Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Vet. Sci., 08 July 2021
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare
Volume 8 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.676888

Using the Judgment Bias Task to Identify Behavioral Indicators of Affective State: Do Eye Wrinkles in Horses Reflect Mood?

  • Division of Livestock Sciences, Department of Sustainable Agricultural Systems, University of Natural Resources and Life Sciences, Vienna, Austria

Identifying and validating behavioral indicators of mood are important for the assessment of animal welfare. Here, we investigated whether horses' eye wrinkle expression in a presumably neutral situation is a measure of mood as assessed in a cognitive judgment bias task (JBT). To this end, we scored pictures of the left and right eyes of 16 stallions for different aspects of eye wrinkle expression and tested the same individuals on a spatial JBT with active trial initiation. Eye wrinkle expressions were assessed by a qualitative assessment, i.e., the overall assessment of how “worried” horses look, the number of wrinkles, and the angle measured at the intersection of lines drawn through the eyeball and the topmost wrinkle. Correlations between the three eye wrinkle measures and the optimism index as a measure of horses' decisions in the JBT were not statistically significant, but with increasing optimism index, horses tended to be scored as looking less worried (qualitative assessment). We discuss our findings from different perspectives and make suggestions for future research, e.g., by calling for experimental induction of mood and thus greater variation within and/or between individuals and by investigating the interplay between shorter-lasting emotional and longer-lasting mood states to further explore the potential use of the JBT to validate eye wrinkles and other facial or body expressions as indicators of mood.

Introduction

Measures of spontaneous behavioral expression, including facial expressions, have great potential for assessing animals' affective states since they are non-invasive and do not require animals to be trained. This research area is still in its infancy (1), but has received increasing attention in recent years (2). One approach to study facial expressions in non-human animals is the Facial Action Coding System (FACS), which details all potential expressions resulting from facial muscle contraction and relaxation. Originally developed for humans (3), FACSs have been adapted to different species encompassing different primates [e.g., (4, 5)], horses (6), dogs (7, 8), and cats (9, 10). They serve as a tool to study facial expressions across different contexts. Another approach is to identify context-specific changes in the face by comparing, for example, facial expressions in a painful and a control situation. So-called pain grimace scales have been developed and applied in a number of species and across various painful situations [e.g., (1114)]. Facial expressions have also been studied as potential indicators of other negative and positive affective states. Most studies compared changes in the face during or immediately after exposure to a presumably positive or negative stimulus to a presumably neutral control situation, focusing on either several parts of the face [e.g., (15, 16)] or only aspects, for example, the ears [e.g., (17, 18)] or eyes [e.g., (19, 20)]. The identified facial expressions are supposed to reflect stimulus-specific short-term affective states, i.e., emotions. Research on facial expressions indicative of longer-term affective states, i.e., moods, is less readily available, mainly because of the quick transitions between contraction and relaxation of facial muscles. However, some studies show that facial expressions are also indicative of chronic pain [e.g., dental disorders in horses: (21); footrot in sheep: (22)]. Beyond the context of pain, researchers discuss whether facial expression may indicate not only short-term discrete emotional states but also possibly emotional valence (23) and long-term mood (24). Research in humans revealed that people suffering from psychological disorders like borderline personality disorders (25) or (unipolar) depression (26, 27) show reduced facial expressiveness (25, 26) or overall different patterns of facial expression (27) when exposed to emotional movies or memories, indicating an interaction between underlying long-term affective states and facial expression of emotion.

