Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 15 April 2021
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare
Volume 8 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.639187

Caloric Restriction in Group-Housed Mice: Littermate and Sex Influence on Behavioral and Hormonal Data

  • 1Center of Scientific Instrumentation, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychobiology, Institute of Neurosciences, Center for Biomedical Research (CIBM), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 3Department of Pharmacology, Faculty of Medicine, Biomedical Research Center, Institute of Neuroscience, University of Granada, Parque Tecnológico de Ciencias de la Salud, Granada, Spain
  • 4Biosanitary Research Institute ibs.GRANADA, Granada, Spain
  • 5Department of Psychobiology, Faculty of Psychology, Center of Investigation of Mind, Brain, and Behavior, University of Granada, Granada, Spain

Much of the research done on aging, oxidative stress, anxiety, and cognitive and social behavior in rodents has focused on caloric restriction (CR). This often involves several days of single housing, which can cause numerous logistical problems, as well as cognitive and social dysfunctions. Previous results in our laboratory showed the viability of long-term CR in grouped rats. Our research has studied the possibility of CR in grouped female and male littermates and unrelated CB6F1/J (C57BL/6J × BALBc/J hybrid strain) mice, measuring: (i) possible differences in body mass proportions between mice in ad libitum and CR conditions (at 70% of ad libitum), (ii) aggressive behavior, using the number of pushes and chasing behavior time as an indicator and social behavior using the time under the feeder as indicator, and (iii) difference in serum adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) concentrations (stress biomarker), under ad libitum and CR conditions. Results showed the impossibility of implementing CR in unrelated male mice. In all other groups, CR was possible, with a less aggressive behavior (measured only with the number of pushes) observed in the unrelated female mice under CR conditions. In that sense, the ACTH levels measured on the last day of CR showed no difference in stress levels. These results indicate that implementantion of long-term CR in mice can be optimized technically and also related to their well-being by grouping animals, in particular, related mice.

Introduction

Caloric restriction (CR) has been widely used in experimental research (1, 2). CR has been used in different modalities (moderate, 70–80% and intense restriction, 50–60%), with respect to the maximum ad libitum (AL) intake. It is proposed as a maintenance method between 6 and 24 months or more (3). Overfeeding is considered one of the most uncontrolled variables in bioassays in general (4). The most commonly used in chronic (24 months) and/or subchronic (12 months) evaluation studies is the moderate (70–80%) CR procedure (47). Traditionally, CR research has focused on how it influences increased longevity (2, 8, 9). CR has also been studied in relation to oxidative stress, where it was found to have an antioxidant effect (10), or in relation to the reduction of inflammatory processes induced by aging and measured in microglia levels (11). Another context in which CR has been studied is anxiety. Thus, CR has proven to have an anxiolytic effect, tested in the open field and in elevated plus maze (12, 13) and it enhances fear extinction learning (14), but has no effect on post-traumatic stress disorder (15). CR research also studies cognitive functions and social behavior. CR has been observed to have negative effects on cognitive functions, probably caused by lower glucose levels (16), and on maternal care, inducing a decline in maternal behavior toward pups (17). However, CR also has positive effects, such as heightened social behavior between mice (18).

Typically, CR experiments require extended periods of time. Body mass control and avoiding potentially aggressive behavior (1921) can force researchers to use single housing for animals. Especially if the aggressive mice behavior is considered (22). Single housing can cause many logistical issues (it requires more cages and racks, space and maintenance staff, etc.) as well as problems related to the well-being of animals (stress induction) (23). Regarding the logistical problems, current legislation [for a review see (24)] limits research installations and resources, discouraging individual maintenance of animals for extended periods of time. Single housing also impacts negatively on the animal's well-being [for a review see (25)], which could be a potential confounding variable in future protocols applied to animals in single housing. It is important to note that mice display complex social behavior such as empathy [for a review see (26, 27)], and the social deprivation associated with single housing in CR research may therefore have severe effects on the animal's behavior, by denying the animal access–for example–to the various benefits of social interaction. In this sense, social interaction has been shown to improve memory processes, reduce hippocampal damage in aged mice (28), induce brain-derived neurotrophic factor (29), reduce the impact of CR (30), induce higher food consumption (31), reduce anorexic behavior in adolescent mice (32) and facilitate cognitive recovery after a social defeat experience (33). All these aspects show the potential benefits of carrying out abundant research into CR with grouped animals. The benefits of grouping animals are not only logistical and economic, they are also very much related to their well-being, as described above.

