Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Vet. Sci., 29 July 2022
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare
Volume 9 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.656833

Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions: A One Health–One Welfare Systemic Approach to Studying HAI

  • Université de Lorraine, UR 4360 APEMAC, Metz, France

The Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions (IMHAI) described herewith provides a conceptual framework for the study of interspecies interactions and aims to model the primary emotional processes involved in human-animal interactions. This model was developed from theoretical inputs from three fundamental disciplines for understanding interspecies interactions: neuroscience, psychology and ethology, with the objective of providing a transdisciplinary approach on which field professionals and researchers can build and collaborate. Seminal works in affective neuroscience offer a common basis between humans and animals and, as such, can be applied to the study of interspecies interactions from a One Health-One Welfare perspective. On the one hand, Jaak Panksepp's research revealed that primary/basic emotions originate in the deep subcortical regions of the brain and are shared by all mammals, including humans. On the other hand, several works in the field of neuroscience show that the basic physiological state is largely determined by the perception of safety. Thus, emotional expression reflects the state of an individual's permanent adaptation to ever-changing environmental demands. Based on this evidence and over 5 years of action research using grounded theory, alternating between research and practice, the IMHAI proposes a systemic approach to the study of primary-process emotional affects during interspecies social interactions, through the processes of emotional transfer, embodied communication and interactive emotional regulation. IMHAI aims to generate new hypotheses and predictions on affective behavior and interspecies communication. Application of such a model should promote risk prevention and the establishment of positive links between humans and animals thereby contributing to their respective wellbeing.

Introduction

The study of human-animal interactions (HAI), relationships (HAR) and bonds (HAB) between humans and animals is an increasingly topical issue in many fields of research. Most HAI studies are conducted in the context of research on companion or agricultural animals, along with an increase in recent years of studies on laboratory, zoo and wild animals (1). HAI can be defined as a dynamic process in which previous human-animal interactions form the basis of a relationship that then exerts a feedback effect on the nature and perception of future interactions (2). The study of HAI is an interdisciplinary research area aimed at quantifying the bidirectional effects of the human-animal relationship on human and animal health and wellbeing (3).

HAI have been shown to provide health benefits for humans (47), and research also demonstrates that the amount and quality or nature of interactions between animals and their caretakers and handlers have a significant effect on the behavior, physiology and wellbeing of farmed (810) and domestic animals (11, 12).

However, in the literature and to our knowledge, there has yet to be a unified theoretical framework allowing to model the way humans and animals interact together. Without the development of an integrative theoretical framework to approach the study of HAI, scientific progress in the various areas of HAI/HAB/HAR will remain limited. In this article, we present the Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions (IMHAI) which mobilizes current knowledge from three disciplines: neurosciences, psychology and ethology, as well as processes identified directly from over 5 years of action research in order to propose a conceptual framework that can serve as a common basis for future interdisciplinary work in the field of HAI, the underpinning of which is outlined in section The Integrative Model of Human–Animal Interactions. More specifically, we propose to focus on the study of a process common to humans and animals which we hypothesize is central and conditions HAI: emotions.

Emotions are considered at the heart of many definitions of animal welfare circulating in the scientific community (1318). Indeed, if we consider that, in animals, emotion has a valence (positive or negative), i.e., that it is pleasant or unpleasant, it actually improves or compromises the animal's wellbeing (19). To date, emotions have mostly been studied in animal welfare science as intra-individual phenomena, i.e., as coordination mechanisms that guide the animal to take appropriate actions (19). Influenced by previous research in humans, several theoretical frameworks have been developed to study and assess emotions in animals, such as the cognitive appraisal theory (20), or the theoretical framework developed by Mendl et al. (21) integrating both the dimensional approach to emotions with the discrete emotions approach. However, on many occasions, emotions are generated in social contexts: emotions have an important social dimension, have high communicative value, carry meaning (22), and have many impacts on cognitive and social processes (23, 24). In particular, in social species, interactions with peers are critically important to the development of individuals and have a preponderant impact on their health and wellbeing (25, 26). Beyond context, it is therefore highly likely that the emotional regulation of social beings is dependent on the interactions and relationships they have with each other (27).

Based on the previously cited work but also societal developments, there appears to be a concomitant paradigm shift in our current appreciation of animal welfare and human-animal relationships (28). A good example of this is the evolution of the 2020 Five Domains model (29) referencing contemporary verified scientific thinking for assessing animal welfare. This model includes five domains: (1) nutrition, (2) physical environment, (3) health, (4) behavioral interactions, and (5) mental/emotional state, which is the subjective experience of the animal derived from the previous four domains. The notable evolution of the model is in domain 4, previously called “Behavior,” renamed “Behavioral Interactions,” and subdivided according to the nature of the animals' interactions with (a) their environment, (b) other non-human animals, and (c) humans. It appears that the assessment of welfare, previously based on linear causality, has been abandoned in favor of an interactionist approach that studies dynamic and relational phenomena in constant evolution (30).

Through this article, we will present the Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions (IMHAI), which argues that attention to emotions during HAI should enhance our understanding of interspecies communication and may open new avenues for positively influencing human and animal health and wellbeing when interacting with each other. In particular, we propose to embed this model in a systemic approach in congruence with the One Health–One Welfare concepts for the study of HAI.

The Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions

The goal of the Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions (IMHAI) is to propose a common model for both humans and animals in order to better understand interspecies interactions. We propose to study the primary emotions, according to the individual's perception of the environment as well as to social context. To develop the IMHAI, an inductive qualitative research methodology was used, in the tradition of Grounded Theory (31) and Action Research (32) which propose to theorize different phenomena based on a constant back and forth between research and practice. This methodology enabled us to explore the primary emotional processes occurring in HAI and to gain new theoretical insights. In this analysis, we present the result of a theoretical reflection carried out within the setting of a more global research conducted over a period of 5 years, the results of which are currently being published. At present, the model is intended for the study of interactions between humans and other mammals and birds. Herein, the focus is on HAI/HAR by referring to the non-human animal as “animal” for simplicity of text. However, this model could conceivably be the object of additional modifications and proposals that could allow for the study of interactions between humans and other types of living beings such as fish and reptiles.

Toward a Systemic One Health–One Welfare Model of HAI

The One Health initiative takes a multidisciplinary approach to optimizing human, animal and environmental health (33), while the One Welfare approach aims to integrate animal welfare, human welfare and the physical and social environment, with the goal of improving global wellbeing and achieving gains in development (34). According to Jordan and Lem (35), “One Health initiatives have traditionally focused on threats to human and animal health, such as zoonoses and a secure food supply, they have not typically promoted an understanding of the many beneficial physical and psychosocial impacts of human-animal relationships and how these can be leveraged to improve both human and animal health around the world.”

New thinking is emerging, particularly in the field of animal-assisted therapy, for the development of a One Health–One Welfare framework applied to HAI (36). The main tenet being that HAI themselves are leveraged for the development of synergistic health and welfare benefits for both the human and the animal during the intervention. To achieve this outcome, Hediger et al. (37) support the need for a systemic approach to studying the interrelationship and reciprocal influence of human-animal relationships by including environmental factors and social contexts. Furthermore, Menna et al. (38) suggest studying HAI as a complex system (39), strongly encouraging the study of an interspecies relationship as a reciprocity. Indeed, in its very etymology, interaction suggests the precept of mutual, reciprocal action of several elements. The notion of reciprocity implies one of feedback, a circular process in which the receiver's reaction to a message (or action) is driven in the direction of the sender and in turn acts as a stimulus, an influence on him within a series of exchanges (40).

Based on these elements, the IMHAI proposes to study HAI from an interactionist and systemic perspective, studying the processes that modulate HAI, taking into account context and environmental factors, in order to promote the establishment of positive interactions between humans and animals that contribute to their health and wellbeing. Adopting an interactionist approach to interspecies social phenomena signifies taking a point of view where the basic unit of social analysis is not the individual action, but rather the system formed by the set of actions that occur between individuals which, in a given context, respond to each other to generate a situation, a reality to be observed and analyzed (40). Finally, it focuses on the communication process considered as a global phenomenon and integrating all patterns of behavior that can have a communicative value, such as speech, facial expressions, gaze, gestures, interpersonal distance, etc.

In the following section Processes Possibly Involved in Interspecies Communication, we propose to explore interspecies emotional communication through the hypothesis of emotional transfer including automatic processes such as emotional contagion but also more complex processes involving social cognition.

Processes Possibly Involved in Interspecies Communication

Emotional Contagion

Emotional contagion, a process considered as the basis of primary empathy, occurs when the perception of another individual's emotional state automatically activates shared representations, eliciting a corresponding emotional state in the observer (4143). For example, a signal emitted by an individual may induce the same arousal level (i.e., contagion of emotional arousal) and the same valence (i.e., contagion of emotional valence) in the receiver (44). The most recent empirical works have predominantly focused on fright given that the behavioral responses generated by this emotion are easily conditionable and observable under experimental conditions (45, 46).

Primary empathy is a bottom-up control process presumed to be already present in birds and mammals, giving individuals the intrinsic abilities to resonate with the pains and joys of their conspecifics through primitive emotional contagion (42, 47). In a study by Reimert (48), the authors examined the phenomenon of emotional contagion in pigs (i.e., Sus scrofa domesticus) and showed that a negative treatment of social isolation not only affected the emotional state of the animal itself, but also that of other pigs that were not subjected to the negative treatment, with the stress effect being prolonged even beyond the duration of the experiment.

The process of emotional contagion is equally studied in the context of interspecies interactions, particularly through the effect of mimicry (body and facial expression) (49). However, motor mimicry is not a prerequisite for emotional contagion, but rather one of its modalities. For example, in juvenile common ravens (i.e., Corvus corax), emotional contagion in the context of play could occur through a general mood state transfer resulting in increased locomotion and social play rather than through the reproduction of the same motor patterns (50). Human body odors produced in states of happiness and fear also stimulate animals emotionally and induce sympathetic and parasympathetic changes, suggesting an interspecies transfer of emotions through this means (i.e., autonomic mimicry) (5153). Emotional contagion through vocalizations is also studied in different animal species (54, 55) and shows compelling results (56, 57). In particular, it is established that animals, like humans, have the ability to discriminate vocal expression of emotions (58). Furthermore, vocalizations have the potential to influence the receivers' affective states through direct (e.g., acoustic startle reflex) or indirect effects [e.g., affective learning and learned affect (59)], which could lead to state matching, as discussed in the section Embodied Communication with processes of social affordance and embodied cognition.

