Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Endocrinol., 13 July 2023
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Stars and Rising Stars in Neuroendocrinology View all 16 articles

Hormonal contraceptive exposure relates to changes in resting state functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala

Esmeralda Hidalgo-Lopez,*Esmeralda Hidalgo-Lopez1,2*Isabel Noachtar,Isabel Noachtar1,2Belinda Pletzer,*Belinda Pletzer1,2*
  • 1Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria

Introduction: Hormonal contraceptives (HCs), nowadays one of the most used contraceptive methods, downregulate endogenous ovarian hormones, which have multiple plastic effects in the adult brain. HCs usually contain a synthetic estrogen, ethinyl-estradiol, and a synthetic progestin, which can be classified as androgenic or anti-androgenic, depending on their interaction with androgen receptors. Both the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the amygdala express steroid receptors and have shown differential functionality depending on the hormonal status of the participant and the use of HC. In this work, we investigated for the first time the relationship between ACC and amygdala resting state functional connectivity (rs-FC) and HC use duration, while controlling for progestin androgenicity.

Methods: A total of 231 healthy young women participated in five different magnetic resonance imaging studies and were included in the final analysis. The relation between HC use duration and (i) gray matter volume, (ii) fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations, and (iii) seed-based connectivity during resting state in the amygdalae and ACC was investigated in this large sample of women.

Results: In general, rs-FC of the amygdalae with frontal areas, and between the ACC and temporoparietal areas, decreased the longer the HC exposure and independently of the progestin’s androgenicity. The type of HC’s progestin did show a differential effect in the gray matter volume of left ACC and the connectivity between bilateral ACC and the right inferior frontal gyrus.

1 Introduction

Consistent evidence has demonstrated ongoing plastic changes in the adult brain, including those related to ovarian hormones (1, 2). From neurogenesis to remyelination, the neuroactive actions of ovarian hormones are vast and still not fully understood (1, 3). In humans, brain network dynamics are also affected by ovarian hormone fluctuations, and more importantly, these effects appear to be suppressed during the use of hormonal contraceptives (HCs) (4). While women experienced physiological fluctuations of ovarian hormones throughout their adulthood, HCs, nowadays one of the most used contraceptive methods (5), abolish this cycle. In general, HCs downregulate the hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal axis, decreasing the endogenous ovarian hormones’ production and maintaining their levels low, comparable to the levels observed in naturally cycling women during menses (6). In general, the synthetic hormones’ bioavailability remains stable during the active HC use and prevents follicle growth and ovulation (7, 8). HCs usually contain a synthetic estrogen, ethinyl-estradiol, but on the side of synthetic progestins, the range of compounds is wider. The latter can be classified into androgenic or anti-androgenic, depending on their interaction with androgen receptors (9, 10). On the one hand, those progestins derived from 19-nortestosterone (i.e., norethindrone, desogestrel, gestodene, and norgestimate) can be classified as androgenic due to their rapid metabolism to levonorgestrel, which demonstrates agonistic binding to the androgen receptor (11). On the other hand, those progestins derived from spironolactone (i.e., drospirenone), or from 17-hydroxyprogesterone (i.e., chlormadinone acetate or cyproterone acetate), can be classified as anti-androgenic due to their antagonistic actions at the androgen receptor (12). Additionally, dienogest, although derived from 19-nortestosterone, also exhibits anti-androgenic activities (13).

Despite the sparsity of literature regarding HC effects on the female brain, animal research and a few human studies hint at structural (14, 15) and functional changes (see reviews by 16, 17) related to HC use. Among the most consistently reported brain areas in ovarian hormones research are those belonging to the salience network. This network, crucial for emotion processing, is of special interest given that adverse mood effects are the major reason to discontinue HC use (18, 19). Both the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the amygdala, at the core of this network, express steroid receptors (1, 3), and have shown differential functionality depending on the hormonal status of the participant (2022). ACC not only shows differences in activation across the menstrual cycle, but also has been reported to be thinner (23) and to increase its activation to emotional stimuli (24) in HC users compared to naturally cycling women. Likewise, amygdala structure (25) and response to emotional stimuli differs between naturally cycling women and HC users (26), and across the menstrual cycle (27, 28).

Resting state functional connectivity (rs-FC), assessed as the correlated activity of different brain areas in the absence of an explicit task, offers a valuable measure to understand the brain’s intrinsic network organization (29, 30). Nowadays, we can investigate changes in rs-FC to disentangle the dynamics of the healthy brain functioning. Within this framework, neuroimaging studies have shown differences in the intrinsic connectivity of the salience network across menstrual cycle phase, and between naturally cycling women and HC users (for a review, see 16). Across the natural menstrual cycle, rs-FC between the ACC with the middle frontal, superior temporal, transverse temporal, and postcentral gyri (31) increased during the luteal phase when progesterone levels are high (31). In a recent study, we also described increased effective connectivity from the middle frontal gyrus to the ACC, but decreased effective connectivity from the medial prefrontal cortex to the ACC during the luteal phase (22). However, decreased rs-FC between ACC, middle frontal gyrus, and cuneus has also been described for luteal woman (32). Also, during this phase, the amygdala rs-FC with the right middle frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, and paracentral lobule has been reported to increase (31). During the late luteal phase, though, rs-FC between the left amygdala and left angular gyrus and posterior cingulate cortex was decreased compared to the mid-follicular phase (33).

Regarding the HC effects on rs-FC, Engman et al. (31) investigated changes in the ACC and amygdala connectivity in response to a levonorgestrel containing HC in a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. They observed an increased rs-FC of the right ACC with the precuneus and left superior frontal gyrus and a decreased rs-FC between the right amygdala and the left postcentral gyrus, during HC intake (31). Additionally, between-group comparisons revealed increased rs-FC between the left ACC and precuneus, and left amygdala and left postcentral gyrus and precuneus for the naturally cycling luteal women compared to the HC users (31). Active HC users have also shown an increased rs-FC of the ACC within the salience network compared to naturally cycling women (34), but decreased rs-FC between the salience network and the left middle frontal gyrus, compared to both HC inactive phase and naturally cycling women (32). Lisofsky et al. (25) followed women before and after the start of HC use and reported decreased rs-FC between the left amygdala and the right IFG. However, these latter studies did not control for the androgenicity of the HC used, and it remains unclear whether androgenic and anti-androgenic HCs differentially modulate rs-FC of the salience network. Given that exogenous testosterone in women decreases connectivity between ACC and the inferior frontal gyrus (35), and between amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex (36), it seems relevant to account for the type of progestin that the HCs contain. In this work, we investigated for the first time the relationship between ACC/amygdala rs-FC and HC use, while controlling for the progestin’s androgenicity.