Previous research in our laboratory showed the viability of group-housing while sustaining CR for long periods in male rats (34). Our results indicated the effectiveness of CR in different groups, regardless of the relationship between the rats. No extreme body mass changes were observed in CR rats, nor did they display aggressive behavior or show alterations in their corticosterone levels. To our knowledge, no similar data has been reported about the possibility of group-housing mice under CR. We decided to study CR in CB6F1/J mice for two reasons: it is a inbred strain often used in experimental research (35) and has a tendency to show aggressive behavior under grouped conditions (22). Our main objective was to determine how CR at 70% of ad libitum affects the body mass, relationships and behavior of littermates and unrelated (male or female) grouped mice, as well as to study as an indicator of stress, in relation to animal welfare (36, 37) by analyzing serum adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) levels. It is expected that against aggressive behavior in C57BL/6 mice (22), a normal interaction with absence of significant aggressive behaviors will be observed in the CR at 70% mice. Likewise, we expected to find no significant differences in serum ACTH levels between the groups.

Materials and Methods

Ethical Approval and Other Ethical Considerations

Animals were kept in accordance with EU Directive 2010/63/EU and Spanish Royal Decree 53/2013. The University of Granada's Research Ethics Committee and the Junta de Andalucía, Consejería de Agricultura, Ganadería, Pesca y Desarrollo Sostenible approved the experimental protocol with reference number 09/08/2019/137. All animal procedures carried out for this study were subject to review by Animal-Welfare Officials and a designated veterinarian of the Animal Facility at CIBM/UGR.

Animals

The experiment used fifty-four CB6F1/J (#100007; F1 generation hybrids from breeding of BALB/cJ females and C57BL/6J males from Jackson Laboratories) mice (27 female) from the Biomedical Investigation Center (CIBM) of Granada. Mice were divided into groups of three and kept in transparent methacrylate cages (215 × 465 × 145 mm) in rooms at 22 ± 1°C and with a 12-h light/darkness cycle (lights on at 7:30 AM). Standard Type-II Tecniplast LTD 370 cm2 cuvettes -allowing a maximum of 5 mice- were used for the maintenance of mice in the experimental phase, with pine wood shavings from Rettenmair Ltd, and enrichment elements (pieces of paper). This is the usual size used in the experimental maintenance of chronic and subchronic mice at the SPEA/IC/CI/CIBM facilities. The experimental subjects came from a 10 monogamous breeding pairs, in which the offspring were separated into groups of three after weaning in separate cages into males and females of the same litter, with ad libitum feeding for the littermate groups. For the unrelated groups, males and females were randomly selected from the cages of unrelated individuals. At the beginning of the experiment, the average body mass was 20.9 ± 1.13 g for females and 27.2 ± 1.53 g for males. Throughout the 23 days of the experiment, water was accessible ad libitum and a standard laboratory pellet diet (Harlan Teklad Research diet, Madison, WI, USA) was administered as described in the “Method” section below.

Method

Cages with CR and ad libitum groups had the same body mass proportions and housekeeping conditions. To control the effectiveness of the restriction process, 18 unrelated mice (Group ad libitum; nine females and nine males housed in groups of three mice per cage) were designated as control groups. Since there was an absence of interaction during feeding in these unrelated controls groups, we consider not necessary to include another 18 littermate mice control group. Thirty-six mice (18 females and 18 males) were exposed to 70% food restriction. Each cage held a group of three mice, and they were distributed into four groups. In two groups, 18 unrelated mice (Group Restricted unrelated; nine females and nine males) were subjected to 70% food restriction, and 70% food restriction was also introduced in the other two groups of nine littermates mice (Group Restricted-Related; nine females and nine males). The tails of mice from each cage were marked in different colors (red, blue or no mark) to identify them.

Recording Body Mass and Observing Behavior

Every day at 1:00 p.m., each mouse was weighed on a scale and food was administered. The ad libitum group was given 200 g of food, and groups on food restriction were given 70% of the food eaten each day by the ad libitum group of mice (The uneaten food from the ad libitum group was weighed). The remaining food (pellets) was removed before the CR groups had access to their food, to ensure there was a 70% food reduction. The order in which food was administered was rotated each day, thus producing parity between groups with regard to the time that mice had to wait for food (and the resulting added stress). After calculating the mean and standard deviation from the recorded body mass the body mass proportion between the three animals in each cage was calculated by considering the weight of the heaviest cage-mate mouse as 100% and applying the following equation:

% Body Mass=mouse weight (g) * 100heaviest mouse weight(g)

The greater differences between mice weight, the lower the average % Body mass per cage.