Facial expressions reflecting long-term affective states would be highly valuable as indicators for the assessment of animal welfare. Here, we present a first study investigating one particular aspect of facial expression, eye wrinkle expression in horses, as a potential indicator of mood. Eye wrinkles above the eyeball originate from the contraction of the inner brow raiser and have been described in detail in two studies. In the first study, eye wrinkles were assessed in horses exposed to two presumably positive and two presumably negative situations (28). The angle, one measure of eye wrinkle expression, increased in one negative situation (caused by muscle contraction) and decreased in one positive situation (caused by muscle relaxation), while other measures remained unaffected. In the second study, the effect of age, sex, breed type, and coat color on eye wrinkle expression in a presumably neutral situation was assessed in 181 horses (29). Except for breed type affecting the width of the angle, none of the investigated factors explained the high variation in eye wrinkle expression between individuals; differences in mood could be a potential explanation (29). Eye wrinkles develop or deepen when the “skin above the inner corner of the eye is pulled dorsally and obliquely toward the medial frontal region,” which is defined as Action Unit 101 in the equine FACS [(6), p. 8], similar to Action Unit 1 in the human FACS (3). In humans, Action Unit 1 is activated when a person is sad [e.g., (24, 30)], and it forms part of the “Veraguth fold,” a fold above the eyes which appears when the inner eyebrows are lifted and pulled together in depressed individuals [e.g., (31, 32)], suggesting a relationship between a contraction of the inner brow raiser and negative mood, at least in humans.

The present study aimed to investigate the relationship between eye wrinkle expression and mood in horses. To this end, we assessed eye wrinkles in 16 stallions on pictures taken in their home environments and tested the same individuals on a judgment bias task (JBT) as a measure of mood. In a JBT, decision making under ambiguity is examined, and animals' responses to the ambiguous cues are interpreted as either “optimistic” (i.e., the animal responds as if expecting a positive outcome) or “pessimistic” (i.e., the animal responds as if expecting a negative outcome). For details on the background and theory of the JBT, see the (systematic) reviews by Mendl et al. (33), Roelofs et al. (34), Neville et al. (35), and Lagisz et al. (36). If horses' eye wrinkles are an indicator of mood, we predicted that with decreasing scores in eye wrinkle expression, horses will be more likely to respond optimistically by showing go responses when exposed to ambiguous trials in the JBT.

Animals, Materials, and Methods

Animals

This study includes data from 16 Franches-Montagnes stallions between 4 and 22 years of age (mean ± standard deviation: 9.1 ± 4.8 years). Horses were housed in standard single boxes (3 × 3.5 m) on wood shavings or straw with visual contact to conspecifics at the Swiss National Stud Farm of Agroscope (Avenches, Switzerland). For more information, see Hintze et al. (37).

JBT

The detailed training and test protocols as well as the individual performance of the horses during testing are described in Hintze et al. (37). In short, we used a spatial task design with the location of two out of five goal-boxes signaling either reward or non-reward. The opening of one of the goal-boxes served as a cue for the horses. Before the discrimination training, horses learned to actively initiate each trial by touching a bottle opposite the goal-boxes. After reaching the learning criterion (i.e., 80% correct responses in positive and negative trials across two consecutive sessions), horses were exposed to six test sessions, in each of which they were confronted with three intermediate cues (near positive, middle, and near negative) interspersed between 25 positive and 25 negative reference cues.

Eye Wrinkle Assessment

Pictures from the eye area of the horses were taken in their home boxes approximately 2 weeks after JBT testing had finished. The experimenter who had trained and tested the horses and was thus familiar to them (SH) took all pictures in a presumably neutral situation, i.e., between noon and 2 p.m. when it was most quiet at the stud farm. Moreover, she stopped the process if any visual or acoustic disturbance occurred. All details of the picture-taking process are described in Schanz et al. (29). We randomly chose six pictures per horse, three from the left eye area and three from the right eye area. All 96 pictures were scored according to the eye wrinkle assessment scale by Schanz et al. (29). This scale includes five outcome measures: (1) qualitative assessment, “qualitative”, i.e., overall first assessment of how “worried” the horse looks assessed on a visual analog scale ranging from 0 to 100 (“not worried” to “extremely worried”); (2) “brow raised”, i.e., the amount the skin above the eye is raised assessed on a visual analog scale ranging from 0 to 100 (“not raised” to “strongly raised”); (3) “number”, i.e., the number of wrinkles above the eyeball; (4) “markedness”, i.e., depth and width of the wrinkles assessed in the three categories “no wrinkles”, “weak”, “strong”; and (5) “angle” measured in degree at the intersection of lines drawn through the eyeball and the topmost wrinkle.

Ethical Considerations

This study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines of the Swiss Animal Welfare Ordinance (TSchV 455.1). The experimental procedures were approved by the Cantonal Veterinary Office in Vaud, Switzerland (license number: 2804.1).

