Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Vet. Sci., 02 August 2023
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare
Volume 10 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2023.1250251

Choice, control, and animal welfare: definitions and essential inquiries to advance animal welfare science

  • 1Animal Welfare Science Program, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, United States

The ways in which humans can support good welfare for animals in their care is an ongoing subject of debate: some place emphasis on the animals’ physical health; others, on animals’ ability to live “natural lives”; and others on animals’ affective states or psychological wellbeing. Recently, there has been an increase in interest in how an animal’s ability to exercise control over its environment could impact their welfare. In this article, we take the stance that the relevant aspects of the first two concepts of animal welfare (physical health, natural lives) are largely addressed when an animal’s psychological wellbeing is prioritized. Through that lens, we review the current state of the literature regarding the psychology of control, and the intersection between choice, control, and welfare. We clarify terms to support future work, and propose future directions that could lead to a better understanding of the psychological benefits of choice and control and ultimately inform animal care decisions.

1. The multidimensional concept of animal welfare

The concept of animal welfare is a complicated one. There is some philosophical controversy about how welfare should be defined, and care for an animal can differ dramatically depending upon which definition of welfare one adopts (1).

Veterinarians and farmers have historically placed emphasis on animals’ physical health in conceptions of welfare [(e.g., 24)]. In other words, animals that were free from disease, injury, and illness, and were able to effectively produce and reproduce were considered to have good welfare. A predominant criticism of this view is that animals could be in excellent physical form but suffer psychologically (57). Others have proposed that the best means by which to promote good welfare is to enable animals to live “natural” lives, that is, by allowing animals to engage in the full range of behaviors that are expressed in their non-managed (wild) environment [(e.g., 810)]. But it has been since been argued that definitions that rely on the expression of natural behaviors are also insufficient [(see 11), or (12) for reviews], because some natural behaviors would be distressful and incongruent with our common sense of good welfare (e.g., a prey animal being chased by a predator), and some unnatural behaviors are rewarding to animals and support good health and positive experiences [e.g., engaging with computer tasks; (13)].

A third perspective, and perhaps the dominant perspective now, focuses on affective states, or “feelings”, of animals as central for determining animal welfare [(e.g., 1, 57, 11, 14)]. Scientists in the “feelings” camp argue that the relevant aspects of the other two definitions (physical health, expressing natural behaviors) would often automatically be addressed if animals’ psychological needs are met, making those definitions are redundant. In other words, an animal that is wounded or ill would likely experience negative affective states, and therefore experience poor welfare (7). Similarly, others argue that so-called natural behaviors have little connection to welfare in and of themselves; any welfare impact stems from the effectiveness of behaviors (natural or otherwise) in promoting positive mental states (11, 15). Arguably the most prominent current model of animal welfare is the Five Domains Model, which considers health and behaviors relevant to an animal’s welfare state are dependent upon how the mental domain interprets or processes them (16, 17). We share the view that animals’ psychological states are the most important factor for determining welfare, and we propose that providing captive animals with opportunities to exercise control (i.e., via choices) is an excellent mechanism by which to improve psychological wellbeing and enhance welfare.

2. The intersection between choice, control, and welfare

Dawkins (18, 19) proposed that the simplest way to ensure that animals experience good welfare is to keep them healthy and give them what they want. Providing animals with choices seems a straightforward way to satisfy these requirements. First, providing choices can directly positively impact animals’ affective states by giving animals a sense of control and agency, which is imperative for psychological and biological health [(see 20) for a review]. Second, theoretically, providing choices minimizes the role of human caregivers as deciders of what it is that animals want, instead allowing animals themselves to seek out their preferred alternatives (13, 19, 21). But before we can discuss these ideas further, we must first consider what is meant by choice and control.

