Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 04 April 2024
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare
Volume 11 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2024.1374858

How does chronic pain impact the lives of dogs: an investigation of factors that are associated with pain using the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid

Rachel Malkani* Sharmini Paramasivam Sarah Wolfensohn
  • School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom

Introduction: Chronic pain can profoundly affect the wellbeing of dogs and our understanding is limited regarding the multidimensional impact it has on dog quality of life. This study aimed to assess the factors that are significant and predictive of chronic pain in dogs using the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid (AWAG) to further understand what factors influence their welfare.

Methods: Seventy six AWAG assessments were undertaken across 46 dogs that clinicians diagnosed as having musculoskeletal conditions that caused chronic pain. Wilcoxon-rank sum tests were used to assess the difference in scores between dogs with chronic pain and a cohort of healthy dogs (n = 143).

Results: All physical factors besides body condition, and all psychological, environmental, and procedural factors were significantly different between healthy dogs and dogs with chronic pain, evidencing how chronic pain impacts all domains of a dog’s life. Spearman Rank Correlation Coefficient (RS) revealed several significant strong positive correlations such as the association between the severity of clinical symptoms with poorer mobility and the frequency at which the dog experienced fearful stimuli. Logistic regression showed that fears and anxieties frequency, the dog’s reaction to stressors, engagement with enrichment, and social interactions were significant predictors of chronic pain in dogs.

Discussion: This highlights that typical signs of musculoskeletal disorders such as gait changes, stiffness, lameness might manifest after behavioral changes such as increased fearfulness, prolonged recovery from a stressful event, a reduced interested in social interactions, toys or play. Owners only seeking veterinary attention when the presence of physical signs of disease are evident may result in a delayed veterinary attention resulting in reduced welfare. Regular veterinary assessments combined with use of the AWAG can proactively identify these behavioral indicators and result in prompt treatment and improved quality of life.

Introduction

Pain is defined as an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage; or an aversive sensory and emotional experience typically caused by, or resembling that caused by, actual or potential tissue injury (1).

Pain may be classified in a range of ways according to its duration (acute, chronic, or intermittent) and severity (mild, moderate, severe, excruciating) (2). Chronic pain is described as pain that has persisted for longer than 3–6 months and may have several components. The inciting cause may or may not be present (3). Reorganization of cortical neurotransmitters occurs after injury and this can induce central sensitization, hyperalgesia, and allodynia (4, 5). Changes in the brain as a result of chronic pain can impact cognitive and emotional health, resulting in poor welfare states (6, 7).

Chronic pain does not seem to serve any useful purpose for an animal besides having a protective function, and may be very difficult to recognize behaviorally (2). Chronic pain is notoriously difficult to assess and quantify; the pain that a patient with osteoarthritis might exhibit may bear no resemblance to the radiographic features of the disease (3, 8). However, it is important to prioritize objective assessments aimed at evaluating the quality of life impact of chronic pain, as opposed to solely quantifying the severity of the primary condition.

A well-recognized indication of both acute and chronic pain in dogs is behavior change (9, 10). Reported signs include a general reduction in activity levels, change in appetite, reduced sleep quality and change of sleeping position, altered posture and gait changes that may include stiffness or lameness. These changes are mapped out on the physical parameter of the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid (AWAG) (11). Psychological and environmental factors also included in the AWAG that are reported in the literature to be indicative of pain include aggression, personality changes, reduced sociability and play, and reluctance or refusal to perform normal behaviors (2, 3, 10, 12, 13). Many pain scoring tools include these examples of behavior change coupled with facial expressions to score the severity of pain (1416).

There have been many scales developed to quantify the severity of acute pain. However, there is no gold standard as the experience is unique to the individual and because of its multidimensional nature. This presents a challenge in assessing the affective component of pain as ideally, a comprehensive evaluation needs to consider physiological, psychological, endocrine, immune, and behavioral measures (17), and this can be further compounded by the assessor’s bias. Multidimensional scales that have been developed for use in dogs include the Glasgow Composite Measure Pain Scale (CMPS) (18, 19), the University of Melbourne Pain Scale (20, 21), and the Colorado State University Canine Acute Pain Scale (22).

In canine chronic pain, studies have highlighted that the owner is the preferred proxy because behavioral changes may be so subtle and gradual in onset that they are apparent only to someone very familiar with the individual dog and these subtle behavioral changes may not be obvious to veterinary professionals in a clinical setting where they may be masked by fear, excitement, or anxiety associated with the unfamiliar environment (10). Therefore, caregiver reports regarding behavioral change are likely to be highly beneficial when assessing chronic pain in a dog. However, owners are reported to focus on movement-based behavior changes in their dogs and have difficulty associating behavioral changes with pain in the older dogs (23), while other studies have shown that when using structured chronic pain assessment tools, owner’s may not be able to identify behavioral changes associated with pain in their dogs (24). Additionally, there is also bias in veterinary assessments of pain. 90% of veterinarians and the public believe that larger breed dogs have less sensitivity to pain, despite there being no physiological basis for this (25). Moreover, female veterinarians are reported to give higher pain scores compared to males (26). Therefore, conducting assessments based on both owners and veterinary input, may provide the most accurate data using chronic pain assessment tools.