Embodied Communication

To study interspecies communication, we suggest that the processes of social affordance and embodied cognitions are key investigation points of interest, as they include the contribution of social cognition in an ecological approach to behavior, where perception is considered as a process emerging from the individual-environment system. According to Gibson (60), both humans and animals are responsive to “affordances,” i.e., to possibilities for action offered by an environment, and are selectively responsive to a specific affordance over another. Social affordances are a subcategory of affordances, namely possibilities for social interaction offered by an environment. In this perspective, a number of different authors [see for example (61, 62)] postulate that the intentionality of actions and the ability to understand the intentions of others are based more on primary and sensorimotor processes than on highly specialized cognitive abilities such as inferences and mentalization. For instance, in a situation where one individual observes what the other is doing, the process of resonance via mirror neurons can be understood as part of an intersubjective perceptual process (63). By observing the other individual's expressive actions and movements, in the context of the surrounding external environment, the individual perceives their meaning (61). Thus, perceiving the embodied behavior in the situation provides sufficient information for understanding, reacting and interacting with the other. The ability of humans and animals to communicate with each other therefore does not presuppose an a priori cognitive processing for the interpretation of “emotional” signals (64). Emotional expressions (generated by the activation of primary emotional systems) are considered as social affordances produced by individual A to act on individual B (65). In this sense, they precondition individual B for action, depending on the nature of the expressed emotion, the social context, the socio-affective skills and the motor repertoire of the protagonists. By producing emotional expression, individual A instigates different intentions to act on individual B; these intentions to act are not necessarily conscious and can lead to different social consequences. For certain emotional expressions, the aim of the signal from individual A is to make individual B react (anger); for other emotional expressions, the function of the signal is to “contaminate” individual B (i.e., emotional contagion) (fear) by prompting the latter to act in a congruent manner. Other emotions are intended to gain the attention, curiosity and affection of another individual. Thus, in social contexts, emotional expressions provide information about an individual's willingness to enter into a relationship with another individual, or conversely, their unwillingness to enter into a relationship (22). In particular, it would be interesting to study how interspecies emotional transfer influences the characteristics of the interaction and the subsequent relationship, namely the frequency and duration of the interaction, the initiation of physical contact, or even behavioral synchronization and behavioral correspondence (66), that increase mutual affiliation (67).

In the following section Attachment and Interactive Emotional Regulation, we propose focusing on a third process that is likely to influence interspecies communication, namely the interactive emotional regulation associated with the autonomic nervous system, in order to explore the factors potentially involved in the emergence of positive or negative interactions, contributing in the longer term to the establishment of affective links between humans and animals.

Attachment and Interactive Emotional Regulation

Through extensive study of attachment and autonomic regulation via the vagus nerve, various authors (6872) argue that the need to connect with others is a primary biological imperative, both for humans and social animals. The findings of their respective seminal works are consistent with the precepts of the attachment theory that were first defined by Bowlby (68, 73). Social bonds strongly support emotional balance and promote wellbeing, mental and physical health throughout the life span of mammals and birds (7476).

According to Schore (76, 77), the attachment theory is essentially a regulation theory. Interactive emotional regulation implies that emotions are regulated through interaction. It prompts us to depart from the traditional individual-centered analysis of social cognition and proposes to consider social cognition as grounded by neurophysiological processes that are distributed across brains and bodies and is manifested in the coregulation of behaviors (78). In a dyadic interaction, coregulation refers to a process whereby the emotions of both individuals are moderated over time and return to homeostatic levels. Codysregulation, on the other hand, means that the emotions of both individuals are amplified and move away from homeostatic equilibrium (79). For example, through the process of emotional contagion, an interspecies emotional codysregulation can be observed: fear experienced by the human can be transmitted to the animal which, in turn, undergoes a fear-related sympathetic activation, observable at the physiological and behavioral level (80). Repeated experiences of codysregulation lead to difficulties in the establishment of a good relationship, and even to a rupture of bonds (81). In contrast, coregulation promotes emotional security, trust and leads to bonding (82). In this context, both individuals feel a sense of wellbeing and safety that is conducive to the development of emotional bonds and learning. In particular, the interactive emotional coregulation between the child and his caregiver during the first years of life would be linked to the development of a sense of safety and self-regulation skills and would modulate the neurodevelopmental aspect of the child's future adaptive functioning (8386).

Furthermore, the attachment theory proposes that the history and quality of early interactions with primary caregivers, via interactive emotional regulation, shape the young's internal attachment patterns forming more generalized mental representations of self and others as adults (68, 87, 88). Researchers thereafter studied how the human's attachment style can affect the relationship toward the animal (89, 90). For example, dogs (i.e., Canis lupus familiaris) which are aggressive toward humans are less sociable, and have owners who are less emotionally stable, more distant, and less clingy and controlling, compared to non-aggressive dogs (91). Avoidant owners are less responsive to the dog's needs and do not provide a secure-base for the dog when needed, which would result in a higher risk for the dog to develop a separation-related disorder (92). Domestic animals are also likely to show attachment patterns to their owners (9395), but these must be empirically identified. Interestingly, there is research in social neuroscience that identifies neural pathways associated with attachment patterns (96) and how dysfunctional attachment patterns impact interpersonal interactions [see (81, 97) for more details]. Indeed, while the ability of domestic animals to provide a safe-haven and secure-base for humans is well documented (98100), the moderating role of attachment styles of pet owners to the animal is not solely an extension of the general interpersonal attachment patterns that we have identified in humans (101). Additional studies thus appear warranted with regard to specific qualities of the human-animal bond.

In any event, studying the autonomic nervous system when individuals interact together could provide valuable insight on how emotion regulation impacts interspecies relationships. The model of neurovisceral integration (102) postulates that cardiac vagal tone, indexed by heart rate variability (HRV), can indicate the functional integrity of neural networks involved in emotion-cognition interactions. For example, in humans, it has been shown that lower resting HRV is associated with hyper-vigilance and maladaptive cognitive responses to emotional stimuli, thereby impeding emotional regulation (103, 104). Therefore, measuring the autonomic state via HRV is likely to provide insight (1) on the individual's emotional regulation capacity, and (2) on how the autonomic state of interacting individuals evolves according to the expressed emotional states creating coregulation or, on the contrary, codysregulation. Note that the synchronization of heart rates is an appealing notion indicating shared autonomic experiences between individuals; however, the synchronization of heart rates between animals and humans has not been definitively proven (105).

Thus, beyond taking into account the expressed affect, it is also a matter of taking into account the capacities of each individual for emotional regulation. In this sense, actions aimed at preventing risks in HAI do not only consist in preventing stimuli that could frighten an animal or human, but also in increasing their respective capacity to regulate their emotions in the presence of various stimuli. In addition, the goal is to study the manner in which an individual is able to regulate his emotions alone (self-regulation) as well as during an interaction with others (interactive emotional regulation). In particular, in the setting of interspecies interactions, interactive emotional regulation could require considerable adjustments on the part of both humans and animals, justifying a thorough investigation of this process in future research.

Throughout the above section Processes Possibly Involved in Interspecies Communication, a central element appears to stand out through the process of interspecies communication: emotions. The latter could represent the basic unit contained in the “messages” exchanged between individuals, perceived in a conscious manner or not. The emotions could take different channels—olfactory, visual, auditory—allowing their transmission to the other individual and produce different effects on the receiver, preparing him to interact in a phenomenon of reciprocity. In the following section Emotions as a Key Process for the Study of HAI, we propose to detail emotions as a key process for the study of HAI, and in particular the basic or primary emotions that we consider fundamental for designing a model of HAI in a One Health–One Welfare systemic approach.

Emotions as a Key Process for the Study of HAI

Despite an abundance of literature both in biology and psychology on the science of emotion, its definition remains the subject of passionate debate and divergent views. Indeed, emotion as a psychological entity or as a category with clearly-defined boundaries does not exist. The concept of emotion covers a myriad of phenomena involving a diverse array of activation processes and configurations (106). Most taxonomies of human emotions, in agreement with previous models [i.e., (107, 108)], postulate the existence of a fairly small set of basic emotions such as sadness, joy, anger, fear, surprise and disgust (109). The emotions described in these taxonomies are not only recognizable in human faces (110, 111) but also in those of animals such as different primate species (112115), the dog [DogFACS: (116)], the cat [Cat FACS: (117)], or the horse [EquiFACS: (118)]. Thus, there appears to be evolutionary continuity between humans and mammals and birds in the recognition of emotional states (119, 120).

The ability of all animals to experience affective states was the subject of an international consensus at the Francis Crick Memorial Conference on Consciousness in Human and non-Human Animals, recognizing that: “The absence of a neocortex does not appear to preclude an organism from experiencing affective states. Convergent evidence indicates that non-human animals have the neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit intentional behaviors.” (121). In this sense, primary emotions meet the requirement of the “One Biology” concept defined by Tarazona et al. (122), which adjoins and complements those of One Health–One Welfare, implying that biological principles are the same for humans and all other animals, although there are specific differences between species and individuals. Because humans and animals share similar primary emotional states that are recognizable across species, we hypothesize that it is possible to study HAI on the basis of primary emotional states that influence interspecies communication. In particular, we will focus on primary/basic emotions, emotions that are studied with reference to central emotional states, evolutionarily conserved, whose brain activation is identifiable in the brains of a wide variety of animal species (123).

Currently, research remains to be conducted to determine the nature of these central emotional states, some of which, such as fear, could be common to the entire animal kingdom (124126). While awaiting new results in this field and an eventual broad consensus, we have chosen to study the basic or primary emotions common to humans and mammals (and birds), identified and documented in the seminal work of Jaak Panksepp, of which we propose to expose the key elements with, in particular, the description of the seven primary emotional systems, in section Description of the Seven Primary/Basic Emotional Systems. The study of primary emotional systems was first carried out from a comparative perspective between humans and animals, and in this article, we propose to use the latter for the study of interspecies interactions. However, certain authors may consider emotions other than those presented in this article, provided they are identified in both humans and animals.

Description of the Seven Primary/Basic Emotional Systems

According to Panksepp (127129), much of human and animal behavior depends on specific emotional circuits in the brain. These connections not only help organize coherent behavioral responses to important environment challenges, but also provide emotional values to guide everyday decision-making. Primary-process emotional feelings are organized within primitive subcortical regions of the brain including the basal forebrain, diencephalon, midbrain and other brainstem systems, that are anatomically, neurochemically and functionally homologous in all mammals studied1 (129, 131). Mammalian brains contain at least seven basic emotional systems: SEEKING, PANIC, CARE, PLAY, LUST, FEAR and RAGE (capital letters reflect a proposed primary-process terminology by Panksepp, so as to minimize semantic confusions). The anatomical trajectories of these seven higher emotion-generating subcortical systems are wired into the neocortical limbic regions of the brain (43). The primary emotional systems that give rise to emotional states involve various components: neurodynamics (excitation and inhibition), neuromodulators (neurochemicals) as well as neuroanatomical (brain structures and areas) and behavioral aspects. Detailed descriptions of these systems are provided below, as outlined by Panksepp (43, 132, 133), including their primary functions, key brain areas (anatomies), neuromodulators and predetermined key behaviors associated with these systems (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of the seven primary emotional systems identified by Panksepp (71).