2.1. The difference between choice, control, and related constructs

To set the stage for empirical investigation of the relationship between choice and welfare, there is a need to disentangle some related constructs (Figure 1). We define choice as the act of choosing or selecting from more than one alternative (22). Control is the ability to predictably produce desired results – i.e., to be the cause that instantiates an expected effect (23) – and is related to the concept of autonomy (24, 25). It follows that choice is a main mechanism by which individuals can exert control over their environment (20). A similar, related construct is that of challenge. A challenge is an opportunity that requires the use of some skill and focused attention in order to achieve a goal, and has been argued to promote an animal’s sense of competence (2628). Autonomy (i.e., the ability to exercise control) and competence (i.e., the ability to overcome challenge) are considered innate psychological needs for humans (24, 25), and from an evolutionary perspective, it follows that these needs should also be satisfied for positive psychological wellbeing in nonhuman animals who, like humans, evolved to be functional agents in unpredictable environments (20, 27).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual definitions of choice, control, challenge, and agency. The organism/individual is represented by gray triangles, potential actions of the organism are represented by black arrows, and potential outcomes are represented by black location markers. The dashed lines represent (unpredictable) environmental factors, and the solid black circles represent obstacles (physical or mental).

Specifically, we propose that control and (surmountable) challenge are necessary for an animal to experience agency. Agency has been defined as the ability to successfully engage with the environment beyond satisfying immediate needs – that is, by achieving goals, developing skills, acquiring information, and pursuing future plans (26, 27). Agency is relevant to welfare because engaging in such intrinsically-motivated behavior is inherently rewarding (24, 28). Furthermore, building competency through agency allows for better coping with later difficult situations (26), and the ability to cope with the environment is directly related to an animal’s welfare state (4). But individuals only reap the benefits of competence if it is accompanied by a perceived “internal locus of causality” (i.e., control) over the situation at hand (24, 25, 29). Animals would likely have little motivation to engage in any sort of challenge if they had no control over the environment around them (20).

In interim summary, to promote positive welfare in animals, we must strive to increase the perception of agency. Špinka (26) and Špinka and Wemelsfelder (27) have provided excellent explanations for why challenge is integral to agency and necessary for good welfare. We argue that another critical component of agency is the ability to exert control over one’s environment through choices. Many studies, described below, have demonstrated the psychological relevance of environmental control, although fewer have investigated the direct welfare implications of providing choice to animals.

2.2. Control as a psychological imperative

Leotti et al. (20) argued that perceiving control over one’s environment is evolutionarily adaptive because increased control over the environment will improve an animal’s chance of survival. When faced with choices, animals are likely to choose options that provide them with the most benefit and avoid options that may cause harm. Leotti et al. (20) proposed that making choices is the main mechanism by which animals can exercise such control, and that having access to choice has evolved to become a biological imperative. The authors reviewed evidence that control is critical for typical, healthy development, and that a diminished sense of control can lead to maladaptive behaviors and reduced wellbeing (20).

Although Leotti et al. (20) discuss the importance of choice in enabling animals to experience control, the majority of research on control has investigated not choice behavior, per se, but the ability to start or stop aversive or rewarding stimuli. For example, Seligman and Maier (30) demonstrated that dogs who were repeatedly exposed to inescapable shock later did not attempt to escape electric shock even when it was avoidable, a phenomenon dubbed as “learned helplessness”, which has since been evidenced in a wide variety of species, from flies (31) to humans (32). Additionally, Hanson et al. (33) found that monkeys who could press a lever to avoid an unpleasant noise exhibited similar plasma cortisol levels as monkeys who were never exposed to the noise, whereas monkeys who did not have control over the noise exhibited elevated cortisol levels. Research on the positive effects of controllability over rewarding stimuli have also been found: Buchanan-Smith and Badihi (23) demonstrated that marmosets experienced improved welfare (as evidenced by positive activity patterns and scent marking) in the presence of supplementary light and/or heat, and that these effects were further enhanced when marmosets were given control over the light/heat source.

Research that has investigated control via choice, rather than stimulation management, has demonstrated that many species of animals prefer having two options available rather than one, even where there was no difference in reward [e.g., pigeons, (34); rats, (35); and monkeys, (36)], and that providing choices improves motivation and performance. For example, Washburn et al. (37) found that rhesus monkeys performed better on computer tasks when they were able to choose the tasks compared their performance on those same tasks when they were assigned. Beran et al. (38) found that capuchin monkeys demonstrated improved task performance when the monkeys only chose the order in which they completed tasks (not the tasks themselves) and even when the assigned order was yoked to match animals’ established preferences. Interestingly, the monkeys who demonstrated improved performance in the Beran et al. (38) study did so even though they had access to enriched living arrangements which already provided them with ample opportunities for choice and control.