Animals were recorded in their respective cages for 15 min every day, using a digital JVC camera model Everio HDD GZ-MG680BE, immediately after making the food available to both groups under CR. Of that time, the first 5 min were used to analyze behavior. At the end of the experimental procedure, a global analysis of social behavior was performed with the recorded material by using Behavioral Observation Research Interactive Software (BORIS) (38). Based on data obtained with rats in past research (34), the behavioral analysis focused on the number of times each mouse pushed its cage companions while eating. This push action can be compared to wrestling behavior observed in other studies (39, 40); a form of defense from other mice, using the front or back paws to indicate fighting behavior, or an attempt to force the mouse who is receiving the push to submit (submission response) (41). However, our previous results with rats (34) showed that pushes, under CR conditions, can be interpreted as social behavior. Also, potentially aggressive behavior (26) was recorded during the 15 min after food was made available to CR mice. Whenever such behavior was observed, the cage was eliminated from the experiment and CR was suppressed to prevent physical injuries.

In addition to the measurement of pushes, two additional behavioral from the total of 15 min recorded were analyzed. On the one hand, and within the aggressive behaviors, there was the recording of chasing behavior, understood as the time in which at least two mice chased each other to get a piece of food. On the other hand, and as part of the recording of more social behaviors, we measured the time in which the three animals were eating at the same time under the feeder without the presence of pushing and shoving. There were no other significant behavior to analyze.

Hormonal Analysis

On the 23rd day of the experiment, after being deeply anesthetized with isoflurane, fifty microliters of blood were extracted intracardially from the 44 mice (10 samples were not collected due to the elimination of the Unrelated Male CR (9 samples) and the insufficient volume of blood for one female from the Unrelated CR group). To acclimatize mice to the procedure, they were previously exposed to the isoflurane box on two occasions. After the blood draw, animals were sacrificed with a cervical dislocation. Of the 44 mice, 18 were unrelated mice with food ad libitum (nine females and nine males), and another eight were unrelated, female mice on 70% food restriction. The remaining 18 were littermates on 70% food restriction (nine females and nine males). Serum was obtained from these blood samples for the hormonal analysis. Hormonal analyses were performed using the Milliplex map pituitary magnetic bead panel kit (MPTMAG-49K-01) for ACTH and the Luminex 200TM HTS, FLEXMAP 3D. Preparation of serum samples was performed as follows: blood was allowed to clot for at least 30 min before centrifugation, for 10 min at 1,000 x g. Serum was removed and assayed immediately. For each serum sample, 150 μl of the antibody-bead and assay buffer were added to the mixing bottle, resulting in a total volume of 2,850 μl. Next, samples were incubated overnight on a shaker at 4°C. Samples were then measured on the Lumina 200TM. Median Fluorescent Intensity (MFI) was recorded using a weighted 5-parametrer logistic or spline curve-fitting method to analyze concentrations in samples. The validation of the measurements made on the hematology counter was performed with a commercial artificial blood. Specifically, Myt-5D Hematology controls (normal control) from ORPHEE SA (CH-1228 Geneva/Pla-les-Quates SWITZERLAND) were used.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using JASP version 0.10.2. Behavioral data were analyzed considering the cage-mate mice as non-independents. Rest of the analyses were done considering each mouse as independent. Body mass and body mass proportion were compared using repeated measures (RM) Analyses of Variance (ANOVA) and Chasing behavior and Time under the feeder were analyzed by one-way ANOVA since the normality assumptions (sphericity and the equality of variances) were respected. Kruskal-Wallis non-parametric analysis was applied for ACTH levels and Total number of Pushes since the criteria of normality were not assumed in these cases. Whenever a significant difference between groups was found, the Bonferroni correction was applied in a post-hoc derived from the main analysis. The significance level in all cases was p < 0.05.

Results

On the second day of CR, intensely aggressive behavior (26) was observed in two cages (six mice) of the group Restricted unrelated males. There was fierce fighting between animals, with blood and several skin injuries. This forced us to apply the ethical protocol and, as was mentioned above under Method, the nine animals of the group Restricted unrelated males were discarded from the experiment and CR was interrupted to avoid further fighting and possible injuries to mice.