The activation of emotional systems emerges from the synthesis of an individual's perception of the environmental interactions that involve the individual personally, which include not only challenges and threats but also the ability to respond to them (22, 135). Primary emotional systems play an important role in the expression of emotions in humans and animals, whether or not they have a subjective perception of these states, and mediate what is commonly referred to as “action tendencies” (136). These systems organize physiological and behavioral responses to prepare the body for an optimal response, creating very broad variations in the responses observed. Thus, emotional expressions reflect the state of an individual's permanent adaptation to constantly changing environmental demands. When primary emotional systems function properly, they promote flexible adaptation to environmental changes (137).

The seven primary emotional systems do not imply that emotional states are limited to seven emotions. Each system has abundant descending and ascending components working together to coordinate various instinctual emotional behaviors and associated autonomic changes, as well as raw affective states (as evaluated by the rewarding and punishing properties of these systems) (129). These primary emotional systems interact with each other and form a basis from which an infinite number of emotional states and expressions can emerge, becoming increasingly complex through secondary processes (learning and memory) and tertiary processes (higher-order functions of the mind associated with cortical expansions enabling thoughts and planning) (138, 139). On the other hand, the perception and processing of information related to primary, secondary and tertiary processes can vary not only from one species to another, but also at the individual level. For instance, in adulthood, the activation of the CARE system, which mediates grooming behaviors in social species, will primarily be activated to provide care to the offspring in so-called solitary species.

The primary emotional systems are distinct and represent a primary affect dedicated to a specific adaptive function. However, they are also highly interconnected with each other. A systemic study of the interconnections between primary emotional systems could be very rewarding in the field of HAI. Indeed, although it is complex to study the interconnection between an individual's emotional systems in real time, we suggest that their study can support metapsychological thinking regarding behavior and generate hypotheses that are important for the development of new study designs.

First, some emotional systems function antagonistically; that is, they activate and inhibit each other, producing bidirectional effects on behavior. When the defensive systems RAGE and FEAR and the separation distress system PANIC are activated, they inhibit the exploration system SEEKING and the social systems CARE, PLAY and LUST in the individual (71, 129). The RAGE, FEAR and PANIC systems can thus be associated, more generally, with negative affects, whereas the SEEKING, CARE, PLAY and LUST systems are associated with positive affects (140).

Second, there are strong interconnections between certain emotional systems. For example, we can cite the existing interconnections between the triad of emotional systems PANIC, CARE and SEEKING, which have already been described at length in the literature by various authors (68, 73, 141143). On the one hand, in social species, the crying of the infant (PANIC system activated) activates the parent's PANIC separation distress system, which in turn activates the parent's caregiving system (CARE system). The PANIC system enables the empathy function in this instance, that of being sensitive to the suffering of the other, and the CARE system enables the response with tender and caring acts (43). The strong interconnection of the PANIC and CARE emotional systems allows the parent and the infant to enter into an emotional connection, contributing to the child's good physical and emotional development. This internal dynamic appears from our point of view as extremely rich in learning, in particular because it conditions an appropriate response during an interaction as well as an emotional connection between two individuals: here, the parent comes to console the infant, to feed him, etc. On the other hand, the separation distress system (PANIC system) and the exploration system (SEEKING system) maintain an important interconnection, where the activation of one inhibits that of the other (144). Indeed, rewarding social interactions cause a strong secretion of endogenous opioids (inhibited PANIC system), which in turn act on the mesolimbic dopamine pathway by stimulating dopamine production in ventral tegmental area neurons, activating the SEEKING system, which generates a state of arousal and motivation to explore the environment and undertake actions (145, 146). As a result, the more the individual feels reassured by the presence of significant others, the more likely he will actively explore the environment (147).

Thus, rather than studying the expression of a single emotional system, we suggest that it is necessary to look at the interconnections between the primary emotional systems of interacting individuals, constituting a complex system. In the following section The PLAY System Involved in Social and Emotional Regulation, we present in more detail the PLAY emotional system, which is of particular interest for the study of HAI, since this system is essentially activated in the context of social interactions and participates in their regulation.

The PLAY System Involved in Social and Emotional Regulation

Social play is a window on cognitive and communicative abilities of species (148, 149). The PLAY system is thought to provide fundamental learning experiences of emotional coregulation leading to higher forms of empathy. A key function of the social PLAY system is to help young animals develop social skills and refine the subtle social interactions needed to thrive (132). In this manner, the PLAY system may be one of the major emotional forces driving the epigenetic construction of higher social brain functions, perhaps even of mirror neurons (150). It may also be the main primary brain process favoring numerous types of social communication (151).

Interspecific play in particular is a fertile venue to explore the capacity to correctly perceive and interpret signals emitted by partners (148). Porges (152) defines interactive play as a neural exercise which requires synchronous and reciprocal behaviors between individuals as well as an awareness of the level of social engagement of each individual. When the PLAY system is activated, play sequences can also activate predetermined action patterns of the emotional systems RAGE and FEAR, sometimes LUST system, without threat or intent to harm (71). The PLAY system appears to be the only emotional system in which mobilization of the body is both activated and inhibited. Through rough-and-tumble play, such as mimicking fights, young mammals would not only learn behavioral patterns that enable them to learn how to flee or defend themselves, but also emotional self-regulation skills (153, 154). Acquisition of these skills would effectively inhibit defensive responses that are inappropriate to the situation. The development of self-regulation would occur through interactive regulation, constructed in settings of dyadic interactions (155). However, given that the PLAY system has strong neural connections with both the FEAR and RAGE systems (71), the animal can easily switch from a play behavior to behavior that poses a risk of injury to humans.

Researchers now consider that play has both immediate and delayed benefits (156, 157). However, the benefits associated with play do not make it a reliable indicator of the presence of a state of wellbeing in the individual. First, in adults, activation of the PLAY system is less intense than in youths. Second, studies in New World monkeys, i.e., Callithrix jacchus, and horses, i.e., Equus callabus, have shown that adults who play the most have the highest stress score (158, 159). Activation of the PLAY system in adults could be used as a coping strategy with the function of releasing aggressive tensions while preserving social bonds, thus promoting emotional resilience (160). The PLAY system could also enhance the individual's ability to be flexible in using different coping strategies according to the characteristics of the situation (161, 162), by enhancing ANS flexibility in humans and animals (163).

In the following section Framework for the Observation and Assessment of Primary Emotions Across Species, we propose a reflection regarding the development of a framework for the observation and assessment of primary emotions during HAI, drawing on a holistic and interdisciplinary approach.

Framework for the Observation and Assessment of Primary Emotions Across Species

To assess the activation of central emotional states or primary emotional systems, it is necessary to develop various methodologies depending on the observed species and the surrounding context. Indeed, in order to be able to study the circularity process inherent to HAI, i.e., the effect that the sender's message or action has on the receiver, and then the impact of the receiver's reaction on the sender, we must be able to identify in the individual, the activation of primary emotional systems and understand their impact on the physiological and behavioral responses observed. Following the principle of One Biology, primary emotions provide a common framework for the study of human and animal behaviors, allowing observations to be made using the same measurement tools. As such, we suggest that the primitive emotion framework put forward by Anderson and Adolphs (123) is relevant for the study and assessment of emotions. The basic emotion features proposed by Anderson and Adolphs (123) (see Figure 1) include (1) valence (emotions are positive or negative), (2) persistence (emotions tend to outlive their trigger), (3) intensity (emotions can be weak or strong), and (4) generalization (the same emotion can occur in different contexts or be triggered by different stimulus conditions). For these authors, all behaviors of living beings, from humans down to the most primitive organism, must fulfill these four characteristics in order to be considered as the expression of an emotion. Figure 1 illustrates the different characteristics and modalities through which emotions are expressed, forming a prism from which it is possible to observe and assess the expression of emotions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Expression of emotions across species.

The assessment of interspecies emotions can be studied either directly in the brain, and/or through different listed modalities. Central emotion states (i.e., Panksepp's primary emotional systems) trigger “action systems” in multiple modalities, notably behavioral, physiological and biochemical, offering numerous possibilities to measure the activation of the primary emotional systems in the brain in an indirect and non-invasive manner. For example, the emotional state of fear can cause eye widening, increased breathing, attentional changes, blood redistribution, hormone release, motor responses such as tonic immobilization or flight, and sustained avoidance (164); another example is activation of the neural circuits associated with the SEEKING system often leads to an increase in approach and exploration locomotor behaviors, scent behaviors, and orientation of gaze and ears toward the object. The behavioral observation of the activation of primary emotional systems can be conducted from the ethogram specific to the observed species.

Moreover, the study of primary emotional systems in interspecies interactions poses even greater challenges. A rigorous holistic view of interspecies emotional communication remains to be established through an interdisciplinary approach including field professionals and researchers from various backgrounds, in order to more extensively explore the biological and behavioral bases of the human-animal emotional relationship. For Scopa et al. (44), one of the keys to accessing the emotional interspecies exchange would be to consider the whole of the autonomic emotional responses with a multimodal, multisensory and multidimensional analysis.

The study of HAI by evaluating the activation of primary emotional systems could eventually allow determining the valence of human-animal interaction but also the function it serves, with the consequences that this interaction produces in terms of wellbeing. Rault et al. (165) propose a series of indicators that allow a holistic analysis of HAI in order to determine the positive valence of the interaction, such as the movement and location of the animal in relation to the human, its expressive behavior, the attitude of the animal toward the human, and the characteristics of the interaction during and after (i.e., initiation of physical contact, frequency or duration of physical contact, gaze, etc.). The authors recommend caution in assessing a positive HAI since, for example, the animal's motivation to interact with humans may be in conflict with other motivations at the time, while some indicators of a positive HAI may be species-specific.

Hence, it is necessary to investigate various methods of biological, physiological, neurological, cognitive and behavioral measurements (166168) in order to characterize valid markers of emotional processes during HAI. However, even before investigating these questions through new empirical research, the eventual undertaking of large-scale meta-analyses could provide a major contribution to the field of HAI, since it would allow cataloging the evidence previously obtained in different disciplines and provide insight into phenomena that remain to be studied.

In the following section The Perception of Safety, an Essential Process for the Study of HAI, we propose to add a complementary process to the study of HAI—the feeling of safety—which we hypothesize modulates the activation of primary emotional systems. Its evaluation would then allow anticipating the behavior of humans and animals in order to prevent risky situations and favor positive interactions.