Furthermore, while Engman et al. (31) investigated the effects of short-term HC-use (one intake cycle) on rs-FC of the salience network nodes, Petersen et al. (32) included long-term HC users. Given that results differ between the two studies, it is an interesting question whether changes in rs-FC of the salience network accumulate over time. Previous animal research has shown cumulative effects of sex steroids on the brain substrate, ranging from molecular to cellular levels (see reviews: 3, 3740). In rodents, DNA de-methylation through the estrogen receptor is time-dependent (38), while continuous vs. sequential administration of progesterone elicits a differential gene expression profile in the hippocampus (40). After neural damage, progesterone-dependent recovery is affected by treatment duration, alongside differential effects on the cytoarchitectural structure (39). Relatedly, time-dependent effects on synaptic transmission are mediated by changes in shape and density of dendrites (39) and neurotransmitter’s receptors (41). Other steroids’ cumulative effects found in rodents include changes in the glial cells and consequent myelination, with an important role in neuroprotection (37, 42). Finally, and given that the reversibility of HC effects on the brain is still in question, we further explored if the effects found in HC users were replicated in previous HC users. Accordingly, we here opted for studying in a large sample of women those time-dependent associations that may accumulate over HC use duration, differentiating between androgenic and anti-androgenic progestins. In addition to addressing the temporal dynamics of HC effect or rs-FC of the salience network, the pattern of associations might shed some light into the endocrinological mechanisms underlying these effects. At the moment, it remains unclear whether changes in rs-FC during HC use are related to the progestagenic or androgenic/anti-androgenic actions of the progestin component, estrogenic actions of ethinyl-estradiol, or a by-product of HC effects on endogenous neurosteroids.

Given that the contraceptive effects of HC depend largely on the progestin component, the focus with regard to neuroplastic changes in response to HC is usually on the activation of progesterone receptors. Synthetic progestins possess a higher binding affinity for intracellular progesterone receptors than the endogenous hormone (43). If cumulative effects of HC on the salience network depend on progestagenic actions, we can expect effects as observed in the luteal phase (e.g., increased connectivity of the ACC and amygdalae to the middle frontal gyrus; 22, 31) to increase with increasing use duration, irrespective of the androgenicity of the progestin. Furthermore, one important consideration is that most of the effects of endogenous progesterone are exerted through its metabolite allopregnanolone. Synthetic progestins, however, are not metabolized to allopregnanolone, which is a potent modulator of one of the receptors for inhibitory neurotransmitter γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) (for review, see 40). Although women under HC have increased synthetic progestin levels with high affinity for progesterone receptors, their circulating allopregnanolone levels seem to be decreased (androgenic HC: 44; anti-androgenic HC: 45). Accordingly, if previously reported effects along the natural menstrual cycle were related to endogenous allopregnanolone rather than actions on the progesterone receptor, we expect longer HC duration, related to decreased ACC connectivity with the temporal lobe, middle frontal and postcentral gyri connectivity (22, 31) while increased ACC connectivity with the medial prefrontal cortex (22). Likewise, for the amygdala, decreased connectivity with the middle, superior frontal gyrus and paracentral lobule (31) would be expected. These hypotheses are in line with results obtained from long-term HC users in previous studies (32). On the other hand, animal research hints a more complex scenario, in which allopregnanolone levels depend on the type of progestin (see systematic review, 46). In general, while androgenic progestins decrease allopregnanolone levels in the brain; anti-androgenic progestins increase them. If this is also the case in humans, further differential effects are expected for androgenic vs. anti-androgenic HCs related to levels of allopregnanolone.

More importantly, related to the differential modulation of androgen receptors by different progestins, opposite associations to the duration of androgenic vs. anti-androgenic progestin use can be expected. Although we would expect androgenic HC to have similar effects as testosterone (e.g., decreased ACC–inferior frontal gyrus and amygdala–orbitofrontal cortex connectivity; 35, 36), it is noteworthy that endogenous testosterone is metabolized to estradiol, acting also on estrogen receptors (47). Regarding estrogenic effects on the salience network, higher rs-FC between the amygdala and prefrontal and temporal areas has been reported after an estradiol challenge in postmenopausal women (48). Within naturally cycling women, increased connectivity between the amygdala and cuneus, inferior frontal fyrus, precentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, and temporal lobe has been described in the presence of high estradiol levels, while increased connectivity between amygdalae and the ACC has been described for low estradiol levels (49). Accordingly, if cumulative HC effects on the salience network are derived from the estrogenic actions of ethinyl-estradiol, we expect connectivity between the amygdalae and fronto-temporal areas to increase with use duration irrespective of the androgenicity of the progestin.

In general, a decreased rs-FC of the ROIs with frontal areas like middle frontal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus is expected, related to longer HC exposure (25, 32). We also expect an increased rs-FC of the ACC with the precuneus and left superior frontal gyrus and a decreased rs-FC between the amygdalae and the left postcentral gyrus and precuneus, especially for HC with androgenic progestins (31). Further differential effects of androgenic vs. anti-androgenic HC will be explored.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants and procedure

A total of 231 healthy young women (105 current HC users and 126 past HC users and currently naturally cycling) were included in the final analysis, from five different MRI studies (5052). Participants were recruited via flyers at the University of Salzburg and via online advertisements. Most of the participants were university students and all of them were right-handed. Main exclusionary criteria for women were neurological, psychiatric, or endocrine disorders, any medication intake, or any brain abnormalities displayed on structural MRI. Note that differential brain organization may underlie women’s susceptibility to HC adverse mood side effects, which is the major reason for HC discontinuation (19, 53).

In all studies, scanning sessions were scheduled in the active phase of HC use for current HC users (second or third week of the intake cycle), or locked to their menstrual cycle for naturally cycling women (past users). For the latter, the majority of sessions were scheduled during the early follicular phase (cycle days 1–8), except for six participants that were in their mid-luteal phase (3–10 days before onset of next menses). Participants had to confirm the onset of next menses in retrospect.

As part of our standard screening questionnaire, information on previous contraceptive use was collected. The participants were subdivided into HC users with current androgenic HC use (A-HC; n = 62) and current anti-androgenic HC use (AA-HC; n = 43), and naturally cycling women with previous androgenic HC use (A-NC; n = 45), previous anti-androgenic HC use (AA-NC; n = 52), and unknown androgenicity of the HC (n = 29). Further details regarding the categorization of progestins into androgenic or anti-androgenic and the subsample size of each specific progestogenic component can be found in the Supplementary Material.

Only women currently or previously using HCs with only one and the same type of progestin (either A or AA) were included. Specifically for naturally cycling women, only participants who had not been using any HCs or IUDs for the past 6 months and had a regular menstrual cycle, defined as between 21 and 35 days, and less than 7 days of cycle length variability (54), were included. All participants gave their signed written consent to participate in each study. Every study was approved by the University of Salzburg’s ethics committee and conforms to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

The androgenicity of HC was coded as a categorical variable with the following levels: level “A”: androgenic, level “AA”: anti-androgenic, and level “unknown”. Further details are described in the following paragraphs. In order to compare age and HC use duration between androgenic and anti-androgenic users, independent-samples t-test was performed. For women currently using HC, there was a significant difference in age between A-HC (M = 21.31, SD = 2.55) and AA-HC (M = 22.58, SD = 3.57); t(103) = −2.13, p = 0.04. Anti-androgenic users were, on average, approximately 1 year older than androgenic users. Likewise, use duration was significantly different between A-HC (M = 4.00, SD = 2.47) and AA-HC (M = 5.12, SD = 2.92); t(103) = −2.12, p = 0.04. The HC use duration for anti-androgenic users was, on average, approximately 1 year longer than the androgenic duration. For the naturally cycling women, age and HC use duration were not significantly different between women who had previously taken androgenic or anti-androgenic HC (t > |2.5|, p > 0.05) (Table 1). HC use duration was not related to ethinyl-estradiol levels (r < 0.01, p = 0.23), and the distribution of HC use duration by each different type of progestin followed similar distributions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Participants’ demographics.