Statistical Analyses

Analyses were performed using the statistical programming language R [R version 4.0.2, (38); RStudio version 1.4.1103, (39)]. The graph was produced using the package “ggplot2” (40).

Collinearity Between Eye Wrinkle Measures

The eye wrinkle data we used are a subset of the data published by Schanz et al. (29), in which collinearity between some of the measures was found. We performed the same checks for the subset, i.e., Spearman's rank correlations to assess the association between two continuous measures and Kruskal–Wallis tests with subsequent pairwise Wilcoxon rank sum tests to assess the relationship between one continuous and one categorical measure. A strong correlation between “qualitative” and “brow raised” (rs = 0.92, p < 0.001) was found; “brow raised” was excluded from further analyses. “Qualitative” (χ22 = 31.43, p < 0.001), “brow raised” (χ22 = 33.22, p < 0.001) and “number” (χ22 = 86.07, p < 0.001) were associated with “markedness,” and thus, “markedness” was excluded from further analyses.

Differences in Eye Wrinkle Measures for the Left and the Right Eyes

The mean score per eye and horse was calculated for each eye wrinkle measure to test for a possible difference between the left and the right eyes. Linear mixed-effects models [function: lme, package: nlme, (41)] were run with “eye” (left or right) as a fixed effect and “horse” as a random effect. Model assumptions were verified by visually checking residuals for normal distribution and homogeneity of variance. Transformation of the data was not necessary.

Correlations Between Eye Wrinkle Measures and Performance in the JBT

To correlate the results from the JBT with the eye wrinkle measures, we first calculated an “optimism index” for each horse [see (42)]. In short, for ambiguous trials, the proportion of no-go responses was subtracted from the proportion of go responses, resulting in one value ranging between −1 and 1 per horse. Normal distribution of the “optimism index” and the eye wrinkle measures was visually checked; only the “optimism index” was normally distributed. We thus ran Spearman's rank correlations for the three eye wrinkle measures and the “optimism index”. For “angle”, a subset of the full data set was used; only pictures with at least one wrinkle (“number” ≥ 1) were included since an angle could only be measured if at least one wrinkle was identified (28, 29).

Results

Overview of the Proportion of Go Responses in the JBT

The proportion of go responses ranged from 0.5 to 1 in near-positive trials (mean ± standard deviation: 0.84 ± 0.2), from 0.3 to 1 in middle trials (0.80 ± 0.2) and from 0 to 1 in near-negative trials (0.36 ± 0.3). The “optimism index” across the three ambiguous cues ranged from −0.43 to 1 (0.31 ± 0.41). Information on training duration and test performance can be found in Hintze et al. (37).

Overview of the Eye Wrinkle Measures

“Qualitative” ranged between 0.5 and 100 on a visual analog scale ranging from 0 to 100 (mean ± standard deviation: 45.8 ± 30.9), “number” of wrinkles varied between 0 and 4 (0.8 ± 1.0), and the “angle” ranged from 18.1 to 38.8° (30.9 ± 7.2).

No Differences in Eye Wrinkle Measures Between the Left and the Right Eyes

No statistically significant difference between eyes was found for “qualitative” (F1, 15 = 1.51, p = 0.24), “number” (F1, 15 = 1.71, p = 0.21), or “angle” (F1, 7 = 0.61, p = 0.46).

Correlations Between Eye Wrinkle Measures and Performance in the JBT

The correlations between the three eye wrinkle measures and the “optimism index” were not statistically significant (“qualitative”: rs = −0.49, p = 0.06; “number”: rs = −0.01, p = 0.97; “angle”: rs = 0.09, p = 0.77; Figure 1), but there was a tendency for a moderate negative correlation between “qualitative” and the “optimism index”; i.e., with an increasing “optimism index”, horses tended to be scored as looking less worried.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Correlations between each eye wrinkle measure and the “optimism index”. Spearman's rank correlation coefficients rho (R) with their associated p-values and visualizations (blue line) based on the displayed data points are given for the eye wrinkle measures “qualitative” (A), “number” (B), and “angle” (C).