As pain can inhibit normal behaviors, not only can the emotional experience of physical pain affect quality of life, but dogs may become frustrated if they cannot perform behaviors as normal. This may be as a result of restrictions imposed by the owner, or the physical inability of the dog to perform these. Frustration occurs when an animal’s expectations and desires are not met and is considered a negative emotional state and potential welfare concern (27).

Dogs that have reduced mobility due to pain and increasing age may result in a decline in owner positive attitude toward their dog and a reduction in the amount of time the owner spends together with their dog (28), which can be frustrating and damaging to the welfare of the dog. In younger dogs with decreased mobility, frustration may be experienced with higher intensity to due increased energy and desire to play, run, and interact socially with other dogs and people.

Monitoring chronic pain using diagnostic tools alone can be problematic. Using physiological parameters as a stand-alone measure risks treatments being directed at controlling and improving that parameter rather than examining what is impacting the animal’s quality of life related to the condition and how to improve it (29).

Pain can be an important consideration in many medical conditions and often has an impact on diagnostic and treatment decision making. Pain is the primary concern in dogs with osteoarthritis and other disorders that cause lameness. Conditions affecting the elbow joint are a common cause of lameness in both young and older dogs; the canine elbow can be affected by several different diseases, including elbow dysplasia, humeral intracondylar fissures, congenital luxations, soft-tissue problems, and septic arthritis (30). Each of these diseases usually results in lameness, joint pain, and reduced elbow movement, that adversely affect welfare.

An exploration of welfare impact among common disorders of dogs in the UK identified osteoarthritis to have the highest severity score and second highest welfare impact, with dental disease having the highest welfare impact (31). A large group of UK veterinary surgeons were surveyed about their concerns regarding chronic pain in dogs. Osteoarthritis was perceived as the most common cause of chronic pain in the dog and vertebral and spinal cord conditions were also perceived as a relatively common cause of chronic pain (14). As osteoarthritis is often multifaceted with several disease processes and genetic factors playing a role in the development of the condition, it is important to understand the effects on welfare and how these can be improved.

In addition to current methods of pain evaluation, the AWAG and similar tools may complement and enhance these to provide a holistic approach to chronic pain assessment. This study aims to use the AWAG to understand the effects of chronic pain on canine welfare. The AWAG is a valid and reliable tool scores a range of factors across the four parameters of physical and psychological health, the environment, and procedural and management events (Table 1). Once the user has scored all factors ranging from one the best welfare possible to ten the worst welfare possible using mutually exclusive written descriptors, the AWAG calculates a cumulative welfare assessment score (CWAS) and a mean score for each parameter. The CWAS is the total area of the polygon which is generated by plotting the mean of each parameter across four axes on a radar chart.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Factor scores and their written descriptors.

This study will examine the scores between dogs suffering from chronic pain and those deemed physically and emotionally healthy by clinicians. It also aims to explore correlations among the factors to understand how they interconnect, and to determine which factors of welfare might be predictive of chronic pain in dogs. In doing so, this study seeks to offer a comprehensive understanding of the multifaceted impact of chronic pain on dogs, enhancing strategies for effective management and improving overall dog quality of life.

Methods

The recruitment process for participants in the study involved engaging veterinary surgeons, veterinary nurses, behaviorists, and animal welfare professionals. Individuals were recruited through veterinary networks across the UK. The recruitment strategy encompassed various approaches, including distributing study information posters to partner practices at the University of Surrey and in the Veterinary Times journal. Extensive dissemination of project details were carried out within the researcher’s professional networks. To reach a wider audience, recruitment posters were shared on social networking platforms such as Facebook, Twitter, and LinkedIn from 15/01/2021 to 13/12/2022. This allowed interested individuals to access the recruitment link and share it within their own networks to leverage further dissemination of the AWAG study.

Veterinary surgeons were provided with instructional videos on using the AWAG (accessible via http://awag.org.uk/portal-help). One which provides a systematic walkthrough of how to register and assess a dog, the other on where to see the results and how to interpret these. This provides a consistent and standardized approach among users.

The AWAG1 was used to assess the welfare of dogs that were deemed by the assessing clinician to have a musculoskeletal condition that caused chronic pain. Assessments were undertaken from 23 June 2021 to 31 July 2023. Forty six dogs were assessed, and 76 assessments were undertaken across these dogs. Inclusion criteria included any age and breed of dog at any stage of treatment for a disorder that causes chronic pain. The ages of the dogs ranged from 2 years-old to 17 years-old. Chronic pain could be categorized under the heading “musculoskeletal” as arthritis, osteoarthritis, cruciate disease, hip dysplasia, patellar luxation or other; under the other option, a free text box was available to write additional information about the condition. Throughout this study, the term chronic pain refers to any one of these conditions.