In order to control for possible moderation effects of ethinyl-estradiol levels and given previously reported dose-dependent cognitive effects (55), the following analyses were further performed controlling for the levels present in the HC. No interactive effect of pill duration by ethinyl-estradiol levels was observed, and therefore, the rest of the analyses and results will focus on the main effect of HC use duration and interaction with HC androgenicity.

2.2 fMRI data acquisition

For each study, a resting state scan of approximately 9 min duration was performed at the beginning of the scanning session. Participants were instructed to close their eyes, relax, and let their mind wander. One of the studies, however, instructed the participants to leave their eyes open (HC = 20, 19% of the final sample; NC = 28, 22% of the final sample). When the sample from this study was excluded, partial correlations between HC use duration and brain connectivity effects remained significant (prage, type of scanner > |0.35|, p < 0.05). Therefore, participants from all five studies were included in the following analyses. Two types of scanners were used: a Siemens Magnetom TIM Trio 3 Tesla and a Siemens Magnetom Prisma Fit 3 Tesla, both of them with a 64ch head coil (see Table 1 for demographics). Although we add a regressor for potential confounding effects of the type of scanner in all of the analyses, the combination of these datasets still poses a conceptual limitation. The impact of Siemens Tim Trio to Prisma upgrade has been assessed in different quantitative MRI measures (56) and proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (57). Higher reliability was identified in most of the MRI outputs that were investigated across the Prisma upgrade (56) and inter-scanner variation had average values of approximately 2.2%–3.8% (56). Although there is also a fair level of intra-vendor consistency in individual scans (58), the present datasets also differed in their respective sequences’ TR. Therefore, we explored additional interactions of the type of scanner with the HC used duration effects herein presented. No significant interactive effects were found.

For the first four studies, functional and high-resolution structural images were acquired on the TIM Trio scanner following a field map acquisition. Functional images consisted of a T2-weighted gradient echo planar sequence with 36 transversal slices oriented parallel to the AC-PC line (whole-brain coverage, TE = 30 ms, TR = 2,250 ms, flip angle 70°, slice thickness 3.0 mm, matrix 192 × 192, FOV 192 mm). For structural images, we acquired a T1-weigthed 3D MPRAGE sequence (160 sagittal slices, slice thickness = 1 mm, TE = 2.91 ms, TR = 2,300 ms, TI delay 900 ms, flip angle 9°, FOV 256 × 256 mm). For the most recent study, functional and high-resolution structural images were acquired on the same MRI device, upgraded to the Prisma Fit system. Functional images consisted of a T2-weighted gradient echo planar sequence with 64 transversal slices oriented parallel to the AC-PC line (whole-brain coverage, multi-slice interleaved, TE = 30 ms, TR = 1,400 ms, flip angle 69°, slice thickness 2.3 mm, matrix 202 × 202, FOV 202 mm). For structural images, we acquired a T1-weigthed 3D MPRAGE sequence (176 sagittal slices, slice thickness = 1 mm, TE = 2.91 ms, TR = 2,300 ms, TI delay 900 ms, flip angle 9°, FOV 256 × 256 mm). In the Prisma scanner study, in order to create unwrapped field maps that can be used to do B0 inhomogeneity distortion correction of the functional scans, two echo planar images (EPI) with opposite phase encode directions were acquired right before the resting state.

2.3 fMRI data analyses

For functional images, the first six images of each session were discarded. The remaining scans were despiked using 3d-despiking as implemented in AFNI (afni.nimh.nih.gov). The resulting images were pre-processed using SPM12 standard procedures and templates including (i) realignment and unwarping of the functional images using the field map, (ii) segmentation of the structural images using CAT12, (iii) co-registration of the functional images to the structural images, (iv) normalization of functional images using the normalization parameters as estimated by CAT12, and (v) spatial smoothing using a 6-mm kernel. Additionally, for the Prisma scanner, the field map was calculated from the two EPI images with opposite phase encoding using the FSL “topup” tool (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/TOPUP; 59), and the Field Map Toolbox from SPM12 was used to calculate a voxel displacement map to correct the BOLD EPI images (https://lcni.uoregon.edu/kb-articles/kb-0003; 60). Pre-processing quality control procedures included the automatic exclusion of participants with excessive movement (>3 mm translation, >2° rotation), visual inspection of structural and functional scans ensuring adequate coregistration, and visually checking the normalization to a standard T1 and an EPI MNI template. Finally, we perform ICA-AROMA non-aggressive removal of artifactual components on the resulting images. ICA-AROMA has been shown to reduce motion-induced variation in fMRI signal, while preserving the signal of interest (61).

2.3.1 Gray matter volume and fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations

Gray matter volumes from bilateral ACC and amygdala were extracted using the get_totals script by G. Ridgeway (http://www0.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/gridgway/vbm/get_totals.m), each region of interest (ROI) defined with the AAL atlas (62).

The fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF) maps were calculated from pre-processed resting state images using the DPABI toolbox (63). The fALFF is a measure of oscillatory activity at the resting state, relative to the whole frequency range (64). It is defined as the ratio of the power spectrum of low frequency (0.01–0.08 Hz) to the average square root of each frequencies power within this range (64, 65).

In order to assess the duration of the HC use*androgenicity interactive effect on (i) gray matter volume and (ii) fALFF, they were introduced as dependent variables in linear models in R Version 1.4.1717, using the lm function from the stats package (66). For all models, HC use duration, androgenicity, and their interaction were included as fixed effects. In case no significant interaction between androgenicity and HC use duration was observed, the interaction was removed from the model and the main effect of HC use duration was calculated across both groups. For every model, age and type of scanner were added as nuisance regressors (e.g., fALFF ~ duration of HC use*androgenicity + age + type of scanner). For the models including gray matter volume, we used the total intracranial volume (TIV) as an additional covariate (e.g., GM IFG ~ duration of HC use*androgenicity +TIV + age + type of scanner). All continuous variables were scaled prior to analyses to allow for interpretation of effect sizes based on standard deviations.

