel, M., Beckham, J. C., Wagner, H. R., Elliott, A., Robbins, A. T., et al. (2016). Subthreshold posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analytic review of DSM–IV prevalence and a proposed DSM–5 approach to measurement. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 8, 222–232. doi: 10.1037/tra0000078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuthbert, B. N., and Kozak, M. J. (2013). Constructing constructs for psychopathology: the NIMH research domain criteria. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 928–937. doi: 10.1037/a0034028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/NIMG.1998.0395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Liu, A. K., Fischl, B. R., Buckner, R. L., Belliveau, J. W., Lewine, J. D., et al. (2000). Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81138-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dammers, J., Schiek, M., Boers, F., Silex, C., Zvyagintsev, M., Pietrzyk, U., et al. (2008). Integration of amplitude and phase statistics for complete artifact removal in independent components of neuromagnetic recordings. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 55, 2353–2362. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2008.926677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danielmeier, C., Eichele, T., Forstmann, B. U., Tittgemeyer, M., and Ullsperger, M. (2011). Posterior medial frontal cortex activity predicts post-error adaptations in task-related visual and motor areas. J. Neurosci. 31, 1780–1789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4299-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, T. A., and Hajcak, G. (2009). The late positive potential: a neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 1373–1383. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02168.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

du, J., Rolls, E. T., Cheng, W., Li, Y., Gong, W., Qiu, J., et al. (2020). Functional connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Cortex 123, 185–199. doi: 10.1016/J.CORTEX.2019.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ester, M., Kriegel, Hans-Peter, Sander, Jörg, and Xu, Xiaowei, (1996) ‘A density-based algorithm for discovering clusters in large spatial databases with noise ’. AAAI Press. Washington, DC, U.S.

Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B. H., and Dale, A. M. (1999). High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 272–284. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::aid-hbm10>3.0.co;2-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, M. J., Resick, P. A., Bryant, R. A., and Brewin, C. R. (2011). Considering PTSD for DSM-5. Depress Anxiety 28, 750–769. doi: 10.1002/DA.20767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganzel, B. L., Kim, P., Glover, G. H., and Temple, E. (2008). Resilience after 9/11: multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain. Neuroimage 40, 788–795. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2007.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giustino, T. F., and Maren, S. (2015). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the conditioning and extinction of fear. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:298. doi: 10.3389/FNBEH.2015.00298/XML/NLM

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, G. H. (2011). Overview of functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am. 22, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/J.NEC.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gramfort, A., Luessi, M., Larson, E., Engemann, D. A., Strohmeier, D., Brodbeck, C., et al. (2014). MNE software for processing MEG and EEG data. Neuroimage 86, 446–460. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gramfort, A., Luessi, M., Larson, E., Engemann, D. A., Strohmeier, D., and Brodbeck, C. (2013). MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python. Front. Neurosci. 7:267. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greco, J. A., and Liberzon, I. (2016). Neuroimaging of fear-associated learning. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 320–334. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämäläinen, M., Hari, R., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Knuutila, J., and Lounasmaa, O. V. (1993). Magnetoencephalography—theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev. Mod. Phys. 65, 413–497. doi: 10.1103/RevModPhys.65.413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, J. L., van den Bos, W., Roeber, B. J., Rudolph, K. D., Davidson, R. J., and Pollak, S. D. (2017). Early adversity and learning: implications for typical and atypical behavioral development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 770–778. doi: 10.1111/JCPP.12694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herzallah, M. M., Khdour, H. Y., Taha, A. B., Elmashala, A. M., Mousa, H. N., Taha, M. B., et al. (2017). Depression reduces accuracy while parkinsonism slows response time for processing positive feedback in patients with Parkinson’s disease with comorbid major depressive disorder tested on a probabilistic category-learning task. Front. Psych. 8, 8:84. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopper, J. W., Pitman, R. K., Su, Z., Heyman, G. M., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., et al. (2008). Probing reward function in posttraumatic stress disorder: expectancy and satisfaction with monetary gains and losses. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 802–807. doi: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2007.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, M. X., Yurgil, K. A., Robb, A., Angeles, A., Diwakar, M., Risbrough, V. B., et al. (2014). Voxel-wise resting-state MEG source magnitude imaging study reveals neurocircuitry abnormality in active-duty service members and veterans with PTSD. Neuroimage Clin. 5, 408–419. doi: 10.1016/J.NICL.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyvärinen, A., and Oja, E. (2000). Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications. Neural Netw. 13, 411–430. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00026-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, L. M., Enghdal, B. E., Leuthold, A. C., and Georgopoulos, A. P. (2021). Classification of posttraumatic stress disorder and related outcomes in women veterans using magnetoencephalography. Exp. Brain Res. 6, 13–20. doi: 10.29245/2572.942X/2021/1.1279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, L. M., Leuthold, A. F., and Georgopoulos, A. P. (2022). MEG neural signature of sexual trauma in women veterans with PTSD. Exp. Brain Res. 240, 2135–2142. doi: 10.1007/S00221-022-06405-8/FIGURES/5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keil, J., Weisz, N., Paul-Jordanov, I., and Wienbruch, C. (2010). Localization of the magnetic equivalent of the ERN and induced oscillatory brain activity. Neuroimage 51, 404–411. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. I., Oh, S., Park, H., Min, B., and Kim, J. H. (2020). The prevalence and clinical impairment of subthreshold PTSD using DSM-5 criteria in a national sample of Korean firefighters. Depress Anxiety 37, 375–385. doi: 10.1002/DA.22998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy-Gigi, E., Kéri, S., Myers, C. E., Lencovsky, Z., Sharvit-Benbaji, H., Orr, S. P., et al. (2012). Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder show a selective deficit in generalization of associative learning. Neuropsychology 26, 758–767. doi: 10.1037/a0029361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, L., Gorka, S. M., Funkhouser, C. J., Shankman, S. A., and Phan, K. L. (2017). Impact of posttraumatic stress symptom dimensions on psychophysiological reactivity to threat and reward. J. Psychiatr. Res. 92, 55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lissek, S., and van Meurs, B. (2015). Learning models of PTSD: theoretical accounts and psychobiological evidence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 98, 594–605. doi: 10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, R. D., Olfson, M., Hellman, F., Blanco, C., Guardino, M., and Struening, E. L. (2001). Comorbidity, impairment, and suicidality in subthreshold PTSD. Am. J. Psychiatr. 158, 1467–1473. doi: 10.1176/APPI.AJP.158.9.1467/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/J617F1.JPEG