Body Mass and Body Mass Proportion

Figure 1 shows body masses for the five groups of mice (ad libitum unrelated male and female, Restricted unrelated female, and Restricted related male and female) throughout the 23 days of the experiment (for statistical analyses, first day was treated as a covariate factor). Application of RM-ANOVA here confirms the sphericity and equality of variances (Levene Fs < 2.03, df = 42; p > 0.09). Results showed a significant main effect of Group (ANOVA Between Subjects Effect: F = 131.517, df = 5; p = 0.001; η2 = 0.79) and the interaction between Group and Day (ANOVA Within Subjects Effect: F = 15.171, df = 105; p = 0.001; η2 = 0.078). However, the variable Day did not exert significant main effect (ANOVA Within Subjects Effect: F = 1.553, df = 21; p = 0.054; η2 Magnitude of the effect = 0.002). Analysis of the interaction (keeping the first day as a covariate factor) shows that ad libitum Females have a significant higher body mass than Restricted related Female since D3 to D23 (Bonferroni p = 0.001), Restricted unrelated Female since D4 to D23 (Bonferroni p = 0.001) and Restricted related Male D4 (Bonferroni p = 0.001), D5 (Bonferroni p = 0.002) and since D6 to D23 (Bonferroni p = 0.001). Ad libitum Males have a significant higher body mass than Restricted related Female D3 (Bonferroni p = 0.003), D5 (Bonferroni p = 0.002), and since D6 to D23 (Bonferroni p = 0.001) except D15 (Bonferroni p = 0.079). Ad libitum Males have a significant higher body mass than Restricted unrelated Female and Restricted related Male D3 (Bonferroni p = 0.011), and since D5 to D23 (Bonferroni p = 0.001). Restricted related Male have a significant less body mass than Restricted related Female Days D15 (Bonferroni p = 0.04), D17 (Bonferroni p = 0.02) and D21 (Bonferroni p = 0.006). The rest of the days no differences between these groups were significant. Restricted related Male have a significant less body mass than Restricted unrelated Female only days D19 (Bonferroni p = 0.048) and D21 (Bonferroni p = 0.005). Finally, Restricted related Female and Restricted unrelated Female did not have any significant difference in their body mass. This result shows the effectiveness of CR in male and female mice.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean (±SEM) body mass (g) throughout the 23 days (D) in ad libitum unrelated female and male (Adl.Unrel.), restricted unrelated female (Res. Unrel.) and restricted related female and male (Res.Rel) mice. Results showed the significant main effect of the variable group and the interaction between group and day. However, the variable day did not exert a significant main effect. Analysis of the interaction (keeping the first day as a covariate factor) shows that ad libitum Females have a significant higher body mass than restricted related female (days 3–23), restricted unrelated female and restricted related male (days 4–23). Ad libitum males have a significant higher body mass than restricted related female (days 3, 5, 6–14, 16–23). Ad libitum males have a significant higher body mass than restricted unrelated female and restricted related male (days 3, 5–23). Restricted related male have a significant less body mass than restricted related female (day 15, 17, 21). Restricted related male have a significant less body mass than restricted unrelated female (days 19, 21).

Regarding body mass proportions, after first checked the sphericity and equality of variances (Levene D1-D23 Fs < 2.178; p > 0.08), RM ANOVA showed that Group (ANOVA Between groups effect: F = 0.315, df = 5; p = 0.832; η2 = 0.02) and Day (ANOVA Within groups effect: F = 0.315; df = 22; p = 0.401; η2 = 0.004) variables had no effect, and nor did their interaction (F = 0.286; df = 110; p > 0.8; η2 = 0.019). This means that body mass proportions were similar throughout the days of the experiment.

Pushes

Figure 2 shows the number of pushes under the feeder for Restricted unrelated and Restricted related mice. Due to the total absence of activity under the feeder when food was administered, the ad libitum groups were eliminated for analysis. A one-way ANOVA of the mean of the sum of pushes for each group (Unrelated female, Related female and Related males) throughout the days of the experiment showed a violation of the equality of variances (Levene F = 6.557; p < 0.04). The Kruskal-Wallis analysis showed significant differences between groups (K-W = 6.489, df = 5; p < 0.04). Comparing groups while applying the Kruskal-Wallis analysis shows a higher number of pushes in the group of unrelated females than in Related females and Related males (K-W = 3.857, df = 2; p = 0.05). No differences were found in the number of pushes between Related females and Related males (K-W = 2.33, df = 2; p = 0.12). Subsequent analysis of the cumulative frequencies of the pushes over the days showed for Restricted unrelated females (p = 0.029) and Restricted related males (p = 0.005) a significant linear component was observed (no stabilization of the number of pushes over the days). Quadratic or cubic component was not significant (p > 0.09). However, this linear, quadratic or cubic component was not significant for the group of sister females Restricted related females (p > 0.06).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean (±SEM) of total number of pushes during the 23 days in restricted unrelated female (Res.Unrel) and restricted related female and male (Res Rel) mice. Restricted unrelated females showed a higher number of pushes than restricted related females and males as indicated by (*) sign.

Chasing Behavior and Time Under the Feeder

Analysis of the total chasing behavior through the 23 days was done for Restricted unrelated and Restricted related mice after the absence of activity in the ad libitum groups. Application of ANOVA here confirms the sphericity and equality of variances (Levene F = 0.703, df = 2; p = 0.532). Analysis showed no significant effect of Group (One Way ANOVA: F = 0.290, df = 2; p = 0.748. η2 = 0.088). Similar, for the time under the feeder was done for Restricted unrelated and Restricted related mice. Due to the total absence of activity under the feeder when food was administered, the ad libitum groups were eliminated for analysis. Application of ANOVA here confirms the sphericity and equality of variances (Levene F = 1.697, df = 2; p = 0.261). Analysis showed no significant effect of group (One Way ANOVA: F = 1.395, df = 2; p = 0.318. η2 = 0.318).