2.3.2 Seed-to-voxel connectivity analysis

We investigated bilateral ACC and amygdala functional connectivity with AAL atlas-defined region of interest (ROI) (62). Seed-to-voxel connectivity maps from these ROIs were estimated for each subject using the CONN-toolbox standard procedures and templates (67). The six movement parameters as well as five white matter and cerebrospinal fluid components were used as regressors during the denoising step. A band-pass filter of 0.008–0.09 Hz was applied. For the group-level analysis, full factorial models were used to evaluate the overall connectivity relation to HC use duration. For each of the ROIs, the first-level contrast images were introduced into a full factorial design in order to investigate the interactive effect of duration of HC use and HC group (androgenic vs. anti-androgenic). In case no significant interaction between androgenicity and HC duration was observed, the main effect of HC use duration was calculated across both groups. In order to control for age and scanning upgrade, their interaction with the HC group was additionally modeled as nuisance regressors. For this second level, results were masked with an SPM gray matter template, and we used an extent threshold of k = 20 voxels, an uncorrected primary threshold of p < 0.001, and a secondary cluster-level FWE-corrected threshold of p < 0.05 (indicated as pFWE). In case a cluster of significant interaction between androgenicity and HC use duration emerged, eigenvalues were extracted from this cluster and partial correlations controlling for age and type of scanner were performed separately for androgenic and anti-androgenic HC users. In a follow-up analysis, we checked if the effects in HC users were replicated in naturally cycling previous HC users. The first eigenvector of the time series across voxels was extracted from the significant FWE-corrected clusters found in HC current users, and partial correlations for naturally cycling previous HC users were performed controlling for both age and type of scanner. In case of no significant interaction between androgenicity and HC use duration, the level “unknown” from HC androgenicity variable was included in the partial correlation. Otherwise, only known “A” and “AA” previous HC users were included and partial correlations were explored separately.

3 Results

3.1 Gray matter volume and fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations

For the gray matter volume of the ROIs, there was an interactive effect of HC use duration and androgenicity in the left ACC [b = −0.31, SEb = 0.15, t(98) = −1.99, p = 0.049]. Current androgenic users showed a smaller GM volume of the left ACC the longer the duration of HC use (Figure S1). However, partial correlations were separated by androgenicity, and controlling for age, TIV and scanner type did not survive the significance threshold [prage, type of scanner, TIV = −0.05, p(57) = 0.73 for A-HC, and prage, type of scanner, TIV = −0.23, p(38) = 0.15 for AA-HC].

No further significant relations were found for right ACC or bilateral amygdala gray matter volume. Neither ACC nor amygdala showed any previous HC use duration effect [all prage, type of scanner <|0.015|, p(122) > 0.05].

No significant relations were found for bilateral ACC or amygdala between fALFF and duration of HC use.

3.2 Seed-to-voxel connectivity analysis

Whole brain connectivity maps of bilateral ACC and amygdalae are displayed in the Supplementary Material (S2–S5). For the ACC, positive connectivity maps included insular and medio-temporal areas, while negative connectivity maps included superior parietal lobes and inferior frontal gyri. Positive connectivity maps for the amygdalae included insular, middle cingulate, and ventromedial prefrontal cortices, among other medio-temporal areas, putamen, and thalamus. Negative connectivity maps for the amygdalae included superior, middle frontal, and angular gyri, among others.

3.2.1 Main effect of HC use duration

For the ACC connectivity, we observed an inversed main effect of current HC use duration and connectivity between the right ACC and left post-central gyrus ([-54, -22, 49], 79 voxels, T = 4.63, pFWE = 0.001), right posterior insula ([36, -16, 10], 67 voxels, T = 4.58, pFWE = 0.003), and right pre/post-central gyrus ([39, -16, 61], 121 voxels, T = 4.54, pFWE < 0.001). Functional connectivity between the right ACC and these three clusters was lower the longer the use of HC in current users, irrespective of the androgenicity (Figure 1A). Connectivity between the right ACC and these three clusters of interest did not show any previous HC use duration effect [all prage, type of scanner" < |0.015|, p(122) > 0.05].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Main effect of current HC use duration on ACC and amygdala connectivity. (A) Current HC users showed lower connectivity between the right ACC and right insula/bilateral post-central gyrus, the longer the use of HC, irrespective of the androgenicity (in green). (B) Independently of the androgenicity of the HC, current users showed lower connectivity between left (in blue) and right (in purple) amygdala with left prefrontal cortex the longer the use of the HC. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; PoCG, post-central gyrus; PI, posterior insula; amyg, amygdala; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; HC, hormonal contraceptive; A, androgenic; AA, anti-androgenic.

We also observed an inversed main effect of HC use duration on connectivity between bilateral amygdalae and prefrontal cortex. Connectivity between bilateral amygdalae and the left middle frontal gyrus ([-30, 35, 40], 37 voxels, T = 3.79, pFWE = 0.042, for the left amygdala; [-30, 26, 46], 62 voxels, T = 4.42, pFWE = 0.004, for the right amygdala) and between the right amygdala and the ventral part of the superior frontal gyrus ([-21, 56, -5], 37 voxels, T = 4.47, pFWE = 0.042) was lower the longer the duration of HC, irrespective of the androgenicity (Figure 1B). Only for the connectivity between the right amygdala and the left superior frontal gyrus did we observe a similar effect of previous HC use duration in naturally cycling participants [prage, type of scanner = −0.19, p(122) = 0.035]. Independently of the androgenicity type, the connectivity between the right amygdala and the ventral part of the superior frontal gyrus ([-21, 56, -5], 37 voxels) was lower the longer the use of HC in previous users (Figure S6).

3.2.2 Interactive effect of HC use duration and HC androgenicity

We further observed an interactive effect of HC use duration and androgenicity in the connectivity between left and right ACC with the triangular part of the right inferior frontal gyrus ([45, 47, -2], 50 voxels, T = 4.18, pFWE = 0.012, for the left ACC; [39, 47, -5], 39 voxels, T = 4.35, pFWE = 0.038, for the right ACC). For current users of an androgenic HC, the connectivity between bilateral ACC and the right inferior frontal gyrus was lower the longer the HC use [prage, type of scanner = −0.46, p(58) < 0.001 for the left ACC; prage, type of scanner = −0.44, p(58) < 0.001 for the right ACC]; for anti-androgenic HC users, connectivity strength was higher the longer the HC use [prage, type of scanner = 0.34, p(39) = 0.03 for the left ACC; prage, type of scanner = 0.31, p(39) = 0.04 for the right ACC; Figure 2]. Connectivity between the ACC and inferior frontal gyrus did not show any previous HC use duration effect in naturally cycling participants [all prage, type of scanner < |0.015|, p(122) > 0.05]. No interactive effect of androgenicity and HC use duration was observed for the rs-FC of the amygdalae.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Interactive effect of current HC use duration and androgenicity on the connectivity between left (in red) and right ACC (in orange) with right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Current users of an androgenic HC (A-HC) showed lower connectivity between bilateral ACC and the right IFG the longer the HC use. In contrast, current users of an anti-androgenic HC (AA-HC) showed stronger connectivity between bilateral ACC and the right IFG the longer the HC use. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus HC, hormonal contraceptive; A, androgenic; AA, anti-androgenic.