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, T. J., Badura-Brack, A. S., Becker, K. M., Ryan, T. J., Khanna, M. M., Heinrichs-Graham, E., et al. (2016). Male veterans with PTSD exhibit aberrant neural dynamics during working memory processing: an MEG study. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 41, 251–260. doi: 10.1503/JPN.150058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Megías, J. L., Ryan, E., Vaquero, J. M. M., and Frese, B. (2007). Comparisons of traumatic and positive memories in people with and without PTSD profile. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 117–130. doi: 10.1002/ACP.1282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, M., Lamers, D., Kayhan, E., Hunnius, S., and Oostenveld, R. (2021). Enhancing reproducibility in developmental EEG research: BIDS, cluster-based permutation tests, and effect sizes. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 52:101036. doi: 10.1016/J.DCN.2021.101036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miltner, W. H. R., Lemke, U., Weiss, T., Holroyd, C., Scheffers, M. K., and Coles, M. G. H. (2003). Implementation of error-processing in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a source analysis of the magnetic equivalent of the error-related negativity. Biol. Psychol. 64, 157–166. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00107-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan-López, A. A., Killeen, T. K., Saavedra, L. M., Hien, D. A., Fitzpatrick, S., Ruglass, L. M., et al. (2020). Crossover between diagnostic and empirical categorizations of full and subthreshold PTSD. J. Affect. Disord. 274, 832–840. doi: 10.1016/J.JAD.2020.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mugler, J. P., and Brookeman, J. R. (1990). Three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo imaging (3D MP RAGE). Magn. Reson. Med. 15, 152–157. doi: 10.1002/MRM.1910150117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, C. E., Moustafa, A. A., Sheynin, J., VanMeenen, K. M., Gilbertson, M. W., Orr, S. P., et al. (2013). Learning to obtain reward, but not avoid punishment, is affected by presence of PTSD symptoms in male veterans: empirical data and computational model. PLoS One 8:e72508. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0072508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, D., Naffziger, E. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2021). Positive affect: nature and brain bases of liking and wanting. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 39, 72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldham, S., Murawski, C., Fornito, A., Youssef, G., Yücel, M., and Lorenzetti, V. (2018). The anticipation and outcome phases of reward and loss processing: a neuroimaging meta-analysis of the monetary incentive delay task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 3398–3418. doi: 10.1002/HBM.24184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padoa-Schioppa, C., and Assad, J. A. (2006). Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441, 223–226. doi: 10.1038/nature04676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pechtel, P., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Disrupted reinforcement learning and maladaptive behavior in women with a history of childhood sexual abuse: a high-density event-related potential study. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 499–507. doi: 10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2013.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedregosa, F., Varoquaux, G., Gramfort, A., Michel, V., Thirion, B., and Grisel, O. (2011). Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 12, 2825–2830.