The data used for healthy dogs (n = 143) were from dogs used in a previous study (11). These dogs were assessed by veterinarians and behavior clinicians to be medically and emotionally healthy.

To determine if there was a significant difference between factor scores of healthy dogs and dogs with a condition that causes chronic pain, Wilcoxon rank-sum test (Mann–Whitney U test) were performed for each factor using R Statistical Software (v4.0.1) (32).

A correlation matrix was performed to examine the relationships between each factor using the cor() function, and the resulting matrix was converted to long format using the melt() function from the reshape2 package (33).

To examine what factors were predictive of chronic pain, a logistic regression model was fitted using the glm() function (34). The model coefficients, p-values, and significance levels were extracted using the coef() and format.pval() functions. As several factors were either strongly correlated or were shown to be part of a poorer fitting model using Likelihood Ratio Tests (LRT) and AIC evaluation, eight factors were removed from the model to avoid multicollinearity and to improve model fit. The final logistic regression model formula included:

Logistic regression model formula

logit(p) = β0 + β1·Aggression toward caregiver+β2·Aggression toward unfamiliar people+β3·Behavior during assessment+β4·Enrichment+β5·Fears and anxieties frequency + β6·Reaction to stressors+β7·Social interactions.

Results

Healthy vs. chronic pain

The mean CWAS for dogs with conditions that cause chronic pain was 22.47 and ranged from 3 to 66.87. Healthy dogs scored on average 4.94 and ranged from 2.25 to 15 (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Violin plot of CWAS of healthy dogs and dogs with chronic pain. The violin plot represents the range and concentration of the scores with the widest part showing the highest frequency of scores. The center of each violin plot shows a boxplot with the median line and first and third quartile data on either side. The mean scores are represented as the red point.

Wilcoxon (rank-sum) tests showed that every factor besides body condition significantly different between dogs evaluated to be healthy and dogs with conditions that cause chronic pain (Table 2). The variation between scores can be seen in Figure 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of Wilcoxon (rank-sum) tests between healthy dogs and dogs with chronic pain.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Plot of individual factor scores for each assessment (healthy, n = 143; chronic pain, n = 76).

Factor correlation analysis

Several strong (> ± 0.7) positive and negative correlations were found between several factors (Table 3) of which six were statistically significant (Figure 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Table of correlation coefficients (r) from Spearman Rank correlation analysis.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlation matrix for conditions that cause chronic pain. *p-value < 0.05.

A significant positive correlation (0.93) was found between the dog’s body condition and eating and drinking, i.e., as the dog’s body condition worsened, so did eating and drinking. Clinical assessment was significantly positively correlated mobility and activity (0.91), and also fears and anxiety frequency (0.88) i.e., the severity of clinical symptoms increased with poorer mobility and the frequency at which the dog experienced fearful stimuli. Additionally, fears and anxiety frequency was also positively correlated with mobility and activity (0.84).

A significant negative correlation (−0.79) was found between the dog’s body condition and reaction to stressors, i.e., as body condition improved, the dog took longer to recover from a stressor. Similarly, eating and drinking and the dog’s reaction to stressors were significantly negatively correlated (−0.74) i.e., as eating and drinking improved, the longer the dog took to normalize from a stressful experience.

Logistic regression of factors predictive of chronic pain

The logistic regression model showed that fears and anxieties frequency (ß = 0.47, SE = 0.2, z = 2.35, p = 0.01), reaction to stressors (ß = 1.65, SE = 0.58, z = 2.87, p = <0.01), engagement with enrichment (ß = 1.64, SE = 0.48, z = 3.43, p = <0.001), and social interactions (ß = −1.43, SE = 0.53, z = −2.71, p = <0.01) were significant predictors of chronic pain in dogs.

Discussion

This study investigated the CWAS in dogs with conditions that result in chronic pain, explored the factors that are associated with these, and examined which factors may be predictive of conditions that cause long-term pain. Compared to healthy dogs, dogs with chronic pain were scored much higher across all factors and had an average cumulative welfare score of 22.47 indicating poorer overall welfare. Healthy dogs generally had scores clustered around the mean value, whereas dogs with chronic pain showed a much wider variation. The highest CWAS was 66.87 in dogs with chronic pain, this is 51.87 points higher than the highest CWAS for healthy dogs. The scores not only demonstrate the profound impact pain can have on quality on life but show how varied welfare state can be in dogs that suffer from conditions that cause chronic pain. Therefore, the AWAG shows valuable utility in illustrating the variability in quality of life in dogs with chronic pain.

When examining each factor individually, all factors besides body condition were significantly different in dogs with chronic pain compared to healthy dogs. The analysis showing that body condition scores were not statistically different was an interesting finding given that other studies have shown that quality of life is poorer in overweight and obese dogs (35, 36). However, body condition alone is not an indicator of pain other variables contribute to quality of life. Dogs with conditions such as osteoarthritis may have reduced muscle mass or have low energy and stamina, despite maintaining reasonable body condition. Moreover, the poorer AWAG scores include both overweight and underweight dogs reflecting suboptimal body condition whereas the aforementioned studies only examine overweight dogs.