So far, we have discussed the literature that makes clear that control is essential for positive psychological welfare, and that making choices is a means by which to exercise control. Less is known about the direct link between the provision of choice and psychological wellbeing in animals, though the research is promising. Badihi (39) found that increase in complexity, choice, and control resulted in decreases in undesirable behaviors (e.g., agitated locomotion, and scratching) and increases desirable behaviors (e.g., calm locomotion, exploration, and play) for marmosets. Kurtycz et al. (40) demonstrated that gorillas and chimps exhibited evidence of more positive affective states when given a choice between indoor and outdoor habitats compared to when no choice was given. Similarly, Ross (41) found that polar bears demonstrated decreased stereotypic behaviors and increased social play when they were provided access to private den areas compared to when no such option was provided. The polar bears used the dens only 2% more of the time when they were accessible, suggesting that the welfare benefits were a byproduct of the provision of choice rather than the dens themselves. And, research with humans has shown that anticipating the opportunity to make a choice engages reward-related circuitry in the brain (42).

There is also indirect evidence from choice studies that suggests the provision of choice has potential welfare benefits. Specifically, several studies have demonstrated that making choices serves as a primary reinforcer, such that animals enjoy making choices for the sake of making choices. Perdue et al. (43) found that, given the choice between two icons, one which allowed the monkeys to choose the order in which to complete tasks, and another which would force them to complete the tasks according the order assigned by the computer, monkeys preferred to choose the order in which to complete the tasks. Monkeys maintained this preference for choice even when the computer-assigned order was yoked to match their previous choice selections. In other words, the monkeys preferred making choices even when making such a choice did not lead to greater reward or more preferred outcomes than the non-choice scenario. Perdue and Brown (44) also found that monkeys chose less-preferred alternatives simply to keep those options available. Monkeys could choose between two icons that provided different levels of risk but the same overall rate of reward. After each choice, the unselected icon shrank in size until disappearing entirely (whereas choosing the icon returned it to full size). Perdue and Brown (44) found that capuchin monkeys would periodically choose their less-preferred icon to keep it from disappearing entirely. Together, this research speaks to the intrinsic value of having access to choices that goes beyond the value of the options themselves.

Finally, as Dawkins noted, asking animals what they want is key to supporting good welfare (18, 19). Even if choices were not rewarding in their own right, giving animals options would allow them to choose preferred alternatives, and tell us what things are most necessary or beneficial for their own wellbeing. After all, preferences can change over time (45) and options may still be valuable to animals even if they are not actively being used (21, 40, 41). This idea was nicely summed up by Washburn (13): “Rather than making assumptions about the conditions that will promote psychological wellbeing, we should endeavor to give the animals choices that will indicate those conditions… Allowing animals to indicate their preferences by their behavior provides the clearest and most defensible standard for determining environmental enrichment”.

3. Future directions

With concepts clearly defined and an understanding of what we have learned so far, some high priority research questions emerge. Making progress on the questions below will help lay the foundation for incorporating choice into animal care in a way that promotes good animal welfare.

3.1. What is the most beneficial frequency, abundance, and duration of choices?

Although the literature clearly demonstrates that at least some choice is more desirable and leads to better psychological outcomes than no choice, the relative influence of increasing the number or duration of such choice is less clear [though (see 39)]. The law of diminishing marginal utility (46) would suggest that each additional alternative would be less valuable than the last, and research with humans has indicated that too many choices can lead to suboptimal outcomes (47, 48). It is unclear whether other animals may be susceptible to choice overload (49) or would experience a similar diminished marginal utility for a greater number of alternatives. A fruitful avenue of future research may be to investigate whether there may some optimal number of alternatives (at least in some contexts or for some species), where the perception of control is maximized but the increased number of alternatives does not become trivial or overwhelming. Furthermore, it will be important to investigate the possible consequences of removing choice for a duration of time after it has been established, as this is a practical likelihood for animals in human care.