Google Scholar

Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., et al. (2019). PsychoPy2: experiments in behavior made easy. Behav. Res. Methods 51, 195–203. doi: 10.3758/S13428-018-01193-Y/FIGURES/3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., and Echallier, J. F. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 72, 184–187. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purves, D., Augustine, G. J., Fitzpatrick, D., Katz, L. C., and LaMantia, A.-S. (2018). “Thinking, planning, and deciding” in Neuroscience (New York: Sinauer Associates)

Google Scholar

Radcliffe Hospital, J., Way, H., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S. E. (1989). “Environmental Noise Cancellation for Biomagnetic Measurements” in Advances in Biomagnetism. ed. S. Williamson (Boston, MA, Springer)

Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 55, 11–29. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russ, M. O., Mack, W., Grama, C. R., Lanfermann, H., and Knopf, M. (2003). Enactment effect in memory: evidence concerning the function of the supramarginal gyrus. Exp. Brain Res. 149, 497–504. doi: 10.1007/S00221-003-1398-4/TABLES/1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sassenhagen, J., and Draschkow, D. (2019). Cluster-based permutation tests of MEG/EEG data do not establish significance of effect latency or location. Psychophysiology 56:e13335. doi: 10.1111/PSYP.13335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawyer, A. T., Liverant, G. I., Jun, J. J., Lee, D. J., Cohen, A. L., Dutra, S. J., et al. (2016). Punishment learning in U.S. veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Trauma. Stress. 29, 374–378. doi: 10.1002/JTS.22109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnurr, P. P., Ford, J. D., Friedman, M. J., Green, B. L., Dain, B. J., and Sengupta, A. (2000). Predictors and outcomes of posttraumatic stress disorder in world war II veterans exposed to mustard gas. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 258–268. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.2.258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sescousse, G., Caldú, X., Segura, B., and Dreher, J. C. (2013). Processing of primary and secondary rewards: a quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 681–696. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., and Weiller, E. (1998). The Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59:11980.

Google Scholar

Sripada, R. K., King, A. P., Welsh, R. C., Garfinkel, S. N., Wang, X., Sripada, C. S., et al. (2012). Neural dysregulation in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence for disrupted equilibrium between salience and default mode brain networks. Psychosom. Med. 74, 904–911. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0B013E318273BF33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stark, E. A., Parsons, C. E., van Hartevelt, T. J., Charquero-Ballester, M., McManners, H., Ehlers, A., et al. (2015). Post-traumatic stress influences the brain even in the absence of symptoms: a systematic, quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 56, 207–221. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2015.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, M. B., Simmons, A. N. Ph.D., Feinstein, J. S. B.S., and Paulus, M. P. M.D. (2007). Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am. J. Psychiatr. 164, 318–327. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talmi, D., Fuentemilla, L., Litvak, V., Duzel, E., and Dolan, R. J. (2012). An MEG signature corresponding to an axiomatic model of reward prediction error. Neuroimage 59, 635–645. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.06.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vythilingam, M., Nelson, E. E., Scaramozza, M., Waldeck, T., Hazlett, G., Southwick, S. M., et al. (2009). Reward circuitry in resilience to severe trauma: an fMRI investigation of resilient special forces soldiers. Psychiatry Res. 172, 75–77. doi: 10.1016/J.PSCYCHRESNS.2008.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, I., Debener, S., Müller, K.-R., and Tangermann, M. (2015). On the influence of high-pass filtering on ICA-based artifact reduction in EEG-ERP. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2015, 4101–4105. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7319296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittmann, M., Lovero, K. L., Lane, S. D., and Paulus, M. P. (2010). Now or later? Striatum and insula activation to immediate versus delayed rewards. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 3, 15–26. doi: 10.1037/A0017252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, C. I., Fischer, H., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., and Rauch, S. L. (2001). Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Neuroreport 12, 379–383. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, N., and Sanfey, A. G. (2004). Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 24, 6258–6264. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4537-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zlotnick, C., Franklin, C. L., and Zimmerman, M. (2002). Does “subthreshold” posttraumatic stress disorder have any clinical relevance? Compr. Psychiatry 43, 413–419. doi: 10.1053/COMP.2002.35900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: trauma, PTSD, magnetoencephalography, feedback-based learning, spatio-temporal cluster permutation test

Citation: Sawalma AS, Kiefer CM, Boers F, Shah NJ, Khudeish N, Neuner I, Herzallah MM and Dammers J (2023) The effects of trauma on feedback processing: an MEG study. Front. Neurosci. 17:1172549. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1172549

Received: 23 February 2023; Accepted: 17 October 2023;
Published: 02 November 2023.

Edited by:

Tetsuo Kida, Institute for Developmental Research, Japan

Reviewed by:

Silvia Isabella, San Camillo Hospital (IRCCS), Italy
Samu Taulu, University of Washington, United States

Copyright © 2023 Sawalma, Kiefer, Boers, Shah, Khudeish, Neuner, Herzallah and Dammers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jürgen Dammers, j.dammers@fz-juelich.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.