Regarding the other physical factors, it is unsurprising that clinical assessment and mobility and activity were significantly poorer in dogs with chronic pain. Lameness, altered gait, stiffness, and exercise intolerance are widely cited as predictors of chronic pain in the literature (3739), and these are common motivators to prompt veterinary attention (40). Eating and drinking was found to be poorer in dogs with chronic pain compared to healthy dogs. Although not commonly cited in the literature (41), reduced appetite may be associated with musculoskeletal conditions such as osteoarthritis; but is more commonly linked to other disorders such as cancer and gastrointestinal disease (42, 43) and fear-based behavior problems (44, 45). An alternative explanation may be that owners of dogs with musculoskeletal conditions may attempt to control their dog’s weight through food reduction. These findings may demonstrate that the AWAG can capture vital information about the dog’s quality of life regardless of the condition without having to use more nuanced, disease-specific tools.

Aggression being more prevalent in dogs with chronic pain is consistent with the existing body of literature. Pain and discomfort are reported to lower the threshold for aggressive responses and may also act as a defensive or protective function (12, 46). It is suggested that there are different patterns of pain expression dependent on if the dog has displayed aggression prior to the onset on pain (47). Dogs who previously exhibited no signs of aggression prior to pain are likely to display aggressive behavior more frequently during physical interactions; this should be of importance to clinicians to communicate to dog caregivers that any subtle signs of stress such as yawning, lip-licking, groaning may be indicative of pain during contact. Prior to the behavior escalating into overt aggression, thorough pain investigations and analgesic trials should be undertaken to effectively rule out pain as a differential.

In one study, owners were questioned about their dog’s posture, activity, mobility and behavior and categorized as having chronic pain or not. Dogs presumed to have chronic pain were reported to display protective aggression toward certain body parts and show aggressive behavior toward strangers significantly more often than dogs without signs of chronic pain (23). These findings align with our results where aggression is displayed at higher frequency to both caregivers and strangers where there is evidence of chronic pain.

The dog’s response to stressors and also the frequency at which they encounter these were found to be significantly poorer in dogs with conditions that are associated with musculoskeletal pain. Our results suggest that when faced with a stressor, dogs with chronic pain display signs of fear and anxiety and take longer to recover. Chronic pain is shown to enhance the intensity of behavior through prolonged activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympatho-adrenal-medullary (SAM) axis resulting in evaluated heart rate, respiratory rate, blood pressure, and blood glucose, cortisol, and catecholamines concentrations (48, 49) If a dog is already is a state of physiological arousal prior to encountering a threat, the stress response will be more intense, thus taking longer to normalize. Additionally, the stress response associated with pain can lead to a reduction in serotonin activity (50), and a reduction in serotonin can be linked to aggressive behavior in dogs (51) Furthermore, as serotonin modulates emotional responses, this may be influencing dogs encountering fears and anxieties at a greater frequency. Thus, under the procedural parameter, it is unsurprising that dogs also displayed more signs of emotional distress during AWAG assessments and when being handled.

All factors in the environmental parameter were also found to be worse in dogs with chronic pain compared to healthy dogs. Dogs with pain had less choice, control, and predictability in their lives and also poorer quality social interactions, which may be a result of caregivers limiting their environment and play opportunities for fear of exacerbating pain. Conversely, dogs with chronic pain may attempt to avoid activities or environments that they associate with discomfort in order to prevent pain, but cannot, resulting in frustration and a loss of control and predictability.

Research in humans demonstrates that a sense of control is important for reducing anxiety and depression (52, 53), and this may provide empowerment and a reduction of helplessness. Similarly, when dogs consistently encounter situations that they have no control over such as chronic pain, it is likely to have a negative impact on emotional state. Furthermore, having little or no predictability in the environment may reduce feelings of security, leading to or increasing anxiety-like states.

Dogs in pain may also withdraw themselves from social interactions with people or other dogs (10, 40) and the AWAG scores demonstrate that dogs with chronic pain have poorer quality social interactions and are more socially isolated compared to healthy dogs, who have higher quality direct engagement with people and/or dogs (dependent on the individual’s needs) that involves training or play. Dogs may avoid direct interaction with their caregivers or other dogs as any physical touch may be painful, particularly play where chasing and exaggerated movements are common (54, 55). Social play is reported to decrease in dogs with chronic pain and may use aggression to end interactions with other dogs (56). Dogs may have learned through classical conditioning that physical engagement results in pain, creating negative associations with dogs and people, leading them to become guarded and withdrawn. This may explain why enrichment and social interactions are not highly correlated; dogs with chronic pain may engage more with object play rather than social play as it allows them greater control the situation and remove themselves when necessary. Additionally, dogs with chronic pain are reported to have more sleep disturbances and spend less time resting (57, 58), which may negatively impact emotional state and sociality, culminating in poor welfare. Alternatively, owners may restrict dogs from engaging in play or other social activities to pain, leading to social isolation.