Discussion

We aimed to investigate whether horses' eye wrinkle expression is a potential indicator of mood as assessed in a spatial JBT with active trial initiation. Correlations between the three eye wrinkle measures and the “optimism index” as a measure of horses' performance in the JBT were not statistically significant, but with an increasing “optimism index”, horses tended to be scored as looking less worried (“qualitative”).

The statistical tendency for the moderate negative correlation between the qualitative assessment and horses' decisions in the JBT is in line with our prediction that horses respond less optimistically in the JBT with higher scores for the eye wrinkle expression. This tentatively indicates that one aspect of eye wrinkle expression may reflect mood in horses. However, this result needs to be interpreted carefully since our sample size was relatively small, which increases the risk of obtaining a spuriously large correlation coefficient.

Our statistically non-significant results need to be discussed from different perspectives. First, it is possible that some aspects of eye wrinkle expression, like the angle, are affected by short-term emotional states, as shown by Hintze et al. (28), rather than by longer-term mood states. However, we know that eye wrinkles are also visible in presumably neutral situations (29) and control situation (28) and not only in response to short-term stimuli. Moreover, contraction of the inner brow raiser may potentially be caused by general eye movement. A recent study in dogs demonstrated that “puppy dog eyes”, also resulting from contraction of the inner brow raiser, could be caused by movement of the eyes and that the expression was shown independently of eye movement only in 6% of the cases (43). It would thus be valuable to investigate the relationship between eye wrinkle expression and eye movement in horses as well. However, the puppy-like expression in dogs was only compared across different emotional situations and not to a control situation (43, 44); thus, we do not know whether the expression is also present in rather neutral situations as in the case of horses. Eye wrinkle expression in neutral situations may potentially be less affected by eye movement than during emotional situations, but further research is needed to investigate this association.

Second, the variation between horses' performance in the JBT and in the eye wrinkle measures might have been too small to detect potential associations. All measures were taken in presumably neutral situations without the horses' mood being manipulated by a treatment. A next step would thus be to include a mood-inducing treatment, e.g., varying housing conditions, potentially resulting in greater variation between and, depending on the experimental design, within animals. Moreover, it would be worth considering how underlying mood states affect changes in facial expressions when confronted with short-term stimuli and thus the interaction between longer-term mood and shorter-term emotional states. This suggestion is in line with the results from a recent study by Clarkson et al. (45), who investigated the interplay between mood and emotions and found that negative mood affected the expression of negative but not positive emotion in mice. It would therefore be valuable to assess changes in response to positive and negative short-term stimuli in addition to the implementation of a mood-inducing treatment.

Third, in a correlational approach, it is always important to discuss both variables of interest. Mood may not have affected eye wrinkle expression, but it is also possible that the JBT was not sensitive enough to detect only slight differences in mood states between the horses. So far, to our knowledge, the JBT has been used primarily to detect differences between mood-inducing treatments but not to draw inferences from the animals' baseline mood. Moreover, we calculated an “optimism index” for correlation with the eye wrinkle measures, which accounts for the total number of go and no-go responses in ambiguous trials but ignores the different trial types (i.e., near positive, middle, and near negative); thus, it may not be sensitive enough to detect subtle differences in mood.

Fourth, the temporal delay between the JBT testing and the picture-taking process was not ideal and could potentially explain why we did not find an association between the measures since the stallions' mood may have changed over this time. However, treatment and management of the horses did not change during these two weeks, which is why we do not have an indication that the horses' mood changed, but the methodological limitation of the temporal delay needs to be acknowledged.

Conclusion and Outlook

Previous research has shown that eye wrinkle expression is independent of most characteristics of the horse and that the angle measured at the intersection of lines drawn through the eyeball and the topmost wrinkle may be an indicator of short-term emotional states. The present study does not support our hypothesis that eye wrinkle expression in horses may serve as an indicator of mood as assessed in a JBT, but the tendency for a statistically significant, moderate, and negative correlation between the qualitative assessment of the eye wrinkles and the “optimism index” as a measure of judgment bias warrants further investigation. Future studies using a mood-inducing treatment and investigating the interplay between shorter-lasting emotional and longer-lasting mood states are needed to explore the potential use of the JBT to validate eye wrinkles and other facial as well as body expressions as indicators of mood.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Cantonal Veterinary Office in Vaud, Switzerland (license number: 2804.1).