In summary, for the current HC users, the connectivity between the right ACC and right insula/bilateral post-central gyrus, and between bilateral amygdalae and left prefrontal cortex, was lower the longer the use of HC, irrespective of the androgenicity. For androgenic HC users, the connectivity between bilateral ACC and the right IFG was lower the longer the HC use; for anti-androgenic HC users, connectivity strength was higher the longer the HC use.

4 Discussion

In this study, we investigated for the first time the differences in the resting state functional connectivity (rs-FC) network of the anterior cingulate cortices (ACC) and the amygdalae related to the duration of androgenic (A) or anti-androgenic (AA) HC use. In general, rs-FC of the ACC and temporoparietal areas, and between the amygdalae with frontal areas, decreased the longer the HC exposure and independently of the progestin androgenicity. Androgenicity did show a differential effect in the connectivity between bilateral ACC and the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG).

Longer HC use duration, irrespective of the androgenicity, was related to decreased connectivity between the ACC and the insular cortex, both involved in the salience network. Although naturally cycling women show within salience network connectivity increased related to enhanced endogenous progesterone (22, 53), this effect was absent in women during HC use (53). The impact of HC on salience-dependent processes such as motivation and reward-oriented behavior has been related to a decrease of the insula activation, for example, to sexual cues (68). In animal models, this sexual behavior has been shown to be impaired by exogenous hormonal treatment, changes suggested to be mediated by the blunted levels of allopregnanolone (69). Conversely, Sharma et al. (34) reported an increased salience within-network connectivity with the medial superior frontal gyrus for the HC users compared to the naturally cycling women (34). It needs to be noted, though, that pubertal-onset HC users were included in this comparison and given that this sub-group was reported to show significantly general increased connectivity compared to the adult-onset sub-group, it is unclear to which extent they drove the direction of the results.

Contrary to our expectations, we did not find a decreased connectivity between the amygdala and the postcentral gyrus and precuneus (31). Instead, it was the ACC that showed decreased connectivity to the bilateral pre/postcentral gyri the longer the HC use. In a recent study analyzing effective connectivity in a placebo-controlled trial, connections between the dorsal ACC and parietal areas decreased during androgenic HC treatment (53). In naturally cycling women, connectivity between the ACC and postcentral gyrus increased during the luteal phase (31), and the connectivity of the somatosensorial cortices was also positively related to progesterone levels (70). Connectivity between amygdalae with superior and middle frontal gyrus was also found decreased the longer the HC use in the present sample, in line with Petersen et al. (32). Moreover, although it did not survive the FWE correction in the whole-brain analysis, the right amygdala also showed reduced connectivity with the ipsilateral middle frontal gyrus, the longer the HC use duration (see Supplementary Material, Figure S7). Conversely, increased connectivity between these areas has been reported before during the progesterone-dominated luteal phase in naturally cycling women (31).

Following the argument that while HC women have increased synthetic progestin levels, their allopregnanolone levels remain decreased (44, 45), and that some effects of endogenous progesterone are exerted through this metabolite, these changes in opposite directions may suggest that the ACC-insular/somatosensorial and prefrontal–amygdalar connectivity positively relates to allopregnanolone levels, which, in turn, remains decreased during HC use. Relatedly, exogenous administration of progesterone, which significantly increased allopregnanolone levels, selectively increased amygdala reactivity (71). However, although animal research corroborates these effects for androgenic progestins, except for drospirenone (72), anti-androgenic progestins appear to increase allopregnanolone levels in the rodent brain (73, 74). On the other hand, although ethinyl-estradiol has a greater affinity for estrogen receptors than endogenous estradiol (75), it is administered in a much lower dose and also shows a differential selectivity for the alpha receptor type over the beta receptor type (76). The present findings could be a consequence of the cumulative effect of synthetic hormones, the abolishment of cyclic endogenous hormonal fluctuations, or the combination of both.

Contrary to our hypotheses, we only found an interactive effect of androgenicity and use duration for bilateral ACC. The connectivity of these areas with the right inferior frontal gyrus was lower the longer the androgenic HC use, while it was stronger the longer the anti-androgenic HC use. In post-menopausal women, connectivity between these areas during memory tasks is positively related to estradiol levels (77). Conversely, in pre-menopausal women using HC, bilateral ACC–inferior frontal gyrus connectivity has been reported to decrease after testosterone administration, during an empathy-related task (35). In male patients, anabolic androgen users showed decreased dorsal attention network connectivity with superior and inferior frontal gyri (SFG/IFG) and the ACC, related to use duration (78). Opposed effects of HC depending on the androgenicity of their progestin have also been extended to the behavioral level by some studies (see review, 79). Although these findings indicate opposite cumulative effects of androgenic vs. estrogenic modulation, both androgenic and anti-androgenic progestins reduce overall testosterone bioavailability (80, 81). Therefore, HC’s androgenicity impact needs to be further elucidated, preferably in longitudinal placebo-controlled trials.

Most of the effects observed in current HC users did not replicate for previous HC users, which could be interpreted as reversibility for such effects. Only the connectivity between the right amygdala and the ventral area of the left superior frontal gyrus still showed a decrease the longer the duration of previous HC use. We have previously described an effect of HC exposure on gray matter volume of subcortical structures, some of which also appear to remain after discontinuation (82). Although some studies hint at a chronic decrease in endogenous hormone levels many years after cessation of HC use (83), and long-term effects in task performance (84), the extent to which fronto-amygdalar connectivity is directly influenced by a prolonged HC use is still undetermined. Further longitudinal randomized placebo-controlled studies need to be carried out in order to fully disentangle the causal effect of HC and its reversibility after discontinuation.

Some remarks and limitations need to be noted. First, and important for the interpretations of these results, is that while a cross-sectional group comparison could identify those effects that emerge after the first months of use, but do not accumulate over time, here we investigated time-dependent associations. Therefore, for those results conflicting with previous literature, an alternative explanation is that following this early impact (that would explain the group differences), the changes adapt/regress over time. Further inconsistencies could also be partly attributable to the small sample and effect sizes of past studies. Second, although the categorization of progestins used here corresponds to their androgenic vs. anti-androgenic effects, this does not always reflect the corresponding androgenicity of the HCs. Progestins with a stronger androgenic effect may be found in lower doses in the HC and therefore have lower androgenic effects in the body once dosage is taken into account (85).Third, HC-related differences could be modulated by a continued suppression of the endogenous hormones, by the exposure to synthetic hormones, and/or by the interaction of both effects. As previously described, endogenous estradiol has a lower affinity for estrogen receptors than ethinyl-estradiol, and endogenous progesterone also differs to the different types of synthetic progestins in their extra-progestogenic effects. For example, they present a different affinity and agonist or antagonist modulation of gluco- and mineralocorticoid receptors (86). Furthermore, the involvement of these receptors in the regulation of the stress response is of special importance when considering potential long-term effects of HC on the brain. Last, but not least, we selected the present ROIs based on previous literature and delimited by the AAL atlas (62) for replication purposes (31). However, there are conceptual and practical challenges when selecting a specific parcellation, including the lack of precision in terms of inter-individual homologous correspondence in brain cortex (87, 88). Additional bias towards smaller sub-networks instead of larger brain systems has also been suggested for seed-based analyses (89).