Dogs with chronic pain engaged with enrichment opportunities less often than healthy dogs. Enrichment in this study is defined as anything in the dog’s environment that enhances their welfare state such as exercise, games, training, toys, or feeding devices. Similar to social interactions, dogs may avoid engagement as a protective function, or owners may limit access to any resources the facilitate movement for fear of exacerbating pain.

In the dogs with conditions that cause chronic pain, several expected correlations were observed. Given the causal relationship between body condition and eating and drinking, and clinical assessment and mobility, it is unsurprising that these are highly correlated. Of particular interest were the correlations between fears and anxiety with mobility and activity and clinical assessment. This demonstrates that as clinical assessment and mobility worsens, dogs encounter fears and anxieties at an increasing frequency. There is growing evidence to suggest that certain fears and anxieties such as fearfulness of loud noises are associated with musculoskeletal pain (56). Pain can result in changes in cognition, and it may alter how the dog perceives their environment. They may become more sensitive to sounds and smells, particularly if they find them aversive. Dogs with poorer mobility may feel a heightened sense of vulnerability and anxiety, due to a reduced ability to escape.

An interesting finding to note was the negative correlation between reaction to stressors and eating and drinking and body condition. This suggests that as body condition and eating and drinking improves, the longer a dog takes to recover from a stressor that indices a negative emotional response. However, it is important to note that the direction of the improvement in body condition was not specified (e.g., underweight to normal or from overweight to normal). As we found that eating and drinking was poorer in dogs with chronic pain, overweight dogs might experience an improvement in body condition through reduced appetite, while their emotional state remains negatively affected by the pain, leading to a heightened stress response and longer recovery. Furthermore, in underweight dogs with chronic pain, other comorbid diseases such as Cushing’s disease (hyperadrenocorticism) may cause polyphagia, resulting in improved body condition alongside chronic stress.

The most compelling results of our study pertain to the factors that are reported to be predictors of chronic pain. The dog’s response to stressors, the frequency at which they experience fear and anxiety inducing stimuli, the dog’s engagement with enrichment, and their social interactions are all shown to be significant predictors of conditions that cause chronic pain. These are of particular importance to veterinary surgeons as these are shown to be evident before any clinical signs appear and any change in these should be regarded as an indicator of a musculoskeletal disorder. Chronic pain originating from bones, nerves, muscles, etc. can be incredibly difficult to diagnose (14, 59). Moreover, the dog may still be highly performing activities they are highly motivated to do such as eat, play and run, despite having underlying pain. Therefore, it is important for clinicians not to assume that pain is not a factor where there are no obvious indicators of chronic pain.

Initial indicators of pain may be that the dog responds to stimuli that previously would not elicit a reaction. They may show subtle signs of stress such as yawning, lip licking, hypervigilance, or momentarily freezing. When encountering situations that would normally induce signs of fear, anxiety, or frustration, the dog may take slightly longer to normalize. Caregivers may notice a reduced interest in engagement with members of the household or other dogs, which if unnoticed, may escalate to repulsion behavior from the dog to avoid interactions. However, it is important to consider that individual differences and personality (60) may influence a dog’s behavioral response to pain. Although this may be more applicable to acute pain rather than chronic. Dogs may also spend less time engaging with enrichment that involves exercise, games, and interactions with toys. The dog may still be motivated to engage with their environment; however, prolonged activity may result in feelings of pain or discomfort, leading to an overall reduction in interaction.

Limitations

The primary limitations of the study are that all the disorders categorized as musculoskeletal that can cause chronic pain are combined in the analysis together due to insufficient sample sizes for other conditions that can cause pain such as skin disease and cancer. Therefore, we could not assess if specific conditions impacted quality of life differently. Additionally, as assessments were undertaken as various stages of treatment journeys, we cannot examine pre and post treatment across dogs, only at the individual level. As data collection progresses, additional analyses can be undertaken to evaluate the welfare impact of each disease. Additionally, the inclusion of data from dogs with cancer or neurological pain will provide further insights into the influence of pain quality of life. Another limitation is that the scores used in the analysis are from various timepoints ranging from initial presentation to post treatment; therefore, it is unknown what the effects of analgesia or other treatments have on the collective scores. Further research examining the scores of dogs at initial presentation will contribute to a better understanding of how chronic pain impacts dogs prior to any intervention. In addition, investigating human-dog interactions and their influence in pain management will be of value to understand how this dynamic can improve the welfare of dogs with chronic pain.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study provides a comprehensive examination of the impact of chronic pain on the welfare of dogs with musculoskeletal conditions. The use of the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid proves to be a valuable tool in capturing the variability in quality of life among dogs with chronic pain and highlights the importance of considering both clinician assessment alongside caregiver reports. The study reveals that chronic pain has a profound and multifaceted effect on various aspects of a dog’s life and interacts with physical health, psychological health, the dog’s environment, and any procedures and management events. The recovery time from a stressor, the frequency at which a dog encounters fear and anxiety-inducing stimuli and the quality of social interactions are shown to predictors of chronic pain. Highlighting the importance of the consideration of pain as a differential, especially in cases where typical clinical signs indicative of chronic pain may not be readily apparent.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal studies were approved by NASPA who are a sub-committee of the Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB) University of Surrey. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent was obtained from the owners for the participation of their animals in this study.