3.2. How should natural history inform what and how choices are provided?

The majority of research on choice and control so far has been conducted with primates. Although we hypothesize (and evidence supports) that all animals would reap benefits from being functional agents in their environment, the psychological impact of choice may differ from species to species. Most primates evolved to live in groups with complex social dynamics, consume varied diets, and inhabit heterogenous environments. These traits could make primates especially sensitive to the availability of choice in their environment; or, it is possible that we are better prepared to consider the value of choice for primates due to a bias in our science and publication history (50, 51). Certainly, though, natural history must be considered in how and what choices are provided, as well as in how “good welfare” would be measured (52). More research on choice behavior in non-primate species, particularly understudied species such as reptiles, amphibians, fish, or invertebrates, is needed, as well as strategies for interpreting their responses (19).

3.3. How do we measure the benefit of unselected choices?

Generally, the value of alternatives has been measured based on the active use of the chosen alternative. Only recently [(e.g., 21)] have scientists begun asking what the value could be of alternatives that are not actively being used. Research suggests that choices are valuable even when they aren’t chosen or aren’t preferred [(e.g., 40, 41, 43)]. However, there are practical constraints to the number of choice opportunities that can be provided to animals in human care. Using work-for-access paradigms could shed some light on the relative value of differing alternatives. For example, a study on American minks showed that the animals spent 300% more time in nest boxes than swimming baths when both options were available (53), yet the animals worked much harder for access to their swimming bath than the nest box, and only showed increased cortisol levels when being denied access to the bath, not the nest box (54). Incorporating more research that requires animals to work for access to certain options (or to having the opportunity to make a choice at all) could help us answer questions about when choice is most important, which options are most valuable to individual animals, how those values change over time or in different contexts.

4. Conclusion

In this article, we have reviewed the literature on welfare, choice, and control, and discussed the ways in which these topics intersect. The majority of research has investigated the role of control on psychological wellbeing via stimulation management. However, we also know that making choices is a means by which to exercise control, and that studies on choice behavior have indicated that making choices can have a direct positive impact on welfare. Yet gaps in the literature remain, including how non-primates are affected by choice opportunities, the impact of the duration of choice or number of options available, and the benefit of unselected alternatives. We are optimistic that providing choices to captive animals will continue to prove an excellent strategy to improve their welfare while also enabling us to learn about their preferences, choice behavior, and cognition more generally.

Author contributions

ME and KC developed the concept of the manuscript and reviewed and revised the article. ME wrote the first draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Lincoln Park Zoo.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank our colleagues at Lincoln Park Zoo and Georgia State University for interesting discussions that led to the generation of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Fraser, D. Understanding animal welfare. Acta Vet Scand. (2008) 50:S1. doi: 10.1186/1751-0147-50-S1-S1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Barnett, JL, and Hemsworth, PH. The validity of physiological and behavioural measures of animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1990) 25:177–87. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(90)90079-S

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Barnett, JL, Hemsworth, PH, Cronin, GM, Jongman, EC, and Hutson, GD. A review of the welfare issues for sows and piglets in relation to housing. Aust J Agric Res. (2001) 52:1. doi: 10.1071/AR00057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Broom, DM. Indicators of poor welfare. Br Vet J. (1986) 142:524–6. doi: 10.1016/0007-1935(86)90109-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Duncan, IJ. A concept of welfare based on feelings In: GJ Benson and BE Rollin, editors. The well-being of farm animals: Challenges and solutions. Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Oxford, UK: John Wiley & Sons (2008). 85–101.

Google Scholar

6. Duncan, IJH. Poultry welfare: science or subjectivity? Br Poult Sci. (2002) 43:643–52. doi: 10.1080/0007166021000025109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Duncan, IJ, and Petherick, JC. The implications of cognitive processes for animal welfare. J Anim Sci. (1991) 69:5017–22. doi: 10.2527/1991.69125017x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Farm Animal Welfare Council. (1979). Farm animal welfare council press statement. Available at: https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/ukgwa/20121010012427/http://www.fawc.org.uk/freedoms.htm

Google Scholar

9. Kiley-Worthington, M. Ecological, ethological, and ethically sound environments for animals: toward symbiosis. J Agric Ethics. (1989) 2:323–47. doi: 10.1007/BF01826810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rollin, BE. Animal welfare, science, and value. J Agric Environ Ethics. (1993) 6:44–50.