Overall, these results in a large sample of women suggest cumulative changes in functional connectivity patterns at rest related to the extent of exposure to HC and the abolishment of the endogenous fluctuation of ovarian hormones. Differential effects of the type of progestin arose for some of these functional changes. Given the widespread use of HC among women, and the early onset of HC use, usually starting during adolescence, elucidating the synthetic progestins effects and the functional implications of these findings is of the utmost importance.

Data availability statement

Data and scripts are openly available at https://osf.io/5ezw9/. MR images are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

Every study was approved by the University of Salzburg’s ethics committee and conforms to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

BP designed and made the concept of the study. BP, EH-L, and IN were responsible for acquiring the data. EH-L was responsible for data curation and analysis, interpreting the results, and drafting and revising the manuscript. BP supervised the analysis, contributed in the results’ interpretation, and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) Project P32276 supporting BP; the PhD Programme “Imaging the Mind: Connectivity and Higher Cognitive Function” [W 1233-G17] supporting EH-L and IN; and the European Research Council (ERC) Starting Grant 850953 supporting BP and EH-L.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fendo.2023.1131995/full#supplementary-material

References

1. McEwen B. Estrogen actions throughout the brain. Recent Prog Hormone Res (2002) 57(FEBRUARY 2002):357–84. doi: 10.1210/rp.57.1.357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Wendler A, Albrecht C, Wehling M. Nongenomic actions of aldosterone and progesterone revisited. Steroids (2012) 77(10):1002–6. doi: 10.1016/j.steroids.2011.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Brinton RD, Thompson RF, Foy MR, Baudry M, Wang JM, Finch CE, et al. Progesterone receptors: form and function in brain. Front Neuroendocrinol (2008) 29(2):313–39. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Pritschet L, Santander T, Taylor CM, Layher E, Yu S, Miller MB, et al. Functional reorganization of brain networks across the human menstrual cycle. NeuroImage (2020) 220:117091. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. United Nations. (2022). United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. Available at: https://www.un.org/development/desa/pd/sites/www.un.org.development.desa.pd/files/undesa_pd_2022_wcu_fp-indicators_documentation.pdf.

Google Scholar

6. Fleischman DS, Navarrete CD, Fessler DMT. Oral contraceptives suppress ovarian hormone production. psychol Sci (2010) 21(5):750–2. doi: 10.1177/0956797610368062/ASSET/0956797610368062.FP.PNG_V03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Turgeon JL, Carr MC, Maki PM, Mendelsohn ME, Wise PM. Complex actions of sex steroids in adipose tissue, the cardiovascular system, and brain: insights from basic science and clinical studies. Endocr Rev (2006) 27(6):575–605. doi: 10.1210/er.2005-0020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Jensen JT. Evaluation of a new estradiol oral contraceptive: estradiol valerate and dienogest. Expert Opin Pharmacother (2010) 11(7):1147–57. doi: 10.1517/14656561003724713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kuhl H. Pharmacology of estrogens and progestogens: influence of different routes of administration. Climacteric (2005) 8(SUPPL. 1):3–63. doi: 10.1080/13697130500148875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pletzer BA, Kerschbaum HH. 50 years of hormonal contraception - time to find out, what it does to our brain. Front Neurosci (2014) 8 JUL:256. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Knopp RH, Broyles FE, Cheung M, Moore K, Marcovina S, Chandler WL. Comparison of the lipoprotein, carbohydrate, and hemostatic effects of phasic oral contraceptives containing desogestrel or levonorgestrel. Contraception (2001) 63(1):1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0010-7824(00)00196-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sech LA, Mishell DR. Oral steroid contraception. Women’s Health (2015) 11(6):743–8. doi: 10.2217/WHE.15.82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Foster RH, Wilde MI. Dienogest. Drugs (1998) 56(5):825–33. doi: 10.2165/00003495-199856050-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pletzer B, Kronbichler M, Aichhorn M, Bergmann J, Ladurner G, Kerschbaum HH. Menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptive use modulate human brain structure. Brain Res (2010) 1348:55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. De Bondt T, Van Hecke W, Veraart J, Leemans A, Sijbers J, Sunaert S, et al. Does the use of hormonal contraceptives cause microstructural changes in cerebral white matter? preliminary results of a DTI and tractography study. Eur Radiol (2013) 23(1):57–64. doi: 10.1007/s00330-012-2572-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Brønnick MK, Økland I, Graugaard C, Brønnick KK. The effects of hormonal contraceptives on the brain: a systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Front Psychol (2020) 11:556577. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.556577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Casto KV, Jordan T, Petersen N. Hormone-based models for comparing menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptive effects on human resting-state functional connectivity. Front Neuroendocrinol (2022) 67:101036. doi: 10.1016/J.YFRNE.2022.101036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lindh I, Blohm F, Andersson-Ellström A, Milsom I. Contraceptive use and pregnancy outcome in three generations of Swedish female teenagers from the same urban population. Contraception (2009) 80(2):163–9. doi: 10.1016/j.contraception.2009.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Poromaa IS, Segebladh B. Adverse mood symptoms with oral contraceptives. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand (2012) 91(4):420–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0412.2011.01333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Toffoletto S, Lanzenberger R, Gingnell M, Sundström-Poromaa I, Comasco E. Emotional and cognitive functional imaging of estrogen and progesterone effects in the female human brain: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2014) 50:28–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dubol M, Epperson CN, Sacher J, Pletzer B, Derntl B, Lanzenberger R, et al. Neuroimaging the menstrual cycle: a multimodal systematic review. Front Neuroendocrinol (2021) 60. doi: 10.1016/J.YFRNE.2020.100878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hidalgo-Lopez E, Zeidman P, Harris TA, Razi A, Pletzer B. Spectral dynamic causal modelling in healthy women reveals brain connectivity changes along the menstrual cycle. Commun Biol (2021) 4:954. doi: 10.1038/s42003-021-02447-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Petersen N, Touroutoglou A, Andreano JM, Cahill L. Oral contraceptive pill use is associated with localized decreases in cortical thickness. Hum Brain Mapp (2015) 36(7):2417–851. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Miedl SF, Wegerer M, Kerschbaum H, Blechert J, Wilhelm FH. Neural activity during traumatic film viewing is linked to endogenous estradiol and hormonal contraception. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2018) 87:20–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lisofsky N, Riediger M, Gallinat J, Lindenberger U, Kühn S. Hormonal contraceptive use is associated with neural and affective changes in healthy young women. NeuroImage (2016) 134:597–606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Petersen N, Cahill L. Amygdala reactivity to negative stimuli is influenced by oral contraceptive use. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2014) 10(9):1266–72. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Amin Z, Epperson CN, Constable RT, Canli T. Effects of estrogen variation on neural correlates of emotional response inhibition. NeuroImage (2006) 32(1):457–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Andreano JM, Cahill L. Menstrual cycle modulation of medial temporal activity evoked by negative emotion. NeuroImage (2010) 53(4):1286–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U States America (2003) 100(1):253–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Deco G, Ponce-Alvarez A, Mantini D, Romani GL, Hagmann P, Corbetta M. Resting-state functional connectivity emerges from structurally and dynamically shaped slow linear fluctuations. J Neurosci (2013) 33(27):11239–52. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1091-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Engman J, Sundström Poromaa I, Moby L, Wikström J, Fredrikson M, Gingnell M. Hormonal cycle and contraceptive effects on amygdala and salience resting-state networks in women with previous affective side effects on the pill. Neuropsychopharmacology (2018) 43(3):555–63. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Petersen N, Kilpatrick LA, Goharzad A, Cahill L. Oral contraceptive pill use and menstrual cycle phase are associated with altered resting state functional connectivity. NeuroImage (2014) 90:24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Petersen N, Ghahremani DG, Rapkin AJ, Berman SM, Wijker N, Liang L, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in women with PMDD. Trans Psychiatry (2019) 9:339. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0670-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sharma R, Fang Z, Smith A, Ismail N. Oral contraceptive use, especially during puberty, alters resting state functional connectivity. Hormones Behav (2020) 126(February):104849. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bos PA, Hofman D, Hermans EJ, Montoya ER, Baron-Cohen S, van Honk J. Testosterone reduces functional connectivity during the “Reading the mind in the eyes” test. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2016) 68:194–201. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. van Wingen G, Mattern C, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar J, Fernández G. Testosterone reduces amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex coupling. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2010) 35(1):105–13. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chowen JA, Azcoitia I, Cardona-Gomez GP, Garcia-Segura LM. Sex steroids and the brain: lessons from animal studies. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab (2000) 13(8):1045–66. doi: 10.1515/JPEM.2000.13.8.1045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Finch CE, Felicio LS, Mobbs CV, Nelson JF. Ovarian and steroidal influences on neuroendocrine aging processes in female rodents. Sci Aging Knowledge Environ (2002) 2002(37):cp19–9. doi: 10.1126/sageke.2002.37.cp19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Deutsch ER, Espinoza TR, Atif F, Woodall E, Kaylor J, Wright DW. Progesterone’s role in neuroprotection, a review of the evidence. Brain Res (2013) 1530:82–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Schumacher M, Mattern C, Ghoumari A, Oudinet JPP, Liere P, Labombarda F, et al. Revisiting the roles of progesterone and allopregnanolone in the nervous system: resurgence of the progesterone receptors. Prog Neurobiol (2014) 113:6–39. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Follesa P, Concas A, Porcu P, Sanna E, Serra M, Mostallino MC, et al. Role of allopregnanolone in regulation of GABAA receptor plasticity during long-term exposure to and withdrawal from progesterone. Brain Res Rev (2001) 37(1–3):81–90. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00125-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Garcia-Segura LM, Melcangi RC. Steroids and glial cell function. In: GLIA, vol. 54. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd (2006). p. 485–98. doi: 10.1002/glia.20404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Kuhl H. Pharmacology of progestogens. J Reproduktionsmed Endokrinol (2011) 8:157–77.