Adrenocorticotropic Hormone Levels

Figure 3 shows the adrenocorticotropic hormone values obtained for the five groups. A one-way ANOVA revealed the violation of the equality of variances (Levene F = 3.329; p < 0.03). Kruskal-Wallis analysis shows the absence of significant differences between groups (K-W = 0.721, df = 5; p > 0.9). Additional analysis of the magnitude of the effect using the Cohen's d shows values between the groups negative or under 0.2 (lower effect). Only the differences between the restricted related female with the restricted related male and restricted no-related female had a Cohen's d of 0.242 and 0.221, respectively. This means that the CR procedure produced no significant increases in adrenocorticotropic hormone levels in any group.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean (±SEM) of adrenocorticotropic hormone concentration (pg/mL) in ad libitum unrelated female and male (Adl.Unrel), restricted unrelated female (Res Unrel) and restricted related female and male (Res.Rel) mice collected on the 23rd day of the experiment. No significant differences between groups are observed.

Discussion

The first result that requires comment is the impossibility of applying CR in unrelated male mice, contrary to the lack of aggressive behavior observed in unrelated rats under CR conditions (34). This data supports the greater aggressiveness previously observed in mice (42), and particularly in BALB/C mice (22, 43, 44). This greater aggressiveness in males toward other males (45) made CR impossible among unrelated males. However, in littermates, and in unrelated female mice the aggressiveness does not appear and neither does clear a type of considered cooperative social behavior such as the time under feeder of all the animals at the same time. This possible absence of social behavior (measured in our experimental conditions) can also be interpreted as a sign of less complex empathy in mice. Although mice display empathy (27) and social behavior (46), it seems that under CR conditions this social and cooperative behavior does not appear even though the mice lived together after weaning for 7–8 weeks. Groups of littermates showed lower levels of aggressiveness, confirming previous observations in mice (47) and this was demonstrated in the non-aggressive behavior observed under CR conditions in male and female mice. In the case of unrelated females, the correct development of CR conditions was facilitated by levels of aggressiveness that were lower than usual among non-pregnant females (45). However, an analysis of the pushes, shows a higher total number of pushes in unrelated female mice under CR conditions than in related female and male mice. This was an unexpected result. In previous research, aggressiveness implies another kind of behaviors more violent (attacks, bites or squeaks). In our experiment, this kind of behavior was only showed in the restricted unrelated mice, but was not observed in the rest of the restricted groups. Only chasing behavior was observed and the differences between groups were no significant between restricted unrelated female and the restricted female and male littermates. Differences were observed only in pushing. The pushes observed in unrelated female mice did not imply aggressive behavior, causing (sometimes) other mice to shuffle or fall. Perhaps this low aggressive behavior is explained considering that it was observed in females. Another possible explanation for this behavior could be the 7–8 weeks that unrelated female mice were housing together. This previous cohabitation and the grouping right after weaning might perhaps mitigate this aggressive response that have been observed in restricted unrelated males (47). These data open the possibility to considerer other variables such as time of cohabitation apart and not only the strain or the characteristics of grouping (22, 35). Likewise, consideration of the possible influence of environmental changes on the induction of aggressive behaviors (e.g., as the observed in transportation to research facilities) or housing conditions (42) should be noted.

These positive effects have been observed in the adrenocorticotropic hormone analysis. An absence of significant differences between groups, which could be interpreted as an indirect measure of the absence of alterations in stress levels (36, 37, 48). It is also true that samples were taken at the end of 23 days under CR conditions, and these levels could therefore actually be associated with other biomarkers. In this respect, the possible role played by orexin has been studied as a neuropeptide that might connect prolonged food restriction periods, aggressiveness and social behavior (46, 4951). The long time period between the CR and the adrenocorticotropic measure has perhaps have produced an adaptation as probably other biomarkers such as feeding times, usually done in the dark period [for a review see (52)]. It might be interesting to further investigate in this area, to clarify not only the aggressive response associated to CR in unrelated mice, but also changes in biomarkers. However, in our study the objective was just to evaluate the viability of applying long CR in mice despite its inherent aggressiveness (22).

Lastly, and regarding the effectiveness of CR, while there was a 21% reduction in body mass in related males under CR relative to ad libitum males, which is consistent with previous results in rats (34, 53, 54), body mass reduction associated with CR in females was lower (13%). This result can be attributed to differences observed between female and male metabolism (55, 56).