Google Scholar

11. Browning, H. The natural behavior debate: two conceptions of animal welfare. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2020) 23:325–37. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2019.1672552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mellor, D. Positive animal welfare states and reference standards for welfare assessment. N Z Vet J. (2015) 63:17–23. doi: 10.1080/00480169.2014.926802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Washburn, D. The four Cs of psychological wellbeing: lessons from three decades of computer-based environmental enrichment. Animal Behav Cogn. (2015) 2:218–32. doi: 10.12966/abc.08.02.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Veasey, JS. In pursuit of peak animal welfare; the need to prioritize the meaningful over the measurable. Zoo Biol. (2017) 36:413–25. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Cronin, KA, and Ross, SR. When is “natural” better? The welfare implications of limiting reproduction in chimpanzees In: LM Hopper and SR Ross, editors. Chimpanzees in context. Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press (2020). 509–23.

Google Scholar

16. Mellor, DJ. Operational details of the five domains model and its key applications to the assessment and management of animal welfare. Animals. (2017) 7:60. doi: 10.3390/ani7080060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mellor, DJ, and Beausoleil, N. Extending the “five domains” model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim Welf. (2015) 24:241–53. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.3.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Dawkins, M. Using behavior to assess animal welfare. Anim Welf. (2004) 13:S3–7. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600014317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dawkins, MS. The science of animal welfare: understanding what animals want. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2021).

Google Scholar

20. Leotti, LA, Iyengar, SS, and Ochsner, KN. Born to choose: the origins and value of the need for control. Trends Cogn Sci. (2010) 14:457–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Decker, S, Lavery, JM, and Mason, GJ. Don’t use it? Don’t lose it! Why active use is not required for stimuli, resources or “enrichments” to have welfare value. Zoo Biol. (2023):1–9. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Merriam-Webster. Choice. In Mirriam-Webster.com. (2023). Available at: https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/choice

Google Scholar

23. Buchanan-Smith, HM, and Badihi, I. The psychology of control: effects of control over supplementary light on welfare of marmosets. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2012) 137:166–74. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Deci, EL, and Ryan, RM. Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY USA: Plenum Press (1985).

Google Scholar

25. Ryan, RM, and Deci, EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. (2000) 55:68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Špinka, M. Animal agency, animal awareness and animal welfare. Anim Welf. (2019) 28:11–20. doi: 10.7120/09627286.28.1.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Špinka, M, and Wemelsfelder, F. Environmental challenge and animal agency In: MC Appleby, JA Mench, IAS Olsson, and BO Hughes, editors. Animal welfare. 2nd ed. Wallingford, Oxfordshire: CABI (2011). 27–43.

Google Scholar

28. White, RW. Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychol Rev. (1959) 66:297–333. doi: 10.1037/h0040934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. deCharms, R. Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. Cambridge, MA, USA: Academic Press (1968).

Google Scholar

30. Seligman, ME, and Maier, SF. Failure to escape traumatic shock. J Exp Psychol. (1967) 74:1–9. doi: 10.1037/h0024514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Batsching, S, Wolf, R, and Heisenberg, M. Inescapable stress changes walking behavior in flies—learned helplessness revisited. PLoS One. (2016) 11:e0167066. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0167066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hiroto, DS, and Seligman, ME. Generality of learned helplessness in man. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1975) 31:311–27. doi: 10.1037/h0076270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hanson, JD, Larson, ME, and Snowdon, CT. The effects of control over high intensity noise on plasma cortisol levels in rhesus monkeys. Behav. Biol. (1976) 16:333–40. doi: 10.1016/S0091-6773(76)91460-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Catania, C, and Sagvolden, T. Preference for free choice over forced choice in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. (1980) 34:77–86.