Author contributions

RM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SP: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SW: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding for the PhD in which this study was undertaken was provided by Agria SKK.

Conflict of interest

The authors are involved in the development of the tool; thus have a vested interest in its success. SW developed the tool originally. RM’s research has involved adapting the AWAG for dogs and assisting in the production of the AWAG into an online tool.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

1. International Association for the Study of Pain - IASP (2019). IASP’s proposed new definition of pain. Pain Rep. (2018) 3:e643. doi: 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000643

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Belshaw, Z, and Yeates, J. Assessment of quality of life and chronic pain in dogs. Vet J. (2018) 239:59–64. doi: 10.1016/J.TVJL.2018.07.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Walsh, K. Chronic pain management in dogs and cats. In Pract. (2016) 38:155–65. doi: 10.1136/inp.i1489

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Apkarian, AV, Baliki, MN, and Geha, PY. Towards a theory of chronic pain. Prog Neurobiol. (2009) 87:81–97. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hiraga, S, Itokazu, T, Nishibe, M, and Yamashita, T. Neuroplasticity related to chronic pain and its modulation by microglia. Inflamm Regen. (2022) 42:15. doi: 10.1186/s41232-022-00199-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Christiansen, S, and Cohen, SP. Chronic pain: Pathophysiology and mechanisms. In: Essentials of Interventional Techniques in Managing Chronic Pain (2018)

Google Scholar

7. Mota-Rojas, D, Mariti, C, Marcet-Rius, M, Lezama-García, K, Gazzano, A, Hernández-Ávalos, I, et al. The welfare of fighting dogs: wounds, neurobiology of pain, legal aspects and the potential role of the veterinary profession. Animals. (2022) 12:2257. doi: 10.3390/ani12172257

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gordon, WJ, Conzemius, MG, Riedesel, E, Besancon, MF, Evans, R, Wilke, V, et al. The relationship between limb function and radiographic osteoarthrosis in dogs with stifle osteoarthrosis. Vet Surg. (2003) 32:451–4. doi: 10.1053/jvet.2003.50051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Malkani, R, Paramasivam, S, and Wolfensohn, S. A multidimensional evaluation of the factors in the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid (AWAG) that are associated with, and predictive of, behaviour disorders in dogs. Animals. (2024) 14:528. doi: 10.3390/ani14040528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Reid, J, Nolan, AM, and Scott, EM. Measuring pain in dogs and cats using structured behavioural observation. Vet J. (2018) 236:72–9. doi: 10.1016/J.TVJL.2018.04.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Malkani, R, Paramasivam, S, and Wolfensohn, S. Preliminary validation of a novel tool to assess dog welfare: The Animal Welfare Assessment Grid. Front Vet Sci. (2022) 9:940017. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.940017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Barcelos, AM, Mills, DS, and Zulch, H. Clinical indicators of occult musculoskeletal pain in aggressive dogs. Vet Rec. (2015) 176:465. doi: 10.1136/vr.102823

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Frank, D. Recognizing behavioral signs of pain and disease: A guide for practitioners. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. (2014) 44:507–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2014.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bell, A, Helm, J, and Reid, J. Veterinarians’ attitudes to chronic pain in dogs. Vet Rec. (2014) 175:428. doi: 10.1136/vr.102352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Leach, MC, Klaus, K, Miller, AL, Scotto di Perrotolo, M, Sotocinal, SG, and Flecknell, PA. The assessment of post-vasectomy pain in mice using behaviour and the Mouse Grimace Scale. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e35656. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035656

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Reid, J. (2016) Pain-Measuring What Counts | International Animal Health Journal (IAHJ).

Google Scholar

17. Viñuela-Fernández, I, Jones, E, Welsh, EM, and Fleetwood-Walker, SM. Pain mechanisms and their implication for the management of pain in farm and companion animals. Vet J. (2007) 174:227–39. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Holton, L, Reid, J, Scott, EM, Pawson, P, and Nolan, A. Development of a behaviour-based scale to measure acute pain in dogs. Vet. Rec. (2001) 148. doi: 10.1136/vr.148.17.525

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Reid, J, Nolan, AM, Hughes, JML, Lascelles, D, Pawson, P, and Scott, EM. Development of the short-form Glasgow Composite Measure Pain Scale (CMPS-SF) and derivation of an analgesic intervention score. Animal Welfare (2007) 16. doi: 10.1017/s096272860003178x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Firth, AM, and Haldane, SL. Development of a scale to evaluate postoperative pain in dogs. J Am Vet Med. (1999) 214:651–9. doi: 10.2460/javma.1999.214.05.651