Conclusions

Our results have important implications, particularly in relation to the difficulties attached to long-term CR in unrelated male mice. In that context, and based on the results observed in terms of aggressiveness, the best option when implementing CR in mice would be to group-house littermates.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by The University of Granada's Research Ethics Committee and the Junta de Andalucía, Consejería de Agricultura, Ganadería, Pesca y Desarrollo Sostenible approved the experimental protocol with reference number 09/08/2019/137.

Author Contributions

CP, IM, JZ, and CC: study concept and design. CP and IM: acquisition of data. CP, LR-L, AV-Á, and IM: analysis and interpretation of data and statistical analysis. IM: drafting of the manuscript. CP, LR-L, AV-Á, IM, JZ, and CC: critical revision of the article for important intellectual content. JZ and CC: obtained funding. All authors had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Funding

This study was partially funded by the Master's program in Basic and Applied Neuroscience and Pain, University of Granada, the Junta de Andalucía (grants CTS109 and B-CTS-422-UGR18) and the Spanish Ministry of Health (National Drug Plan grant 2020I049). AV-Á was recipient of a predoctoral fellowship (Grant/Award number: FPU18/05012) of the Ministerio de Universidades, Spain. LR-L has a research contract under the grant B-CTS-422-UGR18 (Programa Operativo FEDER Andalucía 2014-2020).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the technical assistance of Ms. Ana Fernández Ibáñez, who helped with the ACTH hormone analysis, and of Mr. Daniel González Fernández, who edited the final English version of this document.

References

1. Claassen V. Neglect factors in pharmacology and neuroscience research: biopharmaceutics, animal characteristics, maintenance, testing conditions. In: Houston SP, editor. Techniques in the Behavioural and Neural Sciences, Vol 12. Amsterdam: Elsevier (1994).

Google Scholar

2. Speakman JR, Mitchell SE. Caloric restriction. Mol Asp Med. (2011) 32:159–221. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2011.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bertrand HA, Herlihy JT, Ikeno Y, Yu PB. Diatery restriction. Yu BP, editor. In: Methods in Aging Research. CRC Press. Boca Raton (1999). p. 271–300.

4. Keenan KP, Ballan GC, Dixit R, Soper KA, Laroque P, Mattson BA, et al. The effects of diet, overfeeding and moderate dietary restriction on Sprague-Dawley rat survival, disease and toxicology. J. Nutr. (1997) 127:851S−6S. doi: 10.1093/jn/127.5.851S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Fernandes G, Venkatraman JT, Turturro A, Attwood VG, Hart RW. Effect of food restriction on life span and immune functions in long-lived Fischer-344 X Brown Norway F-1 rats. J Clin Immunol. (1997) 17:85–95. doi: 10.1023/A:1027344730553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. MacDonald R. Some considerations for developing diets for mature rodents used in long-term investigations. J Nutr. (1997) 127:847S−50S. doi: 10.1093/jn/127.5.847S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Masoro EJ. Food restriction in rodents: an evaluation of its role in the study of aging. J Gerontolog. (1988) 43:B59–66. doi: 10.1093/geronj/43.3.B59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rehm S, Nitsche B, Deerberg F. Non-neoplastic lesions of female virgin Han:NMRI mice, incidence and influence of food restriction throughout life span. I: Thyroid. Lab Anim. (1985) 19:214–23. doi: 10.1258/002367785780893601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Weindruch R, Walford RL, Fligiel S, Guthrie D. The retardation of aging in mice by dietary restriction: longevity, cancer, immunity and lifetime energy intake. J Nutr. (1986) 116:641–54. doi: 10.1093/jn/116.4.641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Yu BP. Aging and oxidative stress: modulation by dietary restriction. Free Radic Biol Med. (1996) 21:651–68. doi: 10.1016/0891-5849(96)00162-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Niraula A, Sheridan JF, Godbout J. Microglia priming with aging and stress. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:318–33. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Levay EA, Govic A, Penman J, Paolini AG, Kent S. Effects of adult-onset caloric restriction on anxiety-like behaviour in rats. Physiol Behav. (2017) 92:889–96. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mantis J, Fritz CL, Marsh J, Heinrichs SC, Seyfried TN. Improvement in motor and exploratory behavior in Rett syndrome mice with restricted ketogenic and standard diets. Epilep Behav. (2009) 15:133–41. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.02.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Riddle MC, McKenna MC, Yoon YJ, Pattwell SS, Santos PM, Casey BJ, et al. Caloric restriction enhances fear extinction learning in mice. Neuropsychoparmacology. (2013) 38:930–7. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hendriksen H, Bink DI, Vergoosen DL, van Slobbe ES, Olivier B, Oosting R. Food restriction does not relieve PTSD like anxiety. Eur J Pharmacol. (2015) 753:177–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.10.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Yanai S, Okaichi Y, Okaichi H. Long-term restriction causes negative effects on cognitive functions in rats. Neurobiol Aging. (2004) 25:325–32. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00115-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Sabau RM, Ferkin M. Food restriction affects maternal behavior provided by female meadow voles (microtus pennsylvanicus). J Mamm. (2013) 94:1068–76. doi: 10.1644/13-MAMM-A-060.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Govic A, Levay EA, Kent S, Paolini AG. The social behavior of male rats administered an adult-onset calorie restriction regimen. Phys Behav. (2009) 96:581–5. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lister RG, Hilakivi LA. The effects of novelty, isolation, light and ethanol on the social behavior on mice. Psychopharmacology. (1988) 96:181–7. doi: 10.1007/BF00177558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Peters DP. Effects of prenatal nutritional deficiency of affiliation and aggression in rats. Physiol Behav. (1978) 20:359–62. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90313-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Whatson TS, Smart J. Social behaviour of rats following pre- and early postnatal undernutrition. Physiol Behav. (1978) 20:749–53. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90301-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Lidster K, Owen K, Browne WJ, Prescott MJ. Cage aggression in group-housed laboratory male mice: an international data crowdsourcing project. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:15211. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51674-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, El Khoury A, Mathe AA, Ehlers CL. The effects of social isolation on neuropeptide Y levels, exploratory and anxiety-related behaviors in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2006) 83:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mähler M, Berard M, Feinstein R, Gallagher A, Illgen-Wilcke B, Pritchett-Corning K, et al. FELASA recommendations for the health monitoring of mouse, rat, hamster, guinea pig and rabbit colonies in breeding and experimental units. Lab Anim. (2014) 48:178–92. doi: 10.1177/0023677213516312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