Google Scholar

35. Voss, SC, and Homzie, MJ. Choice as a value. Psychol Rep. (1970) 26:912–4. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1970.26.3.912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Suzuki, S. Selection of forced- and free-choice by monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Percept Mot Skills. (1999) 88:242–50.

Google Scholar

37. Washburn, DA, Hopkins, WD, and Rumbaugh, DM. Perceived control in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): enhanced video-task performance. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. (1991) 17:123–9. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.17.2.123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Beran, M, Klein, E, Evans, T, Antworth, R, and Chan, B. Perceived control, motivation, and task performance in capuchin monkeys In: PR Zelick, editor. Issues in the psychology of motivation. Hauppauge, NY, USA: Nova Science Publishers, Inc. (2007). 173–87.

Google Scholar

39. Badihi, I. The effects of complexity, choice and control on the behaviour and the welfare of captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) University of Stirling (2006).

Google Scholar

40. Kurtycz, LM, Wagner, KE, and Ross, SR. The choice to access outdoor areas affects the behavior of great apes. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2014) 17:185–97. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2014.896213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ross, SR. Issues of choice and control in the behaviour of a pair of captive polar bears (Ursus maritimus). Behav Process. (2006) 73:117–20. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Leotti, LA, and Delgado, MR. The inherent reward of choice. Psychol Sci. (2011) 22:1310–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Perdue, BM, Evans, TA, Washburn, DA, Rumbaugh, DM, and Beran, MJ. Do monkeys choose to choose? Learn Behav. (2014) 42:164–75. doi: 10.3758/s13420-014-0135-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Perdue, B, and Brown, E. Irrational choice behavior in human and non-human primates. Anim Cogn. (2018) 21:227–34. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1156-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fultz, A, Yanagi, A, Breaux, S, Beaupre, L, and Naitove, N. How sanctuary chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) use space after being introduced to a large outdoor habitat. Animals. (2023) 13:961. doi: 10.3390/ani13060961

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gossen, HH. The laws of human relations and the rules of human action derived therefrom. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1983).

Google Scholar

47. Iyengar, SS, and Lepper, MR. When choice is demotivating: can one desire too much of a good thing? J Pers Soc Psychol. (2000) 79:995–1006. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Schwartz, B. The paradox of choice: Why more is less. New York City, NY USA: HarperCollins Publishers (2004).

Google Scholar

49. Englund, MD, and Beran, MJ. No evidence of the choice overload effect in a computerized paradigm with rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella). Behav Process. (2022) 194:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Binding, S, Farmer, H, Krusin, L, and Cronin, K. Status of animal welfare research in zoos and aquariums: where are we, where to next? J Zoo Aquar Res. (2020) 8:3. doi: 10.19227/jzar.v8i3.505

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Rosenthal, MF, Gertler, M, Hamilton, AD, Prasad, S, and Andrade, MCB. Taxonomic bias in animal behaviour publications. Anim Behav. (2017) 127:83–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.02.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Watters, JV, Krebs, BL, and Eschmann, CL. Assessing animal welfare with behavior: onward with caution. J Zoo Bot Gard. (2021) 2:75–87. doi: 10.3390/jzbg2010006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Cooper, JJ, and Mason, GJ. Increasing costs of access to resources cause re-scheduling of behaviour in American mink (Mustela vison): implications for the assessment of behavioural priorities. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2000) 66:135–51. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00069-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Mason, GJ, Cooper, J, and Clarebrough, C. Frustrations of fur-farmed mink. Nature. (2001) 410:35–6. doi: 10.1038/35065157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: choice, control, animal welfare, agency, psychological wellbeing, affective states

Citation: Englund MD and Cronin KA (2023) Choice, control, and animal welfare: definitions and essential inquiries to advance animal welfare science. Front. Vet. Sci. 10:1250251. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2023.1250251

Received: 29 June 2023; Accepted: 17 July 2023;
Published: 02 August 2023.

Edited by:

Serenella D’Ingeo, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Italy

Reviewed by:

Damiano Cavallini, University of Bologna, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Englund and Cronin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maisy D. Englund, mbowden.englund@gmail.com

Download