The Perception of Safety, an Essential Process for the Study of HAI

The concept of safety encompasses the notions of physical and psychological security. It is a perceived safety, which may be more or less removed from the reality of the situation. Feeling safe is recognized as a central component of mental health, wellbeing and post-traumatic growth (72, 169). Conversely, feeling unsafe has been identified as a potentially huge source of distress due to its negative effects on life satisfaction and wellbeing (170, 171). Living in a safe environment is essential for personal and social development and helps predict higher levels of satisfaction with life as a whole (172).

Safety cues alert the organism to when the environment is safe, thereby promoting behaviors that lead to natural rewards, such as feeding and mating, whereas danger cues inhibit these behaviors. Thus, safety signals possess reinforcing properties, allowing the individual to learn to be safe. At the neural level, it has been shown that a safety signal can not only inhibit the output of the amygdala complex, but also reduce the ability of a sensory cue to excite the lateral amygdala (173). As a result, the individual who perceives safety cues will be less likely to be frightened in response to different stimuli. More than the absence of danger, the safety stimulus is associated with the presence of protection from danger. In mice, its presence leads to increased exploration and appetitive behaviors (173, 174), reduces immobility and anhedonia, and is thought to have anxiolytic and antidepressant effects (173, 175, 176). When safety learning is impaired, it can lead to maladaptive behavior, chronic stress and mental disorders. Indeed, since the feeling of safety is subjective, appropriate discrimination between safe and unsafe situations and the subsequent decrease in the expression of fear in the presence of safety cues becomes decisive to survival and mental health (177).

At the Interface of the Environment and the Individual: The Perception of Safety

According to several authors (69, 72, 102), the basic physiological state is co-determined by the perception of safety. This perception of safety directs a mammal or bird to prefer a familiar vs. unfamiliar environment, looking for cues that it is safe from harm (177). According to Porges (178, 179), the feeling of safety arises primarily through a neural process that assesses risk, without awareness and in a reflex manner, based on a variety of signals from the environment and interoceptive stimuli, and triggers changes in the autonomic state to support adaptive responses.

In order for an individual to be socially engaged, i.e., to express positive social behaviors toward others as well as exploratory behaviors, it is necessary for the central nervous system (CNS) to recognize that the subject is in a safe and secure environment by processing sensory information through neural pathways (180). When the individual perceives safety, the individual's defensive mobilization reactions are then inhibited (181). Vagal influences related to the CNS optimally regulate the autonomic state of the body, with the ANS supporting health, growth and restoration. Particularly, the vagus nerve helps slow the heart, inhibits the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, reduces inflammation, and regulates self-protective defensive reactions to maintain an optimal arousal level within a functional energy zone [e.g., optimal autonomic balance between the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and the dorsal vagal influences] (152, 182, 183). The activation of the exploration system (SEEKING system) and social systems such as CARE, PLAY and LUST regulated by vagal influences should help the individual to discover his environment with a sense of safety and pleasure and to engage socially with others (184). Moreover, it can be assumed that by moderating SNS activity, vagal influences should facilitate the activation of the FEAR and RAGE systems without hampering the individual's ability to remain socially engaged.

When, on the contrary, the situation is perceived as unsafe, the ventral vagal function supporting social engagement is dampened or withdrawn. The individual is no longer able to engage in social behaviors and positive interactions with others or in exploratory behaviors. The ANS is optimized to support defense, not health (185). Central vasopressinergic pathways (AVP) change the set-point of the baroreceptor reflex in order to facilitate sympathetic excitation and potentiate mobilization behaviors (180). The various defense responses can be classified into two general forms, namely active defense and immobility (186, 187), and are principally mediated by the FEAR and RAGE systems. These defense reactions can manifest themselves in a chain sequence, called “defense cascade”' (188, 189), with a dominance of the SNS or dorsal vagal influences, according to the degree of danger of the situation perceived by the individual (180) (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Examples of observable behaviors based on three types of autonomic regulation and their consequences for the organism.

In addition, beyond the identification of external factors of danger, such as the presence of a predator, we propose that the FEAR system can be activated on the basis of the absence of safety elements, such as for instance the absence of a shelter to hide, but also of internal stimuli pertaining to bodily sensations and the internal state of the body. For example, the interoceptive information of hunger, thirst, pain and fatigue decreases an individual's ability to cope, making the latter more vulnerable to danger, and will tend to express defensive behaviors (aggression, threats, biting, etc.) toward other individuals (69).

By taking into account the process of safety perception, it becomes possible to anticipate the occurrence of behaviors in two main categories: (1) social and exploratory behaviors, and (2) defensive behaviors. When the situation is perceived as safe, the individual will exhibit social and/or exploratory behaviors, which foster the establishment of positive links between individuals and contribute to the formation of lasting relationships that promote wellbeing. On the contrary, when the situation is perceived as dangerous or life-threatening, the individual will exhibit defensive behaviors such as fleeing, fighting or freezing/feigning death, which increase the risk of injuries and accidents during HAI. When these are repeatedly present, without being appeased during the interaction, they contribute to the establishment of deleterious relationships between individuals, with harmful consequences for their health and wellbeing.

The Feeling of Safety in Social Species

In social species, the absence of external danger is not sufficient to feel fully safe. Inclusion in a social group is an essential survival strategy, in particular for protection from predation as well as for easier access to resources (190, 191). Thus, the emergence of a sense of safety is directly linked to the perception of a meaningful emotional and social presence (192). In this context, memorization of safety cues is likely to help avoid danger and recognize a supportive conspecific [for review, see (193, 194)], and as such is an important motivation for repeated social contact seeking. In keeping with the attachment theory, we hypothesize that the separation distress system (PANIC system) is involved in proximity maintenance, which refers to a constant, non-conscious assessment of the distance (physical or mental) separating us from an individual to whom we attach importance. Indeed, Panksepp and Panksepp (43) consider that the activated PANIC networks (i.e., separation distress system) probably correspond to a form of mental suffering that evolved from pre-existing systems that mediated the affective qualities of physical pain, specifically the thalamocingulate division of the brain—which includes the cingulate cortex and connected medial thalamic nuclei (195). Thus, activated PANIC networks are vectors of mental suffering (associated with a decrease in endogenous opioid levels), which motivates the individual to seek contact with other individuals (130, 196). For example, it has been shown that during social acceptance, activation of the μ-opioid receptor in the left ventral striatum is positively correlated with an increased desire for social interaction (197). It has thus been proposed that the activation of the PANIC circuit (related to experiencing the emotional pain of social loss) contributes to the development of attachment as a major force that guides the construction of social bonds and reflects feelings of emotional deprivation and separation anxiety (136).

As previously defined by Bowlby (198) with the notion of “secure base,” the reunion with conspecifics creates a calming of the PANIC system and participates in the creation of the feeling of a “secure neurochemical base” within the brain, via endogenous opioids and the action of neuromodulators such as oxytocin (71). According to Depue and Morrone-Strupinsky (74), in social species, with the evolution of care and attachment, the oxytocin-opiate system (CARE system) has become a key affect regulation system, whereby there is a co-assembly of different affects such as contentment, feeling of safety and wellbeing, as well as various physiological effects on pain thresholds, the immune system and brain maturation that are partially regulated by these neurohormones (75, 77, 144, 180). Signals and stimuli from CARE system activation, such as stoking, holding, tone of voice, facial expressions, and social support have thus evolved as natural stimuli that inhibit the separation distress system (PANIC system) and have the effect of calming and soothing the receivers (199201). The oxytocin-opioid system (CARE system) is thus particularly linked to soothing, calming, feeling of safety and social connectedness (74, 202, 203).

In humans, in adulthood, the ability to visualize a mental presence, for example knowing that a loved one will soon be found, may be sufficient to maintain the PANIC system stable (204). The system is then regulated by top-down cognitive processing and through self-regulation skills. In so-called “solitary” species, the sense of safety is not dependent on the presence of other conspecifics (134). It is rather the aspects of their vital domain which constitute the vectors of safety such as their shelter and familiarity with their environment (205, 206). These tangible elements are likely to allow the emergence of a neurochemical basis for safety, also mediated by endogenous opioids (207).

Feeling of Safety and At-Risk Prevention Associated With HAI

Understanding the implication of safety perception on the activation of primary emotional systems should foster the prevention of risk situations in HAI. In particular, increasing safety cues in the environment while taking into account the individual's internal signals (pain, fatigue, etc.) should prevent the occurrence of the individual's defensive reactions. The safety signals are to be adapted according to the species, but also to individual factors (age, past experiences, emotional management capacity, etc.). On the basis of the previously mentioned work (see sections Description of the Seven Primary/Basic Emotional Systems and The PLAY System Involved in Social and Emotional Regulation), it is possible to develop the feeling of safety of the animal and the human, both from the elements of the context, but also from the relationship itself, since it appears that the human and the animal have the intrinsic capacity to become a source of safety for each other, as a signifying being or attachment figure.

The identification of the affect associated with the animal's behavior could have many positive consequences in the human-animal relationship. On the one hand, the identification of situations generating panic (PANIC system), fear (FEAR system) and anger (RAGE system), associated with a sympathetic activation, will allow the prevention and a proper response to the activation of these primary emotional systems, and thus reduce incidents and the risk of injury. In addition, the deliberate action of inferring the affect underpinning this behavior could increase the empathy capacities of humans toward animals and allow humans to respond in a manner more adapted to the animal's emotional state, thus strengthening their bonds. Indeed, the capacity for empathy, in both animals and humans, is intended to be a true driver of prosocial behavior, when sensitivity to the distress of others is accompanied by a desire to ensure their wellbeing (208). On the other hand, by promoting the activation of the SEEKING, CARE and PLAY social emotional systems, through an autonomous vagal regulation, it becomes possible to improve the learning and training conditions of animals, in particular through the development of playful learning methods that promote confidence and the experience of hedonic sensations (9). In general, identifying specific emotional states and promoting a sense of safety will improve the management of animals and, consequently, the wellbeing of both the animals and humans.

Synthesis of the Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions

The IMHAI proposes to model human-animal interactions by hypothesizing the primary emotional systems that are activated during the interaction. The context and environment in which HAI occurs plays an indirect role on HAI, while the perception of safety modulates the activation or inhibition of primary emotional systems, expressed through the process of interspecies emotional communication, as illustrated in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Diagram of the integrative model of human–animal interactions.