Author Contributions

SH trained and tested the horses on the judgement bias task, took the pictures from the horses' eye, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. LS scored all pictures, performed the data analysis, and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

We gratefully acknowledge the support by the BOKU Vienna Open Access Publishing Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer MS declared a past co-authorship with one of the authors SH to the handling editor.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the staff from the Swiss National Stud Farm for their support with the experimental setup, Iris Bachman for logistical support, Christoph Winckler for his valuable comments on this manuscript, and two reviewers for their very constructive feedback.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fvets.2021.676888/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Descovich KA, Wathan J, Leach MC, Buchanan-smith HM, Flecknell P, Farningham D, et al. Facial expression : an under-utilised tool for the assessment of welfare in mammals. ALTEX. (2017) 34:1–21. doi: 10.14573/altex.1607161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Girard BB, Bellone C. Revealing animal emotions. Science (80-.). (2020) 368:33–4. doi: 10.1126/science.abb2796

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press (1978).

Google Scholar

4. Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick SJ, Bard KA. Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion. (2007) 7:172–81. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Waller BM, Lembeck M, Kuchenbuch P, Burrows AM, Liebal K. GibbonFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for hylobatids. Int J Primatol. (2012) 33:809–21. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9611-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wathan J, Burrows AM, Waller BM, McComb K. EquiFACS: the equine facial action coding system. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0131738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bremhorst A, Sutter NA, Würbel H, Mills DS, Riemer S. Differences in facial expressions during positive anticipation and frustration in dogs awaiting a reward. Sci. Rep. (2019) 9:19312. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-55714-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Waller BM, Peirce K, Caeiro CC, Scheider L, Burrows AM, McCune S, et al. Paedomorphic facial expressions give dogs a selective advantage. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e82686. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bennett V, Gourkow N, Mills DS. Facial correlates of emotional behaviour in the domestic cat (Felis catus). Behav Processes. (2017) 141:342–50. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Caeiro CC, Burrows AM, Waller BM. Development and application of CatFACS : Are human cat adopters influenced by cat facial expressions? Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2017) 189:66–78. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dalla Costa E, Minero M, Lebelt D, Stucke D, Canali E, Leach MC. Development of the Horse Grimace Scale (HGS) as a pain assessment tool in horses undergoing routine castration. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e92281. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gleerup KB, Andersen PH, Munksgaard L, Forkman B. Pain evaluation in dairy cattle. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2015) 171:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Keating SCJ, Thomas AA, Flecknell PA, Leach MC. Evaluation of EMLA cream for preventing pain during tattooing of rabbits: changes in physiological, behavioural and facial expression responses. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e44437. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Langford DJ, Bailey AL, Chanda ML, Clarke SE, Drummond TE, Echols S, et al. Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nat Methods. (2010) 7:447–9. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Finlayson K, Lampe JF, Hintze S, Würbel H, Melotti L. Facial indicators of positive emotions in rats. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0166446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lansade L, Nowak R, Lainé A, Leterrier C, Bonneau C, Parias C, et al. Facial expression and oxytocin as possible markers of positive emotions in horses. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32993-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Boissy A, Aubert A, Greiveldinger L, Delval E, Veissier I. Cognitive sciences to relate ear postures to emotions in sheep. Anim Welf. (2011) 20:47–56.

Google Scholar

18. Proctor HS, Carder G. Can ear postures reliably measure the positive emotional state of cows? Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2014) 161:20–7. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Gómez Y, Bieler R, Hankele AK, Zähner M, Savary P, Hillmann E. Evaluation of visible eye white and maximum eye temperature as non- invasive indicators of stress in dairy cows. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2018) 198:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Sandem A-I, Braastad BO. Effects of cow–calf separation on visible eye white and behaviour in dairy cows - A brief report. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2005) 95:233–9. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Coneglian MM, Borges TD, Weber SH, Bertagnon GH, Michelotto VP. Use of the horse grimace scale to identify and quantify pain due to dental disorders in horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2020) 225:104970. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2020.104970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. McLennan KM, Rebelo CJB, Corke MJ, Holmes MA, Leach MC, Constantino-Casas F. Development of a facial expression scale using footrot and mastitis as models of pain in sheep. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2016) 176:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Feldman Barrett L. Was darwin wrong about emotional expressions? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2011) 20:400–6. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Arellano D, Perales FJ, Varona J. Mood and its mapping onto facial expressions. In: Proc. 8th Int. Conf. AMDO 2014 (Palma de Mallorca), (2014). p. 31–40.