25. Balcombe JP. Laboratory environments and rodents' behavioural needs: a review. Lab Anim. (2006) 40:217–35. doi: 10.1258/002367706777611488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Crawley JN. Social Behavior Tests for Mice. (2007). Available online at: http://ccsystems.us/v2mag/L018.pdf (accessed August 5, 2014).

Google Scholar

27. Keum S, Shin HS. Genetic factors associated with empathy in humans and mice. Neuropharmacology. (2019) 159:107514. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Smith BM, Yao X, Chen KS, Kirby ED. A larger social network enhances novel object location memory and reduces hippocampal microgliosis imaged mice. Front Aging Neurosci. (2018) 10:142. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Strasser A, Skalicky M, Hansalik M, Viidik A. The impact of environment in comparison with moderate physical exercise and dietary restriction on BDNF in the cerebral parietotemporal cortex of aged sprague-dawley rats. Gerontology. (2006) 52:377–81. doi: 10.1159/000095117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Boggiano MM, Cavigelli SA, Dorsey JR, Kelley CEP, Ragan CN, Chandler-Laney PC. Effect of cage divider permitting social stimuli on stress and food intake in rats. Physiol Behav. (2008) 95:222–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Yamada K, Ohki-Hamakazi H, Wada K. Differential effects of social isolation upon body weight, food consumption, and responsiveness to novel and social environment in bombesin receptor subtype-3 (BRS-3) deficient mice. Physiol Behav. (2000) 68:555–61. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00214-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Madra M, Zeltser LM. BDNF-Val66Met variant and adolescent stress interact t promote susceptibility to anorexic behavior in mice. Transl Psychiatry. (2016) 6:e776. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.35

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Zhang F, Yuan S, Shao F, Wang W. Adolescent social defeat induced alterations in social behavior and cognitive flexibility in adult mice: effects of developmental stage and social condition. Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 10:149. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Moneo M, Martín Zúñiga J, Morón I. Caloric restriction in grouped rats: aggregate influences on behavioural and hormonal data. Lab Anim. (2017) 51:490–7. doi: 10.1177/0023677216686805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Swenson KL, Gatt DM, Valdar W, Welsch CE, Cheng R, Chester EJ, et al. High-resolution genetic mapping using the mouse diversity outbred population. Genetics. (2012) 190:437–47. doi: 10.1534/genetics.111.132597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Ehrhart-Bornstein M, Bornstein SR. Cross-talk between adrenal medulla and adrenal cortex in stress. Ann NY Acad Sci. (2008) 1148:112–7. doi: 10.1196/annals.1410.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Möstl A, Palme R. Hormones as indicators of stress. Domest Anim Endocrinol. (2002) 23:67–74. doi: 10.1016/S0739-7240(02)00146-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Friard O, Gamba M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol Evol. (2016) 7:1325–30. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12584

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. Ann Rev Psychol. (1988) 39:43–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.000355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bolivar VJ, Ganus JS, Messeret A. The development of behavioral abnormalities in the motor neurondegeneration (mnd) mouse. Brain Res. (2002) 937:74–82. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02470-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Silverman P. Animal Behaviour in the Laboratory. New York, NY: Pica Press (1978).