The IMHAI offers a theoretical framework that can guide us in our observations and the prediction of hypotheses. In practical use, the IMHAI is intended to help both researchers and field professionals relate affect to observed behavior. The modeling of primary emotional systems should allow metareflective activity in the observation of HAI. This modeling can be used to code each observed behavioral sequence, based on video recordings for example, but also allow the possibility of illustrating only selected reactions and behaviors which appear to be the most decisive for understanding HAI. The modeling of HAI must, among other considerations, take into account the stage of evolution of the species studied, its degree of domestication, socialization and familiarity with the other individuals.

To describe and study the interactive sequences between humans and animals, it is first necessary to define the context in which the two individuals interact, notably by attempting to draw an inventory of the safety vs. unsafety cues (external and internal) possibly perceived by the human and the animal. Secondly, the observer notes the behaviors displayed by the two protagonists during the interaction, reporting the valence of the behaviors and mimics observed, and the effect they produce in the respective individuals in terms of neuronal, physiological and behavioral activation. We can then choose to illustrate, as in the case below in Figure 3, a particular moment of the interaction.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Example of hypothetical IMHAI modeling of the interspecies interactive emotional regulation process.

Figure 3 illustrates an example of a hypothetical IMHAI modeling of the interspecies interactive regulation process, integrating the notions of the activation of primary emotional systems presented in section Emotional Contagion and of emotional communication previously defined in section Toward a Systemic One Health-One Welfare Model of HAI. Here, we present a situation in which a human attempts to steer a gregarious animal, e.g., a horse or donkey, away from its conspecifics, using a restraint such as a halter and a lead rope, in order to provide care. In interactive sequence 1: the animal exhibits a state of panic at being separated from its fellow congeners; frightened, it emits distress vocalizations, calling to the others. The animal shakes its head when the human applies pressure on the halter with the lead rope. It next manifests a startle response when it begins to follow the human and then suddenly stops, no longer wanting to move forward. We hypothesize that these reactions are mediated by the PANIC, RAGE, and FEAR defensive systems respectively, with autonomic activation exerted primarily by the sympathetic nervous system (SNS). The animal perceives the situation as dangerous. In sequence 2: in response, the human finally manifests a compassionate attitude. He releases the pressure on the halter and directs his gaze toward the animal, making slow gestures in an attempt to reassure it, and addresses the animal in a gentle tone. The human perceives the situation as safe, the animal's behavior does not diminish his feeling of safety. We hypothesize that the human's behaviors are mediated by social emotional systems (PANIC and CARE systems), with autonomous regulation by the vagus nerve. A slight activation of the separation distress system (PANIC system) is associated with the process of empathy toward the animal while the activation of the CARE system is involved in the generation of benevolent and compassionate behaviors. However, positive and sustained affective bonds must be developed for the animal's PANIC system to remain stable when the human suggests that it move away from its fellow congeners in order to carry out other activities with it. In this context, when the PANIC system remains stable, it is assumed that the human is recognized as an emotionally meaningful partner by the animal.

Application Perspectives of the IMHAI

We propose the IMHAI as a mid-level model serving as a framework for advancing various domains involving HAI. Application of the IMHAI could be extended to the following different settings:

- the modeling of dyadic interactions, such as the interactions between a companion animal and its owner or between a working animal and its owner;

- the modeling of triadic interactions, particularly in the fields of Animal-Assisted Interventions (i.e., patient-animal-therapist) or horseback riding (i.e., rider-horse-teacher);

- analysis of group dynamics generated by the activation and regulation of several primary emotional systems.

In addition, we suggest several examples of possible applications of IMHAI in the fields of HAI, HAR and HAB. With regard to interactions with companion animals, livestock, laboratory, zoo and wild animals (HAI/HAR), the IMHAI could help field professionals identify, through behavioral observation, the emotional processes present in the animal in order to anticipate certain behaviors or responses and potentially prevent accidents and injuries that could occur during interaction with the animal. The training of professionals in the use of training and learning methods based on positive reinforcement by stimulating the activation of the SEEKING exploration system as well as of social emotional systems such as the CARE and PLAY systems guarantees the improvement of working conditions and animal handling. As such, the IMHAI could serve as a basis for teaching purposes and pedagogical concept knowledge, in order to analyze the actions that had contributed to an endangerment or, conversely, led to beneficial outcomes during HAI. Given that the understanding of primary emotional systems is common to both humans and animals, the settings studied may also target the identification of emotional responses in humans which contribute to coregulation or, oppositely, to codysregulation during interspecies interactions.

In addition, application of the IMHAI could help advance the understanding of relationship building (HAR) and bonding (HAB) between humans and animals. This model can be used as a scaffold for the description of the emotional processes occurring during HAI, allowing to reflect both the valence of the interactions (+/–) and the communicational value of the emotion (translating the relationship that individual A wishes or not to establish with individual B), depending on each individual and each animal species observed. In addition, the study of interactive emotional regulation could be investigated through, for example, the measurement of the autonomic state by measuring the heart rate variability of each protagonist. These studies should help to better define the relationships between the activation of the different primary emotional systems. Hence, the study of the PANIC and CARE systems is likely to lead to promising breakthroughs for the understanding of human-animal relationships and the development of attachment bonds.

Understanding the underlying processes present during human-animal interactions has also become a fundamental issue in the field of animal-assisted interventions (AAI). For example, we plan to use the IMHAI to study “patient-animal-therapist” interactions in animal-assisted therapy. A better understanding of emotional processes during HAI should enable: (i) a better understanding of the role of the animal during the intervention in order to promote animal welfare in AAI, in particular through the promotion of mutually beneficial relationships between humans and animals, and also (ii) to more accurately pinpoint the strategies used by the therapist when taking advantage of the relationship with the animal for the purposes of care. This knowledge should ultimately enable advances in the assessment of AAIs, both in terms of effectiveness and efficiency.

Conclusion

Recognizing that animals have emotions and consciousness (121) changes the way we perceive and view these living beings. Such recognition is the cornerstone of a better understanding of human-animal relationships and the development of attachment bonds. Through the process of empathy, we become more receptive to their needs and suffering, while seeking ways to address these needs. An emotional adjustment can then occur, promoting a change in mindset and attitudes toward animals. It is no longer a matter of dominating, coercing or conditioning, but rather of developing a genuine relationship based on trust, respect and reciprocity of exchanges. Thus, emotional communication could enable a true understanding on both sides, decreasing at-risk situations and increasing the establishment of social bonding.

The theoretical contributions integrated in the IMHAI provide a conceptual framework for the study of affect-related behavior and interspecies interactions, promoting their respective wellbeing from a One Health-One Welfare perspective. With this article, we propose a systemic approach to interspecies primary emotions that should lead, on the one hand, to a better understanding of HAI and, on the other, to foster mutually beneficial interspecies relationships. In a first step, we investigated the concept of emotional transfer with the processes of emotional contagion, embodied communication and interactive emotional regulation, in order to establish a systemic approach to primary emotions in a social context. Within the framework of a One Health–One Welfare–One Biology approach, we proposed that emotions are a central process in the study of HAI. We notably focused on the description of the seven primary emotional systems identified by Panksepp (71) and subsequently presenting a framework for observing and assessing emotional states in the individual and during HAI. Finally, from a holistic perspective, we suggest that the feeling of safety should be considered as a main factor, since it would appear to have a crucial impact on the individual's emotional and physiological state. We argue that taking this factor into account is an essential element to prevent at-risk situations in HAI but also to promote the establishment of positive links between humans and animals. However, these concepts require future empirical research to determine whether they are able to reflect the processes at work in HAI.

Given the perpetually evolving nature of scientific knowledge, the IMHAI does not correspond to a fixed theory or model. Its objective is rather to provide a theoretical basis that can serve as a reflection tool for researchers and field professionals from various fields and disciplines, by encouraging their collaboration. Similarly to an incubator, it is our hope that it will foster fruitful reflections and discussions, which may enhance existing models or inspire new methodologies and research designs. The present work aims to provide access to scientific knowledge to field professionals, who are confronted with these issues on a daily basis. As such, the IMHAI can hopefully provide professionals with a concrete and practical framework to reflect on HAI in order to prevent at-risk situations and to promote environments and interspecies interactions that are conducive to the development of positive and sustainable social bonds. Finally, based on the knowledge gathered herein and from our field observations, different hypotheses are proposed for understanding the processes involved in HAI that remain to be investigated and substantiated in future work. To this end, we also suggest that the experience of field professionals be more fully integrated into the research process and the construction of scientific theoretical frameworks for the study of HAI, thereby creating a collaborative platform that could bring together practitioners and researchers from diverse backgrounds around a common theme, namely HAI.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Pierre Pothier for assistance in the translation of the manuscript.

Abbreviations

IMHAI, Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions.

Footnotes

1. ^Using deep brain stimulation (DBS), previous studies (130, 131) have demonstrated associations between different affects and neural circuits. It has thus been shown that DBS in homologous neuronal regions generate consistent and similar emotional action tendencies in all mammals. The fact that animals exhibit rewarding and punitive states (in the sense that animals “voluntarily” turn them on or off) has been interpreted as empirical evidence for emotional content in animals.

References

1. Hosey G, Melfi V. Human-animal interactions, relationships and bonds: a review and analysis of the literature. Int J Comp Psychol. (2014) 27:117–42. doi: 10.46867/ijcp.2014.27.01.01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Waiblinger S, Boivin X, Pedersen V, Tosi MV, Janczak AM, Visser EK, et al. Assessing the human-animal relationship in farmed species: a critical review. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2006) 101:185–242. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Rodriguez KE, Guérin NA, Gabriels RL, Serpell JA, Schreiner PJ, O'Haire ME. The state of assessment in human-animal interaction research. Hum Anim Interact Bull. (2018) 6:63–81.

Google Scholar

4. Shoesmith E, Shahab L, Kale D, Mills DS, Reeve C, Toner P, et al. The influence of human-animal interactions on mental and physical health during the first COVID-19 lockdown phase in the UK: a qualitative exploration. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:976. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18030976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Wells DL. The state of research on human-animal relations: implications for human health. Anthrozoös. (2019) 32:169–81. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1569902

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Janssens M, Eshuis J, Peeters S, Lataster J, Reijnders J, Enders-Slegers MJ, et al. The pet-effect in daily life: an experience sampling study on emotional wellbeing in pet owners. Anthrozoös. (2020) 33:579–88. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1771061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Endo K, Yamasaki S, Ando S, Kikusui T, Mogi K, Nagasawa M, et al. Dog and cat ownership predicts adolescents' mental well-being: a population-based longitudinal study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:884. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17030884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Fraser D, Duncan IJ, Edwards SA, Grandin T, Gregory NG, Guyonnet V, et al. General principles for the welfare of animals in production systems: the underlying science and its application. Vet J. (2013) 198:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2013.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Boissy A, Manteuffel G, Jensen MB, Moe RO, Spruijt B, Keeling LJ, et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol Behav. (2007) 92:375–97. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Mota-Rojas D, Broom DM, Orihuela A, Velarde A, Napolitano F, Alonso-Spilsbury M. Effects of human-animal relationship on animal productivity and welfare. J Anim Behav Biometeorol. (2020) 8:196–205. doi: 10.31893/jabb.20026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Reese LA, Vertalka JJ. Understanding dog bites: the important role of human behavior. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2021) 24:331–46. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2020.1790371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Menor-Campos DJ, Molleda-Carbonell JM, López-Rodríguez R. Effects of exercise and human contact on animal welfare in a dog shelter. Vet Rec. (2011) 169:388–388. doi: 10.1136/vr.d4757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dawkins MS. Through Our Eyes Only? The Search for Animal Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1993).