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hansen, BD. Assessment of pain in dogs: veterinary clinical studies. ILAR J. (2003) 44:197–205. doi: 10.1093/ilar.44.3.197

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hernandez-Avalos, I, Mota-Rojas, D, Mora-Medina, P, Martínez-Burnes, J, Casas Alvarado, A, Verduzco-Mendoza, A, et al. Review of different methods used for clinical recognition and assessment of pain in dogs and cats. Int J Vet Sci Med. (2019) 7:43–54. doi: 10.1080/23144599.2019.1680044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Demirtas, A, Atilgan, D, Saral, B, Isparta, S, Ozturk, H, Ozvardar, T, et al. Dog owners’ recognition of pain-related behavioral changes in their dogs. J Vet Behav. (2023) 62:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2023.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hielm-Björkman, AK, Kapatkin, AS, and Rita, HJ. Reliability and validity of a visual analogue scale used by owners to measure chronic pain attributable to osteoarthritis in their dogs. Am J Vet Res. (2011) 72:601–7. doi: 10.2460/ajvr.72.5.601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gruen, ME, White, P, and Hare, B. Do dog breeds differ in pain sensitivity? Veterinarians and the public believe they do. PLoS One. (2020) 15:e0230315. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Tomsič, K, Rakinić, K, Sokolov, C, and Seliškar, A. A survey study on the recognition and treatment of pain in dogs and cats by Slovenian veterinarians. Vet Anaesth Analg. (2021) 48:334–43. doi: 10.1016/j.vaa.2020.11.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. McPeake, KJ, Collins, LM, Zulch, H, and Mills, DS. The canine frustration questionnaire—development of a new psychometric tool for measuring frustration in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Front Vet Sci. (2019) 6:152. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wallis, L.J., Range, F., Kubinyi, E., Chapagain, D., Serra, J., and Huber, L. (2017) Utilising dog-computer interactions to provide mental stimulation in dogs especially during ageing. In: ACM International Conference Proceeding Series. Association for Computing Machinery.

Google Scholar

29. Yeates, J, and Main, D. Assessment of companion animal quality of life in veterinary practice and research. J Small Anim Pract. (2009) 50:274–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-5827.2009.00755.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. O’Neill, DG, Brodbelt, DC, Hodge, R, Church, DB, and Meeson, RL. Epidemiology and clinical management of elbow joint disease in dogs under primary veterinary care in the UK. Canine Med Genet. (2020) 7:1–15. doi: 10.1186/s40575-020-0080-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Summers, JF, O’Neill, DG, Church, D, Collins, L, Sargan, D, and Brodbelt, DC. Health-related welfare prioritisation of canine disorders using electronic health records in primary care practice in the UK. BMC Vet Res. (2019) 15:163–20. doi: 10.1186/s12917-019-1902-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing R Foundation for Statistical Computing [Preprint]. (2021). Available at: https://www.R-project.org

Google Scholar

33. Wickham, H. Reshaping data with the reshape package. J Stat Softw. (2007) 21:1–20. doi: 10.18637/jss.v021.i12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Friedman, J, Hastie, T, and Tibshirani, R. Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. J Stat Softw. (2010) 33:1–22. doi: 10.18637/jss.v033.i01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Essner, AE, Zetterberg, L, Hellström, K, Gustås, P, Högberg, H, and Sjöström, R. Psychometric evaluation of the canine brief pain inventory in a Swedish sample of dogs with pain related to osteoarthritis. Acta Vet Scand. (2017) 59:44. doi: 10.1186/s13028-017-0311-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. German, AJ, Holden, SL, Wiseman-Orr, ML, Reid, J, Nolan, AM, Biourge, V, et al. Quality of life is reduced in obese dogs but improves after successful weight loss. Vet J. (2012) 192:428–34. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Brown, DC, Boston, RC, and Farrar, JT. Comparison of Force Plate Gait Analysis and Owner Assessment of Pain Using the Canine Brief Pain Inventory in Dogs with Osteoarthritis. J. Vet. Intern. Med. (2013) 27. doi: 10.1111/jvim.12004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Pettitt, RA, and German, AJ. Investigation and management of canine osteoarthritis. In Pract. (2016) 38. doi: 10.1136/inp.h5763

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Pavarotti, GS, Hivernaud, I, Brincin, M, Roche, R, Barreau, P, Festy, F, et al. Evaluation of a Single Intra-Articular Injection of Autologous Adipose Tissue for the Treatment of Osteoarthritis: A Prospective Clinical Study in Dogs. Vet. Comp. Orthop. Traumatol. (2020) 33:258–266. doi: 10.1055/s-0040-1708524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Belshaw, Z, Dean, R, and Asher, L. Could it be osteoarthritis? How dog owners and veterinary surgeons describe identifying canine osteoarthritis in a general practice setting. Prev Vet Med. (2020) 185:105198. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2020.105198

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Zollers, B, Wofford, JA, Heinen, E, Huebner, M, and Rhodes, L. A Prospective, Randomized, Masked, Placebo-Controlled Clinical Study of Capromorelin in Dogs with Reduced Appetite. J. Vet. Intern. Med. (2016) 30 doi: 10.1111/jvim.1467

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Delaney, SJ. Management of Anorexia in Dogs and Cats. Veterinary Clinics. Small Anim. Pract. (2006) 36:1243–1249.