Google Scholar

42. Webers EM, Dallaire JA, Gaskill BN, Pritchett-Corning KR, Garner JP. Aggression in group-housed laboratory mice: why can't we solve the problem? Lab Anim. (2017) 46:157–61. doi: 10.1038/laban.1219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Cambona K, Dos-Santos Coura R, Grocc L, Carbona A, Weissmann D, Changeuxd JP, et al. Aggressive behavior during social interaction in mice is controlled by the modulation of tyrosine hydroxylase expression in the prefrontal cortex. Neurosciencie. (2010) 171:840–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Gaskill BN, Stottler AM, Garner JP, Winnicker CW, Mulders GB, Pritchett-Corning KR. The effect of early life experience, environment, and genetic factors on spontaneous home-cage aggression-related wounding in male C57BL/6 mice. Lab Anim. (2017) 46:176–84. doi: 10.1038/laban.1225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Albert DJ, Jonik RH, Walsh ML. Hormone-dependent aggression in male and female rats: experiential, hormonal, and neural foundations. Neurosc Biobehav Rev. (1992) 16:177–92. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80179-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Abbas G, Shoji H, Soya S, Hondo M, Miyakawa T, Sakurai T. Comprehensive behavioral analysis of male Ox1r-/- mice showed implication of orexin receptor-1 in mood, anxiety and social behavior. Front Behav Neurosc. (2015) 9:324. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Mendl M, Paul ES. Parental care, sibling relationships and the development of aggressive-behavior in 2 lines of wild house mice. Behavior. (1991) 116:11–41. doi: 10.1163/156853990X00347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Veenema AH, Meijer OC, de Kloet RE, Koolhaas JM, Bohus BG. Differences in basal and stress-induced HPA regulation of wild house mice selected for high and low aggression. Horm Behav. (2003) 43:197–204. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00013-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kotz M. Integration of feeding and spontaneous physical activity: role of orexin. Phys Behav. (2006) 88:294–301. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Sato N, Tan L, Tate K, Okada M. Rats demonstrate helping behavior toward a soaked conspecific. Anim Cogn. (2015) 18:1039–47. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0872-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sucajtys-Szulc E, Turyn J, Goyke E, Korczynska J, Stelmanska E, Slominska E, et al. Differential effect of prolonged food restriction and fasting on hypothalamic malonyl-CoA concentration and expression of orexigenic and anorexigenic neuropeptides genes in rats. Neuropeptides. (2010) 44:17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2009.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Jensen TL, Kiersgaard MK, Sørensen DB, Mikkelsen LF. Fasting of mice: a review. Lab Anim. (2013) 47:225–40. doi: 10.1177/0023677213501659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Gedik CM, Grant G, Morrice PC, Wood SG, Collins AR. Effects of age and dietary restriction on oxidative DNA damage, antioxidant protection and DNA repair in rats. Eur J Nutr. (2005) 44:263–72. doi: 10.1007/s00394-004-0520-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. He XY, Zhao XL, Gu Q, Shen JP, Hu Y, Hu RM. Calorie restriction from a young age preserves the functions of pancreatic β cells in aging rats. Tohoku J Exp Med. (2012) 227:245–52. doi: 10.1620/tjem.227.245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Chowen JA, Argente-Arizon P, Freire-Regatillo, Argente J. Sex differences in the neuroendocrine control of metabolism and the implication of astrocytes. Front Neuroendoc. (2018) 48:3–12. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Link JC, Reue K. Genetic basis for sex differences in obesity and lipid metabolism. Ann Rev Nutr. (2017) 37:225–45. doi: 10.1146/annurev-nutr-071816-064827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: caloric restriction, grouped mice, littermate mice, adrenocorticotropic, eating behavior, social behavior

Citation: Perea C, Vázquez-Ágredos A, Ruiz-Leyva L, Morón I, Zúñiga JM and Cendán CM (2021) Caloric Restriction in Group-Housed Mice: Littermate and Sex Influence on Behavioral and Hormonal Data. Front. Vet. Sci. 8:639187. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2021.639187

Received: 08 December 2020; Accepted: 18 March 2021;
Published: 15 April 2021.

Edited by:

Alan G. McElligott, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong

Reviewed by:

Sarah M. Zala, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Austria
Liane Hobson, Science for Life Laboratory (SciLifeLab), Sweden
Vootele Voikar, University of Helsinki, Finland

Copyright © 2021 Perea, Vázquez-Ágredos, Ruiz-Leyva, Morón, Zúñiga and Cendán. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ignacio Morón, imoron@ugr.es

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Download