Google Scholar

14. Swanson JC. Farm animal well-being and intensive production systems. J Anim Sci. (1995) 73:2744–51. doi: 10.2527/1995.7392744x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Duncan I. Animal welfare defined in terms of feelings. Acta Agric Scand Sect Anim Sci. (1996) 1:27.

Google Scholar

16. Fraser D, Duncan I. “Pleasures,” “Pains” and animal welfare: toward a natural history of affect. Anim Welf. (1998) 7:383–96.

Google Scholar

17. Mendl M, Paul ES. Consciousness, emotion and animal welfare: insights from cognitive science. Anim Welf. (2004) 13:17–25.

Google Scholar

18. Yeates JW, Main DC. Assessment of positive welfare: a review. Vet J. (2008) 175:293–300. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Špinka M. Social dimension of emotions and its implication for animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2012) 138:170–81. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Désiré L, Boissy A, Veissier I. Emotions in farm animals: a new approach to animal welfare in applied ethology. Behav Processes. (2002) 60:165–80. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00081-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Mendl M, Burman OH, Paul ES. An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2010) 277:2895–904. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Tcherkassof A, Frijda NH. Les émotions : une conception relationnelle. LAnnee Psychol. (2014) 114:501–35. doi: 10.3917/anpsy.143.0501

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ashby FG, Isen AM A. neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol Rev. (1999) 106:529. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. (2002) 39:281–91. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Mumtaz F, Khan MI, Zubair M, Dehpour AR. Neurobiology and consequences of social isolation stress in animal model—a comprehensive review. Biomed Pharmacother. (2018) 105:1205–22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2018.05.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gilles YD, Polston EK. Effects of social deprivation on social and depressive-like behaviors and the numbers of oxytocin expressing neurons in rats. Behav Brain Res. (2017) 328:28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Atzil S, Gao W, Fradkin I, Barrett LF. Growing a social brain. Nat Hum Behav. (2018) 2:624–36. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0384-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Webb CE, Woodford P, Huchard E. Animal ethics and behavioral science: an overdue discussion. Bioscience. (2019) 69:778–88. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biz082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Mellor DJ, Beausoleil NJ, Littlewood KE, McLean AN, McGreevy PD, Jones B, et al. The 2020 five domains model: including human-animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals. (2020) 10:1870. doi: 10.3390/ani10101870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Luke KL, McAdie T, Smith BP, Warren-Smith AK. New insights into ridden horse behaviour, horse welfare and horse-related safety. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2022) 246:105539. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Glaser Barney G, Strauss Anselm L. The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. New York, NY: Adline Gruyter (1967). doi: 10.1097/00006199-196807000-00014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Lewin K. Resolving social conflicts and field theory in social science. Am Psychol Assoc. (1997) 2:34–46. doi: 10.1037/10269-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Conti LA, Rabinowitz PM. One health initiative. Infektološki Glas. (2011) 31:176–8.

Google Scholar

34. Pinillos RG. One welfare: a framework to improve animal welfare and human well-being. Wallingford: CABI (2018). doi: 10.1079/9781786393845.0000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jordan T, Lem M. One health, one welfare: education in practice veterinary students' experiences with community veterinary outreach. Can Vet J. (2014) 55:1203–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

36. Chalmers D, Dell CA. Applying one health to the study of animal-assisted interventions. Ecohealth. (2015) 12:560–2. doi: 10.1007/s10393-015-1042-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hediger K, Thommen S, Wagner C, Gaab J, Hund-Georgiadis M. Effects of animal-assisted therapy on social behaviour in patients with acquired brain injury: a randomised controlled trial. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42280-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Menna LF, Santaniello A, Todisco M, Amato A, Borrelli L, Scandurra C, et al. The human-animal relationship as the focus of animal-assisted interventions: a one health approach. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:3660. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16193660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Le Moigne JL. La modélisation des systèmes complexes. Paris: Bordas (1990).

Google Scholar

40. Marc E, Picard D. Interaction. Érès (2016). Available online at: https://www.cairn.info/vocabulaire-de-psychosociologie−9782749229829-page-191.htm. doi: 10.3917/eres.barus.2016.01.0191 (accessed December 21, 2021).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Meyza KZ, Bartal IBA, Monfils MH, Panksepp JB, Knapska E. The roots of empathy: Through the lens of rodent models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 76(Pt B):216–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Panksepp J. Empathy and the laws of affect. Science. (2011) 334:1358–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1216480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Panksepp J, Panksepp JB. Toward a cross-species understanding of empathy. Trends Neurosci. (2013) 36:489–96. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Scopa C, Contalbrigo L, Greco A, Lanatà A, Scilingo EP, Baragli P. Emotional transfer in human-horse interaction: new perspectives on equine assisted interventions. Animals. (2019) 9:1030. doi: 10.3390/ani9121030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Debiec J, Olsson A. Social fear learning: from animal models to human function. Trends Cogn Sci. (2017) 21:546–55. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hernandez-Lallement J, Gómez-Sotres P, Carrillo M. Towards a unified theory of emotional contagion in rodents—a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020). Available online at: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0149763420305674 (accessed June 24, 2021).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

47. De Waal FB. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu Rev Psychol. (2008) 59:279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Reimert I, Fong S, Rodenburg TB, Bolhuis JE. Emotional states and emotional contagion in pigs after exposure to a positive and negative treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2017) 193:37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.03.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Palagi E, Celeghin A, Tamietto M, Winkielman P, Norscia I. The neuroethology of spontaneous mimicry and emotional contagion in human and non-human animals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 111:149–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Wenig K, Boucherie PH, Bugnyar T. Early evidence for emotional play contagion in juvenile ravens. Anim Cogn. (2021) 2021:1–13. doi: 10.1007/s10071-020-01466-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lanata A, Nardelli M, Valenza G, Baragli P, D'Aniello B, Alterisio A, et al. A case for the interspecies transfer of emotions: a preliminary investigation on how humans odors modify reactions of the autonomic nervous system in horses. In: 2018 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). (2018). p. 522–5. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2018.8512327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Semin GR, Scandurra A, Baragli P, Lanatà A, D'Aniello B. Inter- and intra-species communication of emotion: chemosignals as the neglected medium. Anim Open Access J MDPI. (2019) 9:110887. doi: 10.3390/ani9110887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. D'Aniello B, Semin GR, Alterisio A, Aria M, Scandurra A. Interspecies transmission of emotional information via chemosignals: from humans to dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim Cogn. (2018) 21:67–78. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1139-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Briefer EF. Vocal expression of emotions in mammals: mechanisms of production and evidence. J Zool. (2012) 288:1–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00920.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Briefer EF. Vocal contagion of emotions in non-human animals. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2018) 285:20172783. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Perez EC, Elie JE, Boucaud ICA, Crouchet T, Soulage CO, Soula HA, et al. Physiological resonance between mates through calls as possible evidence of empathic processes in songbirds. Horm Behav. (2015) 75:130–41. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Quervel-Chaumette M, Faerber V, Faragó T, Marshall-Pescini S, Range F. Investigating empathy-like responding to conspecifics' distress in pet dogs. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0152920. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Sauter DA, Eisner F, Ekman P, Scott SK. Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2010) 107:2408–12. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908239106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Rendall D, Owren MJ. Vocalizations as tools for influencing the affect and behavior of others. In: Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier (2010). p. 177–85. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-374593-4.00018-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Gibson JJ. The theory of affordances. In: Shaw R and Bransford J, editors. Perceiving, Acting and Knowing. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum (1977)

Google Scholar

61. Gallagher S. Inference or interaction: social cognition without precursors. Philos Explor. (2008) 11:163–74. doi: 10.1080/13869790802239227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Gallese V, Rochat M, Cossu G, Sinigaglia C. Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. Dev Psychol. (2009) 45:103. doi: 10.1037/a0014436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Gallese V. The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2003) 358:517–28. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. De Waal FBM, Preston SD. Mammalian empathy: behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2017) 18:498–509. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Grèzes J, Dezecache G. Communication émotionnelle: mécanismes cognitifs et cérébraux. Cogn Soc Neuropsychol. (2012) 2012:31–45.

Google Scholar

66. Duranton C, Gaunet F. Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: overview of its adaptive value. Adapt Behav. (2016) 24:181–91. doi: 10.1177/1059712316644966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Duranton C, Bedossa T, Gaunet F. Pet dogs exhibit social preference for people who synchronize with them: what does it tell us about the evolution of behavioral synchronization? Anim Cogn. (2019) 22:243–50. doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01241-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. Am J Orthopsychiatry. (1982) 52:664. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Brosschot JF, Verkuil B, Thayer JF. Generalized unsafety theory of stress: unsafe environments and conditions, and the default stress response. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:464. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15030464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Harlow HF. The nature of love. Am Psychol. (1958) 13:673. doi: 10.1037/h0047884

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Panksepp J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (2004).

Google Scholar

72. Porges SW. The Polyvagal Theory: Neurophysiological Foundations of Emotions, Attachment, Communication, and Self-Regulation (Norton Series on Interpersonal Neurobiology). New York City: WW Norton & Company (2011).

Google Scholar

73. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss v. 3, Vol. 1. New York, NY: Basic Books (1969).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

74. Depue RA, Morrone-Strupinsky JV A. neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behav Brain Sci. (2005) 28:313–49. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Sheridan JF, McClintock MK. Multilevel integrative analyses of human behavior: social neuroscience and the complementing. Nature. (2000) 126:829. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Schore JR, Schore AN. Modern attachment theory: the central role of affect regulation in development and treatment. Clin Soc Work J. (2008) 36:9–20. doi: 10.1007/s10615-007-0111-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schore AN. The effects of early relational trauma on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J. (2001) 22:201–69. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22:1<201::AID-IMHJ8>3.0.CO;2-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Semin GR, Cacioppo JT. Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, coordination, and co-regulation. In: Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press (2008). p. 119–47. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511805837.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Reed RG, Barnard K, Butler EA. Distinguishing emotional co-regulation from co-dysregulation: an investigation of emotional dynamics and body-weight in romantic couples. Emot Wash DC. (2015) 15:45–60. doi: 10.1037/a0038561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Keeling LJ, Jonare L, Lanneborn L. Investigating horse-human interactions: the effect of a nervous human. Vet J. (2009) 181:70–1. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2009.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. White LO, Schulz CC, Schött M, Kungl MT, Keil J, Borelli JL, et al. A social neuroscience approach to interpersonal interaction in the context of disruption and disorganization of attachment. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:1437. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.517372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Jakob N, Tolmachoff G, Shiota M, Luecken L, Yee C, School School of Life Sciences. Attachment Style as a Moderator in Co-regulation Between Female Friends. Barrett: The Honors College Thesis/Creative Project Collection (2016) (Academic Year 2015-2016). Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/2286/R.I.37368 (accessed July 23, 2022).