Google Scholar

43. Johnson, LN, and Freeman, LM (2017) Recognizing, describing, and managing reduced food intake in dogs and cats. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 251: 1260–1266. doi: 10.2460/javma.251.11.1260

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Blackwell, EJ, Bradshaw, JWS, and Casey, RA (2013) Fear responses to noises in domestic dogs: Prevalence, risk factors and co-occurrence with other fear related behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 145. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Shaw, J, and De Biasi, M (2021) Dissecting the neural circuitry of fear-induced appetite suppression. Neuron 109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.02.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Mills, DS, Demontigny-Bédard, I, Gruen, M, Klinck, MP, McPeake, KJ, Barcelos, AM, et al. Pain and problem behavior in cats and dogs. Animals. (2020) 10:318. doi: 10.3390/ani10020318

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Camps, T, Amat, M, and Manteca, X. A review of medical conditions and behavioral problems in dogs and cats. Animals. (2019) 9:1133. doi: 10.3390/ani9121133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Gruen, ME, Lascelles, BDX, Colleran, E, Gottlieb, A, Johnson, J, Lotsikas, P, et al. 2022 AAHA pain management guidelines for dogs and cats. J Am Anim Hosp Assoc. (2022) 58:55–76. doi: 10.5326/JAAHA-MS-7292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Srithunyarat, T, Hagman, R, Höglund, OV, Stridsberg, M, Olsson, U, Hanson, J, et al. Catestatin, vasostatin, cortisol, and pain assessments in dogs suffering from traumatic bone fractures. BMC Res Notes. (2017) 10:129. doi: 10.1186/s13104-017-2450-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mellor, DJ, Cook, CJ, and Stafford, KJ. Quantifying some responses to pain as a stressor. In: The biology of animal stress: basic principles and implications for animal welfare (2000)

Google Scholar

51. Amat, M, Le Brech, S, and Manteca, X. The relationship between aggression and physical disease in dogs. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. (2023) 54:43–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2023.08.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Dracup, K, Westlake, C, Erickson, VS, Moser, DK, Caldwell, ML, and Hamilton, MA. Perceived control reduces emotional stress in patients with heart failure. J Heart Lung Transplant. (2003) 22:90–3. doi: 10.1016/S1053-2498(02)00454-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Miller, SM. Controllability and human stress: Method, evidence and theory. Behav Res Ther. (1979) 17:287–304. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(79)90001-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Bauer, EB, and Smuts, BB. Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Anim Behav. (2007) 73:489–99. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sommerville, R, O’Connor, EA, and Asher, L. Why do dogs play? Function and welfare implications of play in the domestic dog. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2017) 197:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.09.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Lopes Fagundes, AL, Hewison, L, McPeake, KJ, Zulch, H, and Mills, DS. Noise sensitivities in dogs: An exploration of signs in dogs with and without musculoskeletal pain using qualitative content analysis. Front Vet Sci. (2018) 5:17. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2018.00017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Knazovicky, D, Tomas, A, Motsinger-Reif, A, and Lascelles, BDX. Initial evaluation of nighttime restlessness in a naturally occurring canine model of osteoarthritis pain. PeerJ. (2015) 3:e772. doi: 10.7717/peerj.772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Smith, M, Mendl, M, and Murrell, JC. Associations between osteoarthritis and duration and quality of night-time rest in dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2022) 253:105661. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105661

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Mathews, KA. Neuropathic pain in dogs and cats: if only they could tell us if they hurt. Vet Clin of North Am Small Anim Pract. (2008) 38:1365–414. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lush, J, and Ijichi, C. A preliminary investigation into personality and pain in dogs. J Vet Behav. (2018) 24:62–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.01.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dog, welfare assessment, quality of life, chronic pain, veterinary medicine

Citation: Malkani R, Paramasivam S and Wolfensohn S (2024) How does chronic pain impact the lives of dogs: an investigation of factors that are associated with pain using the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid. Front. Vet. Sci. 11:1374858. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1374858

Received: 22 January 2024; Accepted: 11 March 2024;
Published: 04 April 2024.

Edited by:

Daniel Mota-Rojas, Metropolitan Autonomous University, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Karina Lezama, Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana, Mexico
Adriana Domínguez-Oliva, Metropolitan Autonomous University, Mexico

Copyright © 2024 Malkani, Paramasivam and Wolfensohn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rachel Malkani, r.malkani@surrey.ac.uk

Download