Google Scholar

44. Rapkin AJ, Morgan M, Sogliano C, Biggio G, Concas A. Decreased neuroactive steroids induced by combined oral contraceptive pills are not associated with mood changes. Fertil Steril (2006) 85(5):1371–8. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2005.10.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Paoletti AM, Lello S, Fratta S, Orrù M, Ranuzzi F, Sogliano C, et al. Psychological effect of the oral contraceptive formulation containing 3 mg of drospirenone plus 30 μg of ethinyl estradiol. Fertil Steril (2004) 81(3):645–51. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2003.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Pletzer B, Winkler-Crepaz K, Hillerer K. Progesterone and contraceptive progestin actions on the brain: a systematic review of animal studies and comparison to human neuroimaging studies. Front Neuroendocrinol (2023) 101060. doi: 10.1016/J.YFRNE.2023.101060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Shay DA, Vieira-Potter VJ, Rosenfeld CS. Sexually dimorphic effects of aromatase on neurobehavioral responses. Front Mol Neurosci (2018) 11:374/BIBTEX. doi: 10.3389/FNMOL.2018.00374/BIBTEX

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ottowitz WE, Derro D, Dougherty DD, Lindquist MA, Fischman AJ, Hall JE. Analysis of amygdalar-cortical network covariance during pre- versus post-menopausal estrogen levels: potential relevance to resting state networks, mood, and cognition. Neuro Endocrinol Lett (2008) 29(4):467.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

49. Engman J, Linnman C, Van Dijk KRA, Milad MR. Amygdala subnuclei resting-state functional connectivity sex and estrogen differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2016) 63:34–42. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2015.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Pletzer B, Harris TA, Scheuringer A, Hidalgo-Lopez E. The cycling brain: menstrual cycle related fluctuations in hippocampal and fronto-striatal activation and connectivity during cognitive tasks. Neuropsychopharmacology (2019) 44(11):1867–75. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0435-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Menting-Henry S, Hidalgo-Lopez E, Aichhorn M, Kronbichler M, Kerschbaum H, Pletzer B. Oral contraceptives modulate the relationship between resting brain activity, amygdala connectivity and emotion recognition – a resting state fMRI study. Front Behav Neurosci (2022) 16:775796. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.775796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Noachtar I, Harris T-A, Hidalgo-Lopez E, Pletzer B. Sex and strategy effects on brain activation during a 3D-navigation task. Commun Biol (2022) 5(1):1–14. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-03147-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Hidalgo-Lopez E, Engman J, Poromaa IS, Gingnell M, Pletzer B. Triple network model of brain connectivity changes related to adverse mood effects in an oral contraceptive placebo-controlled trial. Transl Psychiatry (2023) 13:209. doi: 10.1038/s41398-023-02470-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Fehring RJ, Schneider M, Raviele K. Variability in the phases of the menstrual cycle. JOGNN - J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs (2006) 35:376–84.