Google Scholar

25. Renneberg B, Heyn K, Gebhard R, Bachmann S. Facial expression of emotions in borderline personality disorder and depression. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2005) 36:183–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2005.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Jaeger J, Borod JC, Peselow E. Facial expression of positive and negative emotions in patients with unipolar depression. J Affect Disord. (1986) 11:43–50. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(86)90058-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schwartz GE, Fair PL, Salt P, Mandel MR, Klerman GL. Facial muscle patterning to affective imagery in depressed and nondepressed subjects. Science (80-.). (1976) 192:489–91. doi: 10.1126/science.1257786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hintze S, Smith S, Patt A, Bachmann I, Würbel H. Are eyes a mirror of the soul? What eye wrinkles reveal about a horse's emotional state. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0164017. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Schanz L, Krueger K, Hintze S. Sex and age don't matter, but breed type does — Factors influencing eye wrinkle expression in horses. Front Vet Sci. (2019) 6:154. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bullock AB. The Expression and Regulation of Sadness in Complicated Grief. New York, NY: Columbia University (2012).

Google Scholar

31. Ellgring H. Non-verbal Communication in Depression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1989).

Google Scholar

32. Pampouchidou A, Marias K, Tsiknakis M, Yang F, Meriaudeau F. Designing a framework for assisting depression severity assessment from facial image analysis. In: 2015 IEEE International Conference on Signal Image Processing Applications (ICSIPA). Kuala Lumpur: IEEE (2015). p. 578–83.

Google Scholar

33. Mendl M, Burman OHP, Parker RMA, Paul ES. Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2009) 118:161–81. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Roelofs S, Boleij H, Nordquist RE, van der Staay FJ. Making decisions under ambiguity: Judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 10:119. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Neville V, Nakagawa S, Zidar J, Paul ES, Lagisz M, Bateson M, et al. Pharmacological manipulations of judgement bias: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 108:269–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lagisz M, Nakagawa S, Neville V, Sorato E, Paul ES, Bateson M, et al. Optimism, pessimism and judgement bias in animals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 118:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hintze S, Melotti L, Colosio S, Bailoo JD, Boada-Saña M, Würbel H, et al. A cross-species judgement bias task: integrating active trial initiation into a spatial Go/No-go task. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23459-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Core Team (2020).

Google Scholar

39. R Studio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston, MA: R Core Team (2021).

Google Scholar

40. Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer Nature (2016).

Google Scholar

41. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Core Team T. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models_. R package version 3.1-137 (2018). Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (accessed January 3, 2021).

Google Scholar

42. Rygula R, Pluta H, Popik P. Laughing rats are optimistic. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e51959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051959

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bremhorst A, Mills DS, Stolzlechner L, Würbel H, Riemer S. ‘Puppy dog eyes' are associated with eye movements, not communication. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:568935. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.568935

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Kaminski J, Hynds J, Morris P, Waller BM. Human attention affects facial expressions in domestic dogs. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:12914. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12781-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Clarkson JM, Leach MC, Flecknell PA, Rowe C. Negative mood affects the expression of negative but not positive emotions in mice. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2020) 287:20201636. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.1636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: animal welfare, animal emotion, facial expression, eye wrinkle expression, cognitive bias task

Citation: Hintze S and Schanz L (2021) Using the Judgment Bias Task to Identify Behavioral Indicators of Affective State: Do Eye Wrinkles in Horses Reflect Mood? Front. Vet. Sci. 8:676888. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2021.676888

Received: 06 March 2021; Accepted: 25 May 2021;
Published: 08 July 2021.

Edited by:

Elodie Floriane Briefer, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

Reviewed by:

Marek Spinka, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Czechia
Sandra Düpjan, Leibniz Institute for Farm Animal Biology (FBN), Germany

Copyright © 2021 Hintze and Schanz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Hintze, sara.hintze@boku.ac.at

Download