Google Scholar

83. Rydell AM, Berlin L, Bohlin G. Emotionality, emotion regulation, and adaptation among 5-to 8-year-old children. Emotion. (2003) 3:30. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Lobo FM, Lunkenheimer E. Understanding the parent-child coregulation patterns shaping child self-regulation. Dev Psychol. (2020) 56:1121–34. doi: 10.1037/dev0000926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Abraham E, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Early maternal and paternal caregiving moderates the links between preschoolers' reactivity and regulation and maturation of the HPA-immune axis. Dev Psychobiol. (2021). doi: 10.1002/dev.22089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Callaghan BL, Tottenham N. The neuro-environmental loop of plasticity: a cross-species analysis of parental effects on emotion circuitry development following typical and adverse caregiving. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2016) 41:163–76. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Shaver PR, Collins N, Clark CL. Attachment Styles and Internal Working Models of Self and Relationship Partners. Milton Park: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. (1996)

Google Scholar

88. Collins NL, Read SJ. Cognitive representations of attachment: the structure and function of working models. In: Attachment Processes in Adulthood, Advances in Personal Relationships, vol. 5. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers (1994). p. 53–90.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

89. Arrazola A, Merkies K. Effect of human attachment style on horse behaviour and physiology during equine-assisted activities-a pilot study. Animals. (2020) 10:1156. doi: 10.3390/ani10071156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Green JD, Coy AE, Mathews MA. Attachment anxiety and avoidance influence pet choice and pet-directed behaviors. Anthrozoös. (2018) 31:475–94. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2018.1482117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Gobbo E, Zupan M. Dogs' sociability, owners' neuroticism and attachment style to pets as predictors of dog aggression. Animals. (2020) 10:315. doi: 10.3390/ani10020315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Konok V, Kosztolányi A, Rainer W, Mutschler B, Halsband U, Miklósi Á. Influence of owners' attachment style and personality on their dogs'(Canis familiaris) separation-related disorder. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0118375. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Prato-Previde E, Custance DM, Spiezio C, Sabatini F. Is the dog-human relationship an attachment bond? An observational study using Ainsworth's strange situation. Behaviour. (2003) 140:225–54. doi: 10.1163/156853903321671514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Topál J, Miklósi Á, Csányi V, Dóka A. Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth's (1969) Strange Situation Test. J Comp Psychol. (1998) 112:219. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Topál J, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Virányi Z, Kubinyi E, Csányi V. Attachment to humans: a comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies. Anim Behav. (2005) 70:1367–75. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Nummenmaa L, Manninen S, Tuominen L, Hirvonen J, Kalliokoski KK, Nuutila P, et al. Adult attachment style is associated with cerebral μ-opioid receptor availability in humans. Hum Brain Mapp. (2015) 36:3621–8. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Long M, Verbeke W, Ein-Dor T, Vrtička P A. functional neuro-anatomical model of human attachment (NAMA): insights from first- and second-person social neuroscience. Cortex. (2020) 126:281–321. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Zilcha-Mano S, Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Pets as safe havens and secure bases: The moderating role of pet attachment orientations. J Res Personal. (2012) 46:571–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Payne E, DeAraugo J, Bennett P, McGreevy P. Exploring the existence and potential underpinnings of dog-human and horse-human attachment bonds. Behav Processes. (2016) 125:114–21. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Kurdek LA. Pet dogs as attachment figures. J Soc Pers Relatsh. (2008) 25:247–66. doi: 10.1177/0265407507087958

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Zilcha-Mano S, Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. An attachment perspective on human-pet relationships: conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment orientations. J Res Personal. (2011) 45:345–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Thayer JF, Lane RD A. model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. (2000) 61:201–16. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Park G, Thayer JF. From the heart to the mind: cardiac vagal tone modulates top-down and bottom-up visual perception and attention to emotional stimuli. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Skibo MA, Sturge-Apple ML, Suor JH. Early experiences of insensitive caregiving and children's self-regulation: vagal tone as a differential susceptibility factor. Dev Psychopathol. (2020) 32:1460–72. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Merkies K, Franzin O. Enhanced understanding of horse-human interactions to optimize welfare. Animals. (2021) 11:1347. doi: 10.3390/ani11051347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Philippot P. Émotion et psychothérapie (2e éd). Belg Mardaga1re Éd 2007. (2011)

Google Scholar

107. Cogan T. A Philosophical Treatise on the Passions. London: S Hazard (1800).

Google Scholar

108. Descartes R. The Passions of the Soul. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing Company (1996).

Google Scholar

109. Ekman P. Basic emotions. Handb Cogn Emot. (1999) 98:16. doi: 10.1002/0470013494.ch3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Ekman P. Emotion in the Human Face 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1982).

Google Scholar

111. Izard CE. Measuring emotions in human development. Meas Emot Infants Child. (1982) 1:3–18.

Google Scholar

112. Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick SJ, Bard KA. Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emot Wash DC. (2007) 7:172–81. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Parr LA, Waller BM, Burrows AM, Gothard KM, Vick SJ. Brief communication: MaqFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaque. Am J Phys Anthropol. (2010) 143:625–30. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Waller BM, Lembeck M, Kuchenbuch P, Burrows AM, Liebal K. GibbonFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for hylobatids. Int J Primatol. (2012) 33:809–21. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9611-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Correia-Caeiro C, Waller B, Zimmermann E, Burrows A, Davila-Ross M. OrangFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for orangutans (Pongo spp.). Int J Primatol. (2012) 34:115–29. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9652-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Waller BM, Peirce K, Caeiro CC, Scheider L, Burrows AM, McCune S, et al. Paedomorphic facial expressions give dogs a selective advantage. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e82686. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Correia-Caeiro C, Waller B, Burrows A. The Cat Facial Action Coding System Manual (CatFACS). Portsmouth: University of Portsmouth (2013).

Google Scholar

118. Wathan J, Burrows AM, Waller BM, McComb K. EquiFACS: the equine facial action coding system. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:131738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Waller BM, Julle-Daniere E, Micheletta J. Measuring the evolution of facial 'expression'using multi-species FACS. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 113:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Filippi P, Congdon JV, Hoang J, Bowling DL, Reber SA, Pašukonis A, et al. Humans recognize emotional arousal in vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates: evidence for acoustic universals. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2017) 284:20170990. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.0990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Low P, Panksepp J, Reiss D, Edelman D, Van Swinderen B, Low P, et al. Cambridge declaration on consciousness in non-human animals. In: Francis Crick Memorial Conference on Consciousness in Human and Non-Human Animals. University of Cambridge, Cambridge (2012).

Google Scholar

122. Tarazona AM, Ceballos MC, Broom DM. Human relationships with domestic and other animals: one health, one welfare, one biology. Animals. (2020) 10:43. doi: 10.3390/ani10010043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Anderson DJ, Adolphs R A. Framework for studying emotions across species. Cell. (2014) 157:187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Fernö A, Folkedal O, Nilsson J, Kristiansen TS. Inside the fish brain: cognition, learning and consciousness. In: Kristiansen TS, Fernö A, Pavlidis MA, van de Vis H, editors. The Welfare of Fish. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2020). p. 149–83.

Google Scholar

125. Temizer I, Donovan JC, Baier H, Semmelhack JL. A visual pathway for looming-evoked escape in larval zebrafish. Curr Biol. (2015) 25:1823–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Zacarias R, Namiki S, Card GM, Vasconcelos ML, Moita MA. Speed dependent descending control of freezing behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat Commun. (2018) 9:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05875-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Panksepp J. Chapter 4—The anatomy of emotions. In: Plutchik R, Kellerman H, editors. Biological Foundations of Emotion. New York, NY: Academic Press (1986). p. 91–124.

Google Scholar

128. Panksepp J. Damasio's error? Conscious Emot. (2003) 4:111–34. doi: 10.1075/ce.4.1.10pan

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Panksepp J. Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Conscious Cogn. (2005) 14:30–80. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Panksepp J. The neurobiology of social loss in animals: some keys to the puzzle of psychic pain in humans. Soc Pain Neuropsychol Health Implic Loss Exclusion. (2011) 2011:11–51. doi: 10.1037/12351-001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Panksepp J. Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals (decoding primal affective experiences of animals). PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e21236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Panksepp J. The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: do animals have affective lives? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:1791–1804. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Panksepp J. Emotional endophenotypes in evolutionary psychiatry. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 30:774–84. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Reser JE. Solitary mammals provide an animal model for autism spectrum disorders. J Comp Psychol. (2014) 128:99. doi: 10.1037/a0034519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Lazarus RS, Launier R. Stress-related transactions between person and environment. In: Perspectives in Interactional Psychology. Berlin: Springer (1978). p. 287–327. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-3997-7_12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience of the emotional BrainMind: evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2010) 12:533. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2010.12.4/jpanksepp

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2009) 33:81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Northoff G, Panksepp J. The trans-species concept of self and the subcortical-cortical midline system. Trends Cogn Sci. (2008) 12:259–64. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Panksepp J, Biven L. The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions (Norton Series on Interpersonal Neurobiology). New York: WW Norton & Company (2012).

Google Scholar

140. Davis KL, Montag C. Selected principles of Pankseppian affective neuroscience. Front Neurosci. (2019) 12:1025. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.01025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Main M. Play, exploration and competence as related to child-adult attachment. Unpubl Dr Dissertation. Johns Hopkins University (1973).

Google Scholar

142. Ainsworth MDS. Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship. Child Dev. (1969) 40:969–1025. doi: 10.2307/1127008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Ainsworth MD. Attachment and Dependency: A Comparison. Washington, DC: APA (1972).

Google Scholar

144. Panksepp J, Watt D. Why does depression hurt? Ancestral primary-process separation-distress (PANIC/GRIEF) and diminished brain reward (SEEKING) processes in the genesis of depressive affect. Psychiatry Interpers Biol Process. (2011) 74:5–13. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2011.74.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Fields H. Understanding how opioids contribute to reward and analgesia. Reg Anesth Pain Med. (2007) 32:242–6. doi: 10.1016/j.rapm.2007.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Van Ree JM, Niesink RJM, Van Wolfswinkel L, Ramsey NF, Kornet MW, Van Furth WR, et al. Endogenous opioids and reward. Eur J Pharmacol. (2000) 405:89–101. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(00)00544-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Miljkovitch R. Théorie de l'attachement. Guérir Souffrances Fam. Paris: PUF (2004).

Google Scholar

148. Maglieri V, Bigozzi F, Riccobono MG, Palagi E. Levelling playing field: synchronization and rapid facial mimicry in dog-horse play. Behav Processes. (2020) 174:104104. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Palagi E, Burghardt GM, Smuts B, Cordoni G., Dall'Olio S, Fouts HN, et al. Rough-and-tumble play as a window on animal communication. Biol Rev. (2016) 91:311–27. doi: 10.1111/brv.12172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Panksepp J. Can PLAY diminish ADHD and facilitate the construction of the social brain? J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2007) 16:57–66.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

151. Panksepp J. Emotional causes and consequences of social-affective vocalization. In: Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier (2010). p. 201–8. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-374593-4.00020-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Porges SW. The Pocket Guide to the Polyvagal Theory: The Transformative Power of Feeling Safe. New York City: WW Norton & Co. (2017).

Google Scholar

153. Hawley CR. Self-handicapping play in mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei): how play stimulates emotional regulation. PhD Thesis. Citeseer (2015).

Google Scholar

154. Flanders JL, Simard M, Paquette D, Parent S, Vitaro F, Pihl RO, et al. Rough-and-tumble play and the development of physical aggression and emotion regulation: a five-year follow-up study. J Fam Violence. (2010) 25:357–67. doi: 10.1007/s10896-009-9297-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Schore AN. Contributions from the decade of the brain to infant mental health: an overview. Infant Ment Health. (2001) 22:1–6. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22:1<1::AID-IMHJ1>3.0.CO;2-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Burghardt GM. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2005). doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3229.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Martin P, Caro TM. On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. In: Advances in the Study of Behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier (1985). p. 59–103. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60487-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Hausberger M, Fureix C, Bourjade M, Wessel-Robert S, Richard-Yris MA. On the significance of adult play: what does social play tell us about adult horse welfare? Naturwissenschaften. (2012) 99:291–302. doi: 10.1007/s00114-012-0902-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Norscia I, Palagi E. When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. Primates. (2011) 52:101–4. doi: 10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Spinka M, Newberry RC, Bekoff M. Mammalian play: training for the unexpected. Q Rev Biol. (2001) 76:141–68. doi: 10.1086/393866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Kato T. Development of the coping flexibility scale: evidence for the coping flexibility hypothesis. J Couns Psychol. (2012) 59:262. doi: 10.1037/a0027770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Roussi P, Krikeli V, Hatzidimitriou C, Koutri I. Patterns of coping, flexibility in coping and psychological distress in women diagnosed with breast cancer. Cogn Ther Res. (2007) 31:97–109. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9110-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. VanFleet R, Faa-Thompson T. Animal Assisted Play Therapy. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press (2017).

Google Scholar

164. Zych AD, Gogolla N. Expressions of emotions across species. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2021) 68:57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Rault JL, Waiblinger S, Boivin X, Hemsworth P. The power of a positive human-animal relationship for animal welfare. Front Vet Sci. (2020) 7:590867. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.590867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2005) 29:469–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Pendry P, Vandagriff JL. Salivary studies of the social neuroscience of human-animal interaction. In: Granger DA, Taylor MK, editors. Salivary Bioscience: Foundations of Interdisciplinary Saliva Research and Applications. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2020). p. 555–81. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-35784-9_23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Belin P, Fecteau S, Charest I, Nicastro N, Hauser MD, Armony JL. Human cerebral response to animal affective vocalizations. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2008) 275:473–81. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Morton L, Cogan N, Kolacz J, Nikolic M, Calderwood C, Bacon T, et al. A new measure of feeling safe: developing psychometric properties of the neuroception of psychological safety scale (NPSS). Open Science Framework. (2021). Available online at: https://osf.io/tpe5j (accessed June 2, 2022).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

170. Valente R, Crescenzi-Lanna L. Feeling unsafe as a source of psychological distress in early adolescence. Soc Sci Med. (2022) 293:114643. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Assari S, Caldwell CH, Zimmerman MA. Perceived neighborhood safety during adolescence predicts subsequent deterioration of subjective health two decades later; gender differences in a racially-diverse sample. Int J Prev Med. (2015) 6:e170431. doi: 10.4103/2008-7802.170431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. González-Carrasco M, Casas F, Ben-Arieh A, Savahl S, Tiliouine H. Children's perspectives and evaluations of safety in diverse settings and their subjective well-being: a multi-national approach. Appl Res Qual Life. (2019) 14:309–34. doi: 10.1007/s11482-018-9594-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Rogan MT, Leon KS, Perez DL, Kandel ER. Distinct neural signatures for safety and danger in the amygdala and striatum of the mouse. Neuron. (2005) 46:309–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Walasek G, Wesierska M, Zielinski K. Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats. Acta Neurobiol Exp. (1995) 55:121–121.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

175. Pollak DD, Monje FJ, Zuckerman L, Denny CA, Drew MR, Kandel ER. An animal model of a behavioral intervention for depression. Neuron. (2008) 60:149–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Meyer HC, Odriozola P, Cohodes EM, Mandell JD Li A, Yang R, et al. Ventral hippocampus interacts with prelimbic cortex during inhibition of threat response via learned safety in both mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2019) 116:26970–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1910481116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Christianson JP, Fernando ABP, Kazama AM, Jovanovic T, Ostroff LE, Sangha S. Inhibition of fear by learned safety signals: a mini-symposium review. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:14118–24. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Porges SW. Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology. (1995) 32:301–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01213.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: New insights into adaptive reactions of the autonomic nervous system. Cleve Clin J Med. (2009) 76:S86–90. doi: 10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. Int J Psychophysiol. (2001) 42:123–46. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00162-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Porges SW. Social engagement and attachment: a phylogenetic perspective. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2003) 1008:31–47. doi: 10.1196/annals.1301.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Ogden P, Minton K, Pain C. Trauma and the Body: A Sensorimotor Approach to Psychotherapy. Norton series on interpersonal neurobiology. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company (2006).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

183. Siegel DJ. The Developing Mind: Toward a Neurobiology of Interpersonal Experience. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1999).

Google Scholar

184. Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic contributions to social behavior. Physiol Behav. (2003) 79:503–13. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00156-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biol Psychol. (2007) 74:116–43. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Roelofs K. Freeze for action: neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2017) 372:20160206. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Vila J, Guerra P, Muñoz MÁ, Vico C., Viedma-del Jesús MI, Delgado LC, et al. Cardiac defense: from attention to action. Int J Psychophysiol. (2007) 66:169–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Lang PJ, Davis M, Öhman A. Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. J Affect Disord. (2000) 61:137–59. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00343-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Schauer M, Elbert T. Dissociation following traumatic stress: etiology and treatment. Z Für Psychol J Psychol. (2010) 218:109–27. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409/a000018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Groenewoud F, Frommen JG, Josi D, Tanaka H, Jungwirth A, Taborsky M. Predation risk drives social complexity in cooperative breeders. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2016) 113:4104–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1524178113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Tomasello M. The ultra-social animal. Eur J Soc Psychol. (2014) 44:187–94. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Hazan C, Zeifman D. Pair bonds as attachments. In: Cassidy J and Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (1999). p. 336–54.

Google Scholar

193. Adolphs R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2003) 4:165–78. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. Olsson A, Phelps EA. Social learning of fear. Nat Neurosci. (2007) 10:1095–102. doi: 10.1038/nn1968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. Eisenberger NI. The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2012) 13:421–34. doi: 10.1038/nrn3231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Panksepp J, Herman BH, Vilberg T, Bishop P, DeEskinazi FG. Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1980) 4:473–87. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Hsu DT, Sanford BJ, Meyers KK, Love TM, Hazlett KE, Wang H, et al. Response of the μ-opioid system to social rejection and acceptance. Mol Psychiatry. (2013) 18:1211–7. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.96

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Bowlby J. A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory (collected papers). Lond Tavistock (1988). p. 134–55.

Google Scholar

199. Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ. Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2001) 42:3–48. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Schore AN. Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. London: Routledge (2015). doi: 10.4324/9781315680019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Uvnäs-Moberg K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (1998) 23:819–35. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00056-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

202. Carter CS. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (1998) 23:779–818.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

203. Løseth G, Leknes S, Ellingsen DM. The neurochemical basis of motivation for affiliative touch. In: Olausson H, Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F, editors. Affective Touch and the Neurophysiology of CT Afferents. New York, NY: Springer (2016). p. 239–64. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-6418-5_15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Zayas V, Günaydin G, Shoda Y. From an unknown other to an attachment figure: how do mental representations change as attachments form? In: Zayas V, Hazan C, editors. Bases of Adult Attachment. New York, NY: Springer New York (2015). p. 157–83.

Google Scholar

205. Hansen JE, Hertel AG, Frank SC, Kindberg J, Zedrosser A. Social environment shapes female settlement decisions in a solitary carnivore. Behav Ecol. (2021) 2021:arab118. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arab118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Tchabovsky AV, Popov SV, Krasnov BR. Intra- and interspecific variation in vigilance and foraging of two gerbillid rodents, Rhombomys opimus and Psammomys obesus: the effect of social environment. Anim Behav. (2001) 62:965–72. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Shapiro LE, Meyer ME, Dewsbury DA. Affiliative behavior in voles: effects of morphine, naloxone, and cross-fostering. Physiol Behav. (1989) 46:719–23. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90357-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

208. Decety J, Bartal IBA, Uzefovsky F, Knafo-Noam A. Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2016) 371:20150077. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: human-animal interactions (HAI), affects, behavior, emotions, affective neuroscience, animal welfare, primary emotional systems, feeling of safety

Citation: Leconstant C and Spitz E (2022) Integrative Model of Human-Animal Interactions: A One Health–One Welfare Systemic Approach to Studying HAI. Front. Vet. Sci. 9:656833. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.656833

Received: 21 January 2021; Accepted: 14 June 2022;
Published: 29 July 2022.

Edited by:

Paolo Baragli, University of Pisa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Kim Drnec, Infoscitex Inc., United States
Oceane Schmitt, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Austria

Copyright © 2022 Leconstant and Spitz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Charlène Leconstant, charlene.leconstant@gmail.com

Download