Google Scholar

55. Beltz AM, Hampson E, Berenbaum SA. Oral contraceptives and cognition: a role for ethinyl estradiol. Hormones Behav (2015) 74:209–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.06.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gracien RM, Maiworm M, Brüche N, Shrestha M, Nöth U, Hattingen E, et al. How stable is quantitative MRI? – assessment of intra- and inter-scanner-model reproducibility using identical acquisition sequences and data analysis programs. NeuroImage (2020) 207:116364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Plitman E, Bussy A, Valiquette V, Salaciak A, Patel R, Cupo L, et al. The impact of the Siemens Tim trio to prisma upgrade and the addition of volumetric navigators on cortical thickness, structure volume, and 1H-MRS indices: an MRI reliability study with implications for longitudinal study designs. NeuroImage (2021) 238:118172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Badhwar AP, Collin-Verreault Y, Orban P, Urchs S, Chouinard I, Vogel J, et al. Multivariate consistency of resting-state fMRI connectivity maps acquired on a single individual over 2.5 years, 13 sites and 3 vendors. NeuroImage (2020) 205:116210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Andersson JLR, Skare S, Ashburner J. How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage (2003) 20(2):870–88. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Chang H, Fitzpatrick JM. A technique for accurate magnetic resonance imaging in the presence of field inhomogeneities. IEEE Trans Med Imaging (1992) 11(3):319–29. doi: 10.1109/42.158935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Pruim RHR, Mennes M, van Rooij D, Llera A, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF. ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. Neuroimage (2015) 112:267–77.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

62. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage (2002) 15:273–89.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

63. Yan C-GG, Wang X-D, Di Zuo X-NN, Zang Y-FF. DPABI: Data processing & analysis for (Resting-State) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics (2016) 14:339–51.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

64. Zou QH, Zhu CZ, Yang Y, Zuo XN, Long XY, Cao QJ, et al. An improved approach to detection of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) for resting-state fMRI: fractional ALFF. J. Neurosci. Methods (2008) 172:137–41.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

65. Zang YF, Yong H, Chao-Zhe Z, Qing-Jiu C, Man-Qiu S, Meng L, et al. ltered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev (2007) 29:83–91.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

66. R Core Team. A language and environment for statistical computing. In: R foundation for statistical computing (2018).

Google Scholar

67. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity (2012) 2(3):125–41. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Abler B, Kumpfmüller D, Grön G, Walter M, Stingl J, Seeringer A. Neural correlates of erotic stimulation under different levels of female sexual hormones. PloS One (2013) 8(2):e54447. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Santoru F, Berretti R, Locci A, Porcu P, Concas A. Decreased allopregnanolone induced by hormonal contraceptives is associated with a reduction in social behavior and sexual motivation in female rats. Psychopharmacology (2014) 231(17):3351–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3539-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Arélin K, Mueller K, Barth C, Rekkas PV, Kratzsch JJ, Burmann I, et al. Progesterone mediates brain functional connectivity changes during the menstrual cycle-a pilot resting state MRI study. Front Neurosci (2015) 9:44(FEB). doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Van Wingen GA, Van Broekhoven F, Verkes RJ, Petersson KM, Bäckström T, Buitelaar JK, et al. Progesterone selectively increases amygdala reactivity in women. Mol Psychiatry (2008) 13(3):325–33. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Genazzani AR, Pluchino N, Begliuomini S, Pieri M, Centofanti M, Freschi L, et al. Drospirenone increases central and peripheral β-endorphin in ovariectomized female rats. Menopause (2007) 14(1):63–73. doi: 10.1097/01.gme.0000230348.05745.7d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Lenzi E, Pluchino N, Begliuomini S, Ninni F, Freschi L, Centofanti M, et al. Effects of nomegestrol acetate administration on central and peripheral beta-endorphin and allopregnanolone in ovx rats. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol (2008) 110(1–2):67–75. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2008.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Pluchino N, Lenzi E, Casarosa E, Cela V, Begliuomini S, Ninni F, et al. Dydrogesterone increases allopregnanolone in selected brain areas and in serum of female rats. Fertil Steril (2008) 89(5 Suppl):1384–9. doi: 10.1016/J.FERTNSTERT.2007.03.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Blair RM, Fang H, Branham WS, Hass BS, Dial SL, Moland CL, et al. The estrogen receptor relative binding affinities of 188 natural and xenochemicals: structural diversity of ligands. Toxicol Sci (2000) 54(1):138–53. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/54.1.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Harris HA, Bapat AR, Gonder DS, Frail DE. The ligand binding profiles of estrogen receptors α and β are species dependent. Steroids (2002) 67(5):379–84. doi: 10.1016/S0039-128X(01)00194-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schroeder RA, Thurston RC, Wu M, Aizenstein HJ, Derby CA, Maki PM. Distinctive roles of estrone and estradiol in shaping verbal memory circuitry in postmenopausal women. Alzheimer’s Dementia (2020) 16(S4):e043110. doi: 10.1002/alz.043110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Westlye LT, Kaufmann T, Alnæs D, Hullstein IR, Bjørnebekk A. Brain connectivity aberrations in anabolic-androgenic steroid users. NeuroImage: Clin (2017) 13:62–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Tronson NC, Schuh KM. Hormonal contraceptives, stress, and the brain: the critical need for animal models. Front Neuroendocrinol (2022) 67:101035. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2022.101035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Zimmerman Y, Eijkemans MJC, Coelingh Bennink HJT, Blankenstein MA, Fauser BCJM. The effect of combined oral contraception on testosterone levels in healthy women: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Hum Reprod Update (2014) 20(1):76–105. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dmt038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Hilz EN. Methods and considerations for the use of hormonal contraceptives in rat models of neurobehavior. Front Neuroendocrinol (2022) 66. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2022.101011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Pletzer B, Harris TA, Hidalgo-Lopez E. Previous contraceptive treatment relates to grey matter volumes in the hippocampus and basal ganglia. Sci Rep (2019) 9(1):2417–851. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47446-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Chan MF, Dowsett M, Folkerd E, Wareham N, Luben R, Welch A, et al. Past oral contraceptive and hormone therapy use and endogenous hormone concentrations in postmenopausal women. Menopause (2008) 15(2):332–9. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e31806458d9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Egan KR, Gleason CE. Longer duration of hormonal contraceptive use predicts better cognitive outcomes later in life. J Women’s Health (2012) 21(12):1259–66. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2012.3522

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Hampson E. Oral contraceptives in the central nervous system: basic pharmacology, methodological considerations, and current state of the field. Front Neuroendocrinol (2023) 68. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2022.101040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Regidor PA. The clinical relevance of progestogens in hormonal contraception: present status and future developments. Oncotarget (2018) 9(77):34628–38. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.26015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Poldrack RA. Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2007) 2(1):67–70. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Evans AC, Janke AL, Collins DL, Baillet S. Brain templates and atlases. NeuroImage (2012) 62(2):911–22. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2012.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Cole DM, Smith SM, Beckmann CF. Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front Syst Neurosci (2010) 8. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: resting state fMRI, brain connectivity, hormonal contraceptives, progestins, amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex

Citation: Hidalgo-Lopez E, Noachtar I and Pletzer B (2023) Hormonal contraceptive exposure relates to changes in resting state functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala. Front. Endocrinol. 14:1131995. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2023.1131995

Received: 26 December 2022; Accepted: 09 June 2023;
Published: 13 July 2023.

Edited by:

Alexandre Benani, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Reviewed by:

Elizabeth Hampson, Western University, Canada
Katherine Bottenhorn, Florida International University, United States
Anira Escrichs, Pompeu Fabra University, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Hidalgo-Lopez, Noachtar and Pletzer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Esmeralda Hidalgo-Lopez, esmehl@umich.edu; Belinda Pletzer, belinda.pletzer